Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-t5tsf Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-11T01:52:40.320Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Chapter 4 - Searching for Information, from Infancy to Adolescence

from Part I - What Drives Humans to Seek Information?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  19 May 2022

Irene Cogliati Dezza
Affiliation:
University College London
Eric Schulz
Affiliation:
Max-Planck-Institut für biologische Kybernetik, Tübingen
Charley M. Wu
Affiliation:
Eberhard-Karls-Universität Tübingen, Germany
Get access

Summary

Recent research in cognitive development has supported and built on Piaget’s idea that children’s active engagement with the physical and social world is a crucial component to their learning. In this chapter we offer an overview of the latest results from developmental, cognitive, computational, and educational research on children’s exploration and information search. In particular, we examine the various forms active learning can take across the life span. We start by describing the development of increasingly sophisticated forms of information solicitation in infants and preverbal children, and then draw a developmental trajectory of the effectiveness of children’s exploratory and sampling strategies as well as of their question asking. In doing so, we touch upon three main themes: Children’s sensitivity to environmental inputs, their competence as active learners, and their adaptiveness in tailoring their active learning strategies to different environmental structures. To conclude, we discuss some of the most pressing open questions and promising avenues for future developmental research on active learning and information search.

Type
Chapter
Information
The Drive for Knowledge
The Science of Human Information Seeking
, pp. 77 - 98
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2022

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ackermann, L., Hepach, R., & Mani, N. (2020). Children learn words easier when they are interested in the category to which the word belongs. Developmental Science, 23(3), Article e12915. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12915.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ainsworth, M. D. S. (1992). A consideration of social referencing in the context of attachment theory and research. In Feinman, S. (Ed.), Social referencing and the social construction of reality in infancy (pp. 349367). Plenum.Google Scholar
Alvarez, A. L., & Booth, A. E. (2014). Motivated by meaning: Testing the effect of knowledge-infused rewards on preschoolers’ persistence. Child Development, 85, 783791. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12151.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bazhydai, M., Westermann, G., & Parise, E. (2020). “I don’t know but I know who to ask”: 12‐month‐olds actively seek information from knowledgeable adults. Developmental Science, 23(5), Article e12938. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12938.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Begus, K., & Bonawitz, E. (2020). The rhythm of learning: Theta oscillations as an index of active learning in infancy. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 45, Article 100810. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2020.100810.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Begus, K., & Southgate, V. (2012). Infant pointing serves an interrogative function. Developmental Science, 15, 611617. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2012.01160.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Begus, K., & Southgate, V. (2018). Curious learners: How infants’ motivation to learn shapes and is shaped by infants’ interactions with the social world. In Saylor, M. & Ganea, P. (Eds.), Active learning from infancy to childhood (pp. 1337). Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-77182-3_2.Google Scholar
Betsch, T., Lang, A., Lehmann, A., & Axmann, J. M. (2014). Utilizing probabilities as decision weights in closed and open information boards: A comparison of children and adults. Acta Psychologica, 153, 7486. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2014.09.008.Google Scholar
Betsch, T., Lehmann, A., Lindow, S., Lang, A., & Schoemann, M. (2016). Lost in search: (Mal-)adaptation to probabilistic decision environments in children and adults. Developmental Psychology, 52(2), 311325. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000077.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Callanan, M. A., & Oakes, L. M. (1992). Preschoolers’ questions and parents’ explanations: Causal thinking in everyday activity. Cognitive Development, 7(2), 213233. https://doi.org/10.1016/0885-2014(92)90012-G.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chouinard, M. M., Harris, P. L., & Maratsos, M. P. (2007). Children’s questions: A mechanism for cognitive development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 72(1), i179. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-5834.2007.00412.x.Google Scholar
Cook, C., Goodman, N. D., & Schulz, L. E. (2011). Where science starts: Spontaneous experiments in preschoolers’ exploratory play. Cognition, 120(3), 341349. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2011.03.003.Google Scholar
