Personality disorders are common psychiatric disorders, especially in primary care (Reference Moran, Jenkins and TyleeMoran et al, 2000; Reference Singleton, Bumpstead and O'BrienSingleton et al, 2001), and they carry significant costs for healthcare services (Reference Moran, Jenkins and TyleeMoran et al, 2000). People with personality disorders present a problem in psychiatry because they demonstrate both symptoms of psychological distress and social rule-breaking behaviour. They therefore invite punitive as well as therapeutic responses, which can lead to confusion and negativity in service providers (Reference Watts and MorganWatts & Morgan, 1994).
In the past, people with personality disorders were frequently excluded from mental health services, on the grounds that they could not be treated or that they were not ill. However, exclusion is no longer an option in the UK. Department of Health policy emphasises that service providers should address the needs of people with personality disorders, especially those who harm themselves (Department of Health, 2003; National Collaborating Centre for Mental Health, 2004) or others (Home Office & Department of Health, 1999).
Current models of personality disorder used by psychiatrists tend to focus largely on the problem behaviours. However, behaviours cannot be symptoms: they involve assumed intention, especially where social rules and conventions are breached or broken. It is also not clear how to treat a behaviour, in the absence of an understanding of its underlying cognitive schema and neurobiological basis.
What might be helpful, then, is a model that would allow personality disorders to be understood much as we understand other illnesses that cause harmful dysfunctional states (Reference WakefieldWakefield, 1992). This might offer both a better understanding of why symptoms occur and why and how certain treatments work, and further options for treatment. In this article, we suggest that the major feature of personality disorders is a failure of affect regulation. We present evidence on the neurobiology of affect regulation and on its development within attachment relationships in a heuristic model that explains both the symptoms of and effective treatment strategies for personality disorders. Being heuristic in nature, this model will, we hope, form the basis of further empirical research.
Affects
What are they and where are they formed?
E(x)motion indicates a departure from a basic state of calm (Reference FreemanFreeman, 1999: p. 124) (emotions and affects are essentially similar terms and we use them interchangeably in this article). Reference DamasioDamasio (1994) posits that emotions are bodily experiences (somatosensory states) in response to external and internal influences. Several areas of the somatosensory cortex are associated with the recall of emotional experiences, especially the insula, cingulate cortex, hypothalamus and several nuclei of the brain-stem tegmentum (Reference DamasioDamasio, 2003). The bodily states created include autonomic, neuroendocrine and somatomotor responses that are subjectively experienced as feelings and are expressed through a range of somatomotor responses, including facial, gestural, vocal and behavioural reactions. Thus, behaviour is merely one expression of an affective state; individuals also use words (written and spoken) and facial expression to communicate affective states.
What are their functions?
Affects act as a driving force or catalyst to assist humans in pursuit of goal-directed behaviours that help us to find sources of energy, fend off external obnoxious agents, and make and maintain social relationships to support a life-sustaining homoeostasis (Reference PankseppPanksepp, 1998; Reference RoseRose, 1998; Reference DamasioDamasio, 2003). This is achieved by a complex interplay of multiple systems and events within the body that lead to an automated regulation of life. The ‘machinery’ involved includes a number of systems (Box 1), nested within each other, that are ultimately driven by emotions. No one system acts in isolation: simple systems are regulated by more complex ones (Reference DamasioDamasio, 2003). Affect regulation is one aspect of the more complex systems required for optimal homoeostasis. This nested principle, with the emotions governing the motivational machinery of the body, includes, but goes beyond, the reductionist view of affects simply as states elicited by rewards and punishments (Reference RollsRolls, 2000).
-
• The endocrine system
-
• Simple reflexes (e.g. the startle reflex, which is protective in nature and has survival value)
-
• The immune system
-
• Drives and motivations (e.g. hunger, thirst and curiosity)
-
• Approach and avoidance behaviours that lead to appetites and desires
Humans are unique among animals in their long period of total dependence on others for survival after birth. Like other non-human primates that live in social groups, people instinctively make and maintain different types of social relationship for survival. These relationships are a function of time, complexity and interpersonal attachments. Interpersonal affective responses need to be both regulated and organised to be effective. In particular, the most favourable management of relationships requires the capacity to regulate negative affects such as anger and anxiety. This is especially true of relationships characterised by discrepancies of power and those that involve dependency and neediness, for example relationships with partners, children, family members and professional carers.
We suggest that the need and the ability to coexist with others in order to survive optimally are fundamental to the development and maintenance of affect regulation. In other words, the affects fine-tune the organism’s struggle for survival, but affect regulation can improve the quality of that survival.