Cooper, R. P., & Aslin, R. N. (1990). Preferences for infant-directed speech in the first month after birth. Child Development, 61(5), 15841595. https://doi.org/10.2307/1130766.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Csibra, G., & Gergely, G. (2009). Natural pedagogy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(4), 148153. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2009.01.005.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Delgado, B., Gómez, J. C., & Sarriá, E. (2011). Pointing gestures as a cognitive tool in young children: Experimental evidence. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 110(3), 299312. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2011.04.010.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Simone, C., Battisti, A., & Ruggeri, A. (2021). Differential impact of web habits and active navigation on adolescents’ online learning. PsyArXiv. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/hsvc4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Simone, C., & Ruggeri, A. (2021). What is a good question asker better at? From unsystematic generalization to adult-like selectivity across childhood. Cognitive Development, 59, 101082. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2021.101082.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dewey, J. (1986). Experience and education. The Educational Forum, 50(3), 241252. https://doi.org/10.1080/00131728609335764.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dickstein, S., Thompson, R. A., Estes, D., Malkin, C., & Lamb, M. E. (1984). Social referencing and the security of attachment. Infant Behavior and Development, 7(4), 507516. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0163-6383(84)80009-0.Google Scholar
Domberg, A., Koskuba, K., Rothe, A., & Ruggeri, A. (2020). Goal-adaptiveness in children’s cue-based information search. In Denison., S., Mack, M., Xu, Y., & Armstrong, B.C. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 42nd Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 14371443). Cognitive Science Society. https://cogsci.mindmodeling.org/2020/papers/0298/0298.pdf.Google Scholar
Donnellan, E., Bannard, C., McGillion, M. L., Slocombe, K. E., & Matthews, D. (2020). Infants’ intentionally communicative vocalizations elicit responses from caregivers and are the best predictors of the transition to language: A longitudinal investigation of infants’ vocalizations, gestures and word production. Developmental Science, 23, Article e12843. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12843.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duarte Torres, S., & Weber, I. (2011). What and how children search on the web. In Proceedings of the 20th ACM International Conference on Information and Knowledge Management (pp. 393402). Association for Computing Machinery. https://doi.org/10.1145/2063576.2063638.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunn, K., & Bremner, J. G. (2017). Investigating looking and social looking measures as an index of infant violation of expectation. Developmental Science, 20, Article e12452. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12452.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Einav, S., & Robinson, E. J. (2010). Children’s sensitivity to error magnitude when evaluating informants. Cognitive Development, 25(3), 218232. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2010.04.002.Google Scholar
Elsner, C., & Wertz, A. E. (2019). The seeds of social learning: Infants exhibit more social looking for plants than other object types. Cognition, 183, 244255. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2018.09.016.Google Scholar
Fitneva, S. A., Lam, N. H. L., & Dunfield, K. A. (2013). The development of children’s information gathering: To look or to ask? Developmental Psychology, 49(3), 533542. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0031326.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frazier, B. N., Gelman, S. A., & Wellman, H. M. (2009). Preschoolers’ search for explanatory information within adult–child conversation. Child Development, 80(6), 15921611. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01356.x.Google Scholar
Freeman, J. L., Caldwell, P. H., Bennett, P. A., & Scott, K. M. (2018). How adolescents search for and appraise online health information: A systematic review. The Journal of Pediatrics, 195, 244255. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpeds.2017.11.031.Google Scholar
Goldin‐Meadow, S. (2007). Pointing sets the stage for learning language – and creating language. Child Development, 78, 741745. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01029.x.Google Scholar
Gopnik, A., & Sobel, D. M. (2000). Detecting blickets: How young children use information about novel causal powers in categorization and induction. Child Development, 71(5), 12051222. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00224.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gopnik, A., Frankenhuis, W. E., & Tomasello, M. (2020). Introduction to special issue: “Life history and learning: how childhood, caregiving and old age shape cognition and culture in humans and other animals.” Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2019.0489.Google Scholar
Gossen, T., Low, T., & Nürnberger, A. (2011). What are the real differences of children’s and adults’ web search. In Proceedings of the 34th International ACM SIGIR Conference on Research and Development in Information Retrieval (pp. 11151116). Association for Computing Machinery. https://doi.org/10.1145/2009916.2010076.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gottlieb, J., Oudeyer, P.-Y., Lopes, M., & Baranes, A. (2013). Information-seeking, curiosity, and attention: Computational and neural mechanisms. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17(11), 585593. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2013.09.001.Google Scholar
Goupil, L., Romand-Monnier, M., & Kouider, S. (2016). Infants ask for help when they know they don’t know. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 113(13), 34923496. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1515129113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gregan-Paxton, J., & John, D. R. (1995). Are young children adaptive decision makers? A study of age differences in information search behavior. Journal of Consumer Research, 21(4), 567580. https://doi.org/10.1086/209419.Google Scholar
Greif, M. L., Kemler Nelson, D. G., Keil, F. C., & Gutierrez, F. (2006). What do children want to know about animals and artifacts? Domain-specific requests for information. Psychological Science, 17(6), 455459. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01727.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harris, P. L., Koenig, M. A., Corriveau, K. H., & Jaswal, V. K. (2018). Cognitive foundations of learning from testimony. Annual Review of Psychology, 69, 251273. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-122216-011710.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hembacher, E., deMayo, B. & Frank, M. C. (2017). Children’s social referencing reflects sensitivity to graded uncertainty. In Gunzelmann, G, Howes, A., Tenbrink, T., & Davelaar, E (Eds.), CogSci 2017: Proceedings of the 39th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 496500). Cognitive Science Society. https://cogsci.mindmodeling.org/2017/papers/0101/paper0101.pdf.Google Scholar
Henderson, A. M., & Woodward, A. L. (2011). “Let’s work together”: What do infants understand about collaborative goals? Cognition, 121(1), 1221. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2011.05.008.Google Scholar
Herwig, J. E. (1982). Effects of age, stimuli, and category recognition factors in children’s inquiry behavior. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 33(2), 196206. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-0965(82)90015-7.Google Scholar
Hickling, A. K., & Wellman, H. M. (2001). The emergence of children’s causal explanations and theories: Evidence from everyday conversation. Developmental Psychology, 37(5), 668683. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.37.5.668.Google Scholar
Howse, R. B., Best, D. L., & Stone, E. R. (2003). Children’s decision making: The effects of training, reinforcement, and memory aids. Cognitive Development, 18(2), 247268. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0885-2014(03)00023-6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jones, A., Swaboda, N., & Ruggeri, A. (2020). Developmental changes in question-asking. In Butler, L., Ronfard, S., & Corriveau, K. (Eds.), The questioning child: Insights from psychology and education (pp. 118143). Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108553803.007.Google Scholar
Kaldy, Z., & Blaser, E. (2013). Red to green or fast to slow? Infants’ visual working memory for “just salient differences.Child Development, 84, 18551862. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12086.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kampe, K. K., Frith, C. D., & Frith, U. (2003). “Hey John”: Signals conveying communicative intention toward the self activate brain regions associated with “mentalizing,” regardless of modality. Journal of Neuroscience, 23(12), 52585263. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-12-05258.2003.Google Scholar
Kelemen, D., Callanan, M. A., Casler, K., & Pérez-Granados, D. R. (2005). Why things happen: Teleological explanation in parent-child conversations. Developmental Psychology, 41(1), 251264. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.41.1.251.Google Scholar
Kemler Nelson, D. G., Egan, L. C., & Holt, M. B. (2004). When children ask, “What is it?” what do they want to know about artifacts? Psychological Science, 15(6), 384389. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00689.x.Google ScholarPubMed
Kidd, C., & Hayden, B. Y. (2015). The psychology and neuroscience of curiosity. Neuron, 88(3), 449460. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2015.09.010.Google Scholar
Kidd, C., Piantadosi, S. T., & Aslin, R. N. (2012) The Goldilocks effect: Human infants allocate attention to visual sequences that are neither too simple nor too complex. PLoS One, 7(5), Article e36399. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0036399.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kidd, C., Piantadosi, S. T., & Aslin, R. N. (2014). The Goldilocks effect in infant auditory attention. Child Development, 85, 17951804. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12263.Google Scholar
Koenig, M. A., & Harris, P. L. (2005). Preschoolers mistrust ignorant and inaccurate speakers. Child Development, 76(6), 12611277. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00849.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Köksal-Tuncer, Ö., & Sodian, B. (2018). The development of scientific reasoning: Hypothesis testing and argumentation from evidence in young children. Cognitive Development, 48, 135145. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2018.06.011.Google Scholar