Affect production and regulation
Regulation in any homoeostatic system (including that of affect) means not only initiating a response to a stimulus, but also modulating it appropriately and turning it off when no longer required. Regulation also implies that the response itself is organised and effective. Reference Phillips, Drevets and RauchPhillips et al(2003) suggest that affective experience involves:
-
1 identification of the emotional significance of a stimulus;
-
2 production of an affective state in response;
-
3 regulation of the affective state.
Identification of emotional significance
Two areas of the brain – the amygdala and the insula – are involved in the identification of the emotional significance of a stimulus. The amygdala is responsible for modulation of vigilance and attention to emotionally salient information. The insula conveys aversive sensory information to the amygdala, and the two areas act in concert to detect and respond to threatening and aversive stimuli. They can be conceptualised as a defence radar alerting the organism to the presence of threat in its environment and stimulating a fight or flight self-preservative response (see Reference Phillips, Drevets and RauchPhillips et al, 2003).
Production of a responding affective state
Sites implicated in triggering the production of affective states in response to a stimulus include the amygdala, insula, parts of the anterior cingulate gyrus, striatum, and orbitofrontal and ventromedial prefrontal cortices.
The amygdala subserves fear-conditioning (Reference Bechara, Tranel and DamasioBechara et al, 1995) and autonomic reactions associated with feelings of fear (Reference Gloor and AggletoGloor, 1992). The insula is implicated in induced sadness, and anticipatory, phobic and traumatic anxiety (Reference Charney, Drevets, Davis, Charney and CoyleCharney & Drevets, 2002). It is also activated during internally generated self-directed disgust, i.e. social emotions such as guilt and shame (Reference Shin, Dougherty and OrrShin et al, 2000). Stimulation of the ventral (affective) division of the anterior cingulate gyrus evokes autonomic and visceromotor changes and spontaneous emotional vocalisations (Reference Bancaud and TalaraichBancaud & Talaraich, 1992). The ventral striatum appears to be involved in craving (Reference Breiter, Gollub and WeiskoffBreiter et al, 1997), anticipation of reward (Reference Pagnoni, Zink and MontaguePagnoni et al, 2002) and romantic love (Reference Bartels and ZekiBartels & Zeki, 2000). The orbitofrontal cortex is associated with autonomic changes accompanying affective states such as anger (Reference Dougherty, Shin and AlpertDougherty et al, 1999) and physical aggression (Reference Pietrini, Guazzelli and BassoPietrini et al, 2000). The ventromedial prefrontal cortex is involved in induction of sad mood (Reference Pardo, Pardo and RaichlePardo et al, 1993) and guilt and in responding to facial expressions of negative emotions (Reference Sprengelmeyer, Young and CalderSprengelmeyer et al, 1996).
Regulation of the affective state
Affect regulation is largely dependent on the functioning of two neural systems: a ventral and a dorsal system (Reference Phillips, Drevets and RauchPhillips et al, 2003).
The ventral system includes the amygdala, insula, ventral striatum and ventral (affective) regions of the anterior cingulate gyrus and prefrontal cortex. It is important for rapid appraisal of emotional material, and automatic affective regulation in response to social interactions, including the capacity for interpersonal empathy.
The dorsal system includes the hippocampus and dorsal (cognitive) regions of the anterior cingulate gyrus and prefrontal cortex. It supports selective and sustained attention, planning and effortful (rather than automatic) regulation of affective states, and autonomic responses to those states. Here affect regulation involves cognitive appraisals: using logic and rational evaluations, based on past experience and anticipated future outcomes.
These contributions of the two systems might be summarised as insight and foresight respectively (Reference FreemanFreeman, 1999: p. 124) (Box 2).
Insight is mediated by the ventral system:
-
• amygdala
-
• insula
-
• ventral striatum
-
• ventral (affective) regions of the anterior cingulate gyrus and prefrontal cortex
Foresight is mediated by the dorsal system:
-
• hippocampus
-
• dorsal (cognitive) regions of the anterior cingulate gyrus and prefrontal cortex
The role of attachment relationships in affect regulation
Reference SchoreSchore (2002, Reference Schore and Green2003) has set out an explanatory framework for affect dysregulation, based on research into the neural development of the infant brain. He reviews the evidence that the rearing environment (in the form of the infant’s relationship with the mother or other primary carer) has a direct effect on the development of brain structures and pathways involved in affect regulation. Animal research by Reference Suomi, Cassidy and ShaverSuomi (1999, Reference Suomi2003) has also demonstrated the importance of the interaction between the genetic basis for neural and synaptic development (temperament) and the developing infant’s socio-emotional environment (nurture) in the development of neurotransmitter systems and cytoarchitecture.
Secure attachment
In humans, attachment operates through the interaction of two behavioural systems: caregiving and care-eliciting (Reference George and SolomonGeorge & Solomon, 1996). These foster identification of affects, the response to them and the regulation of the affective system. It is useful to conceptualise the interaction between a caregiver and care-elicitor as one that regulates the experience of emotions through a crescendo–decrescendo process (Reference SchoreSchore, 2002). A distressed infant responds to threats in his (or her) environment by experiencing a high degree of arousal, mediated by the sympathetic division of the autonomic nervous system. This is a catabolic system, making available large amounts of energy to prepare the infant for a self-preservative action repertoire of a fight/flight mode. The infant experiences the peripheral and central effects of noradrenaline (norepinephrine) (e.g. more rapid heart and pulse rate, increased blood pressure, dilated pupils), which are uncomfortable. By soothing the infant, the mother helps in recruiting the infant’s parasympathetic system, which has opposite effects and restores homoeostasis. There is a return to normal rate and rhythm of the autonomic system. The sympathetic system supports an action-consuming state, whereas the parasympathetic system supports a withdrawal–conservation state.
The earliest attachment figure conceivably acts as a primary affect regulator, one that ameliorates and terminates the infant’s distress, augments within reasonable limits its experience of happiness and pleasure, and offers predictable and replicable affect regulation. The basic language of attachment relationships thus consists of episodes of interactive signals produced by the autonomic nervous system in both infant and caregiver. These episodes emerge at about 2 months of age, and they are highly arousing, affect-laden and short interpersonal events that expose the infant to high levels of cognitive and social information (Reference Feldman, Greenbaum and YirmiyaFeldman et al, 1999). As the infant grows, it is the relationship, rather than a particular caregiver, that becomes the (accessory) affect regulator.
A right prefrontal cortex regulation of the autonomic nervous system lies at the heart of the development of affect regulation in an infant. The right hemisphere is also centrally involved in corporeal self-identity and its relation to the environment, distinguishing the self from non-self (Reference DevinskyDevinsky, 2000). Infant–maternal attachment behaviour is almost exclusively body to body, and it is now accepted that the right hemisphere is involved in the social and biological functions of the attachment system in the infant (Reference WangWang, 1997). Furthermore, this hemisphere is crucial in the receptive and expressive empathic processes (Reference Adolphs, Damasio and TranelAdolphs et al, 2000), which are processed unconsciously using extensive reciprocal connections with both the limbic systems.
A good-quality affect regulatory system, based on secure attachment, leads to optimal right hemispheric maturation at a critical period during the first 2–3 years of life (Reference SchoreSchore, 2002). Any experience that disturbs the development of secure attachment at a time of heightened dependence (e.g. abuse, neglect or inconsistent caring) will lead to impaired development of neural pathways that subserve emotional behaviours, such that impaired emotional regulation is likely to persist throughout the individual’s lifetime.
The final task in terms of emotion processing involves the internalisation of affect-regulating capacity. Up to about 5 years of age, children locate both affects and their stimuli outside of the self. Any adverse emotional experience is therefore ascribed to the object (including humans) causing it: an externalisation of affects. Later, children locate emotions internally and still later they can identify mixed and conflicting emotions (Reference Levine, Marziali and HoodLevine et al, 1997). Thus, emotion is initially perceived as being caused, and is in reality regulated, by others, but over the course of early development it becomes increasingly self-regulated as a result of neurophysiological development (Reference Thompson and DienstbierThompson, 1990: p. 371).
Insecure attachment
The successful outcome of secure attachment is the development of the basic machinery to self-regulate affects later in life (Reference Fonagy, Gergely and JuristFonagy et al, 2002). Insecure attachment prevents the development of a proper affect regulatory capacity. The individual is left with either an inability to balance sympathetic hyperarousal in response to threat, or the production of an untimely or inadequate parasympathetic response. Dysregulation of this nature leads to prolonged persistence of a catabolic state of fight/flight hyperarousal, or a sudden and inappropriate shift into an anabolic withdrawal–conservation state of ‘freezing’. The latter occurs when a situation is perceived to be hopeless and one’s own agency inadequate, leading to inhibition and avoidance in order to become ‘unseen’ (a state of dissociation) as a defensive strategy of last resort (see Reference SchoreSchore, 2002). Alternatively, there could be rapid cycling between states of hyperarousal and withdrawal, resulting in gross disorganisation of both affects and associated behaviours.