Kurkul, K. E., & Corriveau, K. H. (2018). Question, explanation, follow‐up: A mechanism for learning from others? Child Development, 89(1), 280294. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12726.Google Scholar
Kutsuki, A., Egami, S., Ogura, T., Nakagawa, K., Kuroki, M., & Itakura, S. (2007). Developmental changes of referential looks in 7- and 9-month-olds: A transition from dyadic to proto-referential looks. Psychologia, 50(4), 319329. https://doi.org/10.2117/psysoc.2007.319.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Legare, C. H., Mills, C. M., Souza, A. L., Plummer, L. E., & Yasskin, R. (2013). The use of questions as problem-solving strategies during early childhood. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 114(1), 6376. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2012.07.002.Google Scholar
Loewenstein, G. (1994). The psychology of curiosity: A review and reinterpretation. Psychological Bulletin, 116(1), 7598. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.116.1.75.Google Scholar
Lucca, K., & Wilbourn, M. P. (2019). The what and the how: Information-seeking pointing gestures facilitate learning labels and functions. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 178, 417436. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2018.08.003.Google Scholar
Lutz, D. J., & Keil, F. C. (2002). Early understanding of the division of cognitive labor. Child Development, 73(4), 10731084. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00458.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Macedo‐Rouet, M., Potocki, A., Scharrer, L., Ros, C., Stadtler, M., Salmerón, L., & Rouet, J.‐F. (2019). How good is this page? Benefits and limits of prompting on adolescents’ evaluation of web information quality. Reading Research Quarterly, 54(3), 299321. https://doi.org/10.1002/rrq.241.Google Scholar
McCormack, T., Frosch, C., Patrick, F., & Lagnado, D. (2015). Temporal and statistical information in causal structure learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 41(2), 395416. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038385.Google ScholarPubMed
Meder, B., Wu, C. M., Schulz, E., & Ruggeri, A. (2021). Development of directed and random exploration in children. Developmental Science. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.13095.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mills, C. M. (2013). Knowing when to doubt: Developing a critical stance when learning from others. Developmental Psychology, 49(3), 404418. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029500.Google Scholar
Mosher, F. A., Hornsby, J. R., Bruner, J., & Oliver, R. (1966). On asking questions. In Bruner, J. (Ed.), Studies in cognitive growth (pp. 86102). Wiley.Google Scholar
Muentener, P., & Schulz, L. (2014). Toddlers infer unobserved causes for spontaneous events. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, Article 1496. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01496.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelson, J. D., Divjak, B., Gudmundsdottir, G., Martignon, L. F., & Meder, B. (2014). Children’s sequential information search is sensitive to environmental probabilities. Cognition, 130(1), 7480. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2013.09.007.Google Scholar
Nyhout, A., & Ganea, P. A. (2019). Mature counterfactual reasoning in 4- and 5-year-olds. Cognition, 183, 5766. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2018.10.027.Google Scholar
Oakes, L. M. (2010). Using habituation of looking time to assess mental processes in infancy. Journal of Cognition and Development, 11(3), 255268. https://doi.org/10.1080/15248371003699977.Google Scholar
Oakes, L. M. (2017). Sample size, statistical power, and false conclusions in infant looking‐time research. Infancy, 22, 436469. https://doi.org/10.1111/infa.12186.Google Scholar
Palmquist, C. M., & Jaswal, V. K. (2015). Preschoolers’ inferences about pointers and labelers: The modality matters. Cognitive Development, 35, 178185. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2015.06.003.Google Scholar
Pelz, M., & Kidd, C. (2020). The elaboration of exploratory play. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 375(1803), 20190503. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2019.0503.Google Scholar
Perez, J., & Feigenson, L. (2020). Violations of expectation trigger infants to search for explanations. PsyArXiv. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/eahjd.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1952). The origins of intelligence in children. International University Press.Google Scholar
Ronfard, S., Zambrana, I. M., Hermansen, T. K., & Kelemen, D. (2018). Question-asking in childhood: A review of the literature and a framework for understanding its development. Developmental Review, 49, 101120. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2018.05.002.Google Scholar
Ruggeri, A., & Feufel, M. (2015). How basic-level objects facilitate question-asking in a categorization task. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, Article 918. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00918.Google Scholar
Ruggeri, A., & Lombrozo, T. (2015). Children adapt their questions to achieve efficient search. Cognition, 143, 203216. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2015.07.004.Google Scholar