Affect dysregulation and symptoms of personality disorder
Individuals who have experienced insecure attachment are at risk of developing dysregulated and disorganised affective systems. Both small-scale (Reference Patrick, Hobson and CastlePatrick et al, 1994; Reference Fonagy, Target, Steele, Atkinson and ZuckerFonagy et al, 1997) and larger studies (Reference Johnson, Cohen and BrownJohnson et al, 1999) have found that early childhood adversity, especially neglect, is a risk factor for the development of personality disorders. Childhood sexual abuse is also a risk factor for the development of self-harming and suicidal behaviour in adulthood (Reference Andrews, Corry, Slade, Ezzati, Lopez and RodgersAndrews et al, 2003) – behaviour commonly seen in people with personality disorders.
One major outcome of this is that people with personality disorders have significant difficulty in establishing and maintaining interpersonal relationships that require good affect regulation. They seem to withdraw from and alienate others and/or engage in confusing and disorganised relationships. This characteristic is observed particularly within dependency relationships during adulthood (e.g. relationships with peers, partners, children and professional carers), which may be experienced as disparities of power and vulnerability, giving rise to a sense of threat and fear. Inability to regulate negative affects within dependency relationships increases the chance of responding with unregulated hostility or anger. This puts these individuals at a double disadvantage: not only do they tend to alienate caregivers, but they are likely to do it at times of greatest need.
In exhibiting this behaviour, individuals with personality disorders are moving away from the recently (in evolutionary terms) evolved adaptive species-preservative behaviour seen in mammals towards a more ancient self-preservative behaviour. As the name suggests, species-preservative behaviour has evolved to improve the chances of survival of a species, and it is based on parental care, nursing, social interaction, pair-bonding and mutual defence (Reference Henry and WangHenry & Wang, 1998). If trauma results in a stressful loss of control, the self-preservative fight/flight catecholamine coping-response takes priority. Problems arise when this style becomes the default coping response to a wide range of events, people and circumstances. It is then maladaptive and inappropriately accessed.
Regulation of negative affect
The problem is not that people with personality disorders are ‘affectless’ but that they have too much or too little affect, depending on the perceived social stimulation, i.e. the affective system is dysregulated and the responses disorganised. Affective dysregulation also implies an unpredictability that goes beyond either an excessive or diminished response.
Heightened perception of threat seems to be a major problem for people with personality disorders, one that emphasises a lack of safety with and an essential untrustworthiness of others. This is compounded by an inability to repair the emotional states stimulated by threat or fear. They seem to lack the capacity to soothe themselves after fearful experiences (Reference Van der Kolk and Fislervan der Kolk & Fisler, 1994), becoming and remaining hyper-aroused in an uncontrollable, dysregulated manner. Their difficulty in providing an internal discourse for themselves to manage negative affects leads to the expectation or requirement of an external solution when they feel bad – preferably from another person whom they identify as having a caring role.
In people with personality disorders there appears to be a deficit, if not an absence, of the shift in locus from external to internal affect regulation. They appear to continue to believe that emotions are almost always an outcome of external developments caused by other people. This is a problem of excessive externalisation of experience of negative affects, a task that should have been resolved around 5 years of age. Such responses are therefore age inappropriate and immature.
Regulation and specific personality disorders
Both ICD–10 (World Health Organization, 1992) and DSM–IV (American Psychiatric Association, 1994) implicitly endorse, without prioritisation, the characteristic disturbances in affect regulation found in most personality disorders and all personality clusters (Reference Geiger, Crick, Ingram and PriceGeiger & Crick, 2001) (Box 3).
Cluster A
Prototype: paranoid personality disorder
-
• Consistent under-regulation of the affects of fear and terror
-
• Over-regulation of positive affects – narrow range of affective expression
-
• Over-regulation (muting) of all affects in schizoid personality disorder
Cluster B
Prototype: borderline personality disorder
-
• Dysregulation (under- or over-) of both positive and negative affects, but predominantly demonstrated with fear, anger, sadness and anxiety
-
• Under-regulation of fear, arousal and anger, and over-regulation of feelings of empathy, remorse and guilt in antisocial and narcissistic personality disorders
-
• Under-regulation of most affects in histrionic personality disorder
Cluster C
Prototype: anxious/avoidant personality disorder
-
• Under-regulation of social emotions, e.g. shame and guilt
-
• Under-regulation of anxiety and sadness
-
• Positive affects usually experienced only when with others
Cluster A
Cluster A personality disorders (paranoid, schizoid, schizotypal) are characterised by increased paranoia and suspiciousness of others. People with paranoid personality disorders have increased suspiciousness and arousal based on the excessive fear that arises from their heightened perception of threat, under-regulation of fear and a fight/flight response pattern. Those with schizoid and schizotypal personality disorders also have a predominantly constricted affect. Individuals with schizoid personality disorder also experience lack of pleasure and an affective indifference towards others, suggesting perhaps a muting of all affective responses on account of over-regulation of affects. Schizotypal personality disorder is characterised by inappropriate affect and heightened social anxiety, secondary to paranoia, and lacking in habituation.
Cluster B
Cluster B personality disorders (borderline, antisocial, histrionic and narcissistic) are the classic example of dysregulation, and borderline personality disorder is the prototype. In this cluster there is clinical evidence of dysregulation of all negative affects, primarily involving fear and anger, but including depression and anxiety.
People with borderline personality disorder alternate between having either no trust in others or a highly risky tendency to fail to see threat when it is present. They also experience predominantly depressive mood disorders and poorly controlled anger, and form under-regulated, intense attachments to others that are often a source of further affective distress and arousal.
Histrionic personality disorder is characterised by shallow or labile affect, excitement seeking and an exaggerated emotional expression.
Individuals with antisocial personality disorder show an excessive capacity for blaming others (externalisation of affect) and have little or no regard for the feeling of others, as exemplified by impairment in empathy, remorse and guilt. They share certain characteristics with those with narcissistic personality disorder, who also show little empathy but also excessive envy and jealousy. People with either disorder seem to see others as highly risky and unstable sources of aggression or threat, and their own aggression, paranoia and cruelty to others is likely to be due to under-regulation of arousal in response to threat. They seem to have difficulty in regulating emotions that have a social valence, suggesting a dysfunction based in the prefrontal cortex. Not surprisingly, people with these particular disorders have the greatest difficulty in adapting to social norms and customs.
Cluster C
The avoidance behaviour so characteristic of Cluster C personality disorders (avoidant, dependent and anakastic) may be seen as avoidance of situations, people and thoughts that provoke unmodulated affect, usually severe anxiety and panic, in a classic behavioural style.
Whereas people with anakastic personality disorders show excessive doubt and caution, avoiding risks altogether, those with avoidant personality disorders have a heightened fear of criticism and disapproval, with a possible heightened sense of shame and ridicule.
People with dependent personality disorders have exaggerated fears of their own ability to care for themselves and therefore avoid being alone, depending on others to validate their existence.
Substance misuse
Substance misuse is a common feature of personality disorders. It is likely that the pathways that mediate the hedonic properties of psychostimulants evolved as neural systems for social attachment. There is evidence that brain activation patterns in adults responding to attachment figures (partners or children) are similar to neural responses to cocaine-induced euphoria (Reference Bartels and ZekiBartels & Zeki, 2000). The brain structures involved include bilateral activation in the anterior cingulate gyrus, medial insula and ventral striatum. These findings suggest that the high rates of substance misuse by people with personality disorders may therefore lie, in part at least, in their dysfunctional or absent social attachments. Substance use (and misuse) acts as a social ‘integrator’, both externally with peers and internally through the induction of a pleasurable state. This state replaces the very basic human quality of gregariousness. Furthermore, substances are used as external regulators of negative affects because the individual perceives these affects to be externally, not internally, caused.
Violence
Some personality disorders are associated with high rates of violence and rule-breaking. Reference BlairBlair (2001) suggests that violence can take one of two forms: reactive violence, which is elicited in response to frustration or threat, and instrumental violence, which is goal-directed, purposeful and apparently unprovoked.
Reactive violence has been conceptualised as a response to perceived threat, mediated by the hypothalamus–periacqueductal grey matter system. The amygdala feeds information into the periacqueductal grey matter system on the current state of threat, thus determining whether the response is fight or flight. The orbitofrontal cortex has extensive projections to autonomic control centres in the medial hypothalamus and periacqueductal grey matter, and it is damage specifically to this part of the frontal lobe that leads to greatest risk of reactive violence (Reference Grafman, Schwab and WardenGrafman et al, 1996). Thus, reactive violence is a consequence of inadequate regulation of threat-based affects, largely by the dorsal prefrontal cortex.
Instrumental violence is a function of cruelty and lack of empathy (Reference Hare, Hart and HarpurHare et al, 1991), which in turn has been linked to muted autonomic responses to sad and fearful facial expressions. In this type of violence, it has been speculated that the affect regulating system of the prefrontal cortex remains intact, but there is a fundamental problem within the amygdala, the area concerned with properly identifying fearful and sad emotions (Reference BlairBlair, 2001). Other studies have implicated bilateral frontotemporal-based cognitive strategies for processing affective material (Reference Blair