Ruggeri, A., Lombrozo, T., Griffiths, T. L., & Xu, F. (2016). Sources of developmental change in the efficiency of information search. Developmental Psychology, 52(12), 21592173. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000240.Google Scholar
Ruggeri, A., Sim, Z. L., & Xu, F. (2017). “Why is Toma late to school again?” Preschoolers identify the most informative questions. Developmental Psychology, 53(9), 16201632. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000340.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ruggeri, A., Swaboda, N., Sim, Z. L., & Gopnik, A. (2019). Shake it baby, but only when needed: Preschoolers adapt their exploratory strategies to the information structure of the task. Cognition, 193, Article 104013. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2019.104013.Google Scholar
Ruggeri, A., Walker, C. M., Lombrozo, T., & Gopnik, A. (2021). How to help young children ask better questions? Frontiers in Psychology, 11, Article 2908. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.586819.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sabbagh, M. A., & Baldwin, D. A. (2001). Learning words from knowledgeable versus ignorant speakers: Links between preschoolers’ theory of mind and semantic development. Child Development, 72(4), 10541070. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00334.Google Scholar
Schulz, E., Pelz, M., Gopnik, A., & Ruggeri, A. (2019). Preschoolers search longer when there is more information to be gained. PsyArXiv. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/5wegk.Google Scholar
Schulz, E., Wu, C. M., Ruggeri, A., & Meder, B. (2019). Searching for rewards like a child means less generalization and more directed exploration. Psychological Science, 30(11), 15611572. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797619863663.Google Scholar
Schulz, L. E., & Bonawitz, E. B. (2007). Serious fun: Preschoolers engage in more exploratory play when evidence is confounded. Developmental Psychology, 43(4), 10451050. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.43.4.1045.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Senju, A., & Csibra, G. (2008). Gaze following in human infants depends on communicative signals. Current Biology, 18(9), 668671. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2008.03.059.Google Scholar
Siegel, M., Pelz, M., Magid, M., Tenenbaum, J. B., & Schulz, L. E. (2021). Children’s exploratory play tracks the discriminability of hypotheses. Nature Communications, 12(3598). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-021-23431-2.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sim, Z. L., & Xu, F. (2017). Learning higher-order generalizations through free play: Evidence from 2- and 3-year-old children. Developmental Psychology, 53(4), 642651. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/dev0000278.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stahl, A. E., & Feigenson, L. (2015). Observing the unexpected enhances infants’ learning and exploration. Science, 348(6230), 9194. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aaa3799.Google Scholar
Stahl, A. E., & Feigenson, L. (2019). Violations of core knowledge shape early learning. Topics in Cognitive Science, 11, 136153. https://doi.org/10.1111/tops.12389.Google Scholar
Stenberg, G. (2009). Selectivity in infant social referencing. Infancy, 14(4), 457473. https://doi.org/10.1080/15250000902994115.Google Scholar
Swaboda, N., Meder, B., & Ruggeri, A. (2020). Finding the (most efficient) way out of a maze is easier than asking (good) questions. PsyArXiv. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/tdaqg.Google Scholar
Vaish, A., Demir, Ö. E., & Baldwin, D. (2011). Thirteen‐ and 18‐month‐old infants recognize when they need referential information. Social Development, 20, 431449. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2010.00601.x.Google Scholar
van den Bos, W., & Hertwig, R. (2017). Adolescents display distinctive tolerance to ambiguity and to uncertainty during risky decision making. Scientific Reports, 7, Article 40962. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep40962.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
VanderBorght, M., & Jaswal, V. K. (2009). Who knows best? Preschoolers sometimes prefer child informants over adult informants. Infant and Child Development, 18, 6171. https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.591.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Schijndel, T. J., Visser, I., van Bers, B. M., & Raijmakers, M. E. (2015). Preschoolers perform more informative experiments after observing theory-violating evidence. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 131, 104119. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2014.11.008.Google Scholar
Vygotsky, L. S. (1987). Problems of general psychology. In Rieber, R. & Carton, A. (Eds.), The collected works of L. S. Vygotsky. Plenum Press. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4613-1655-8.Google Scholar
Walden, T., Kim, G., McCoy, C., & Karrass, J. (2007). Do you believe in magic? Infants’ social looking during violations of expectations. Developmental Science, 10, 654663. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00607.x.Google Scholar
Wu, Y., & Gweon, H. (2019). Preschool-aged children jointly consider others’ emotional expressions and prior knowledge to decide when to explore. PsyArXiv. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13585.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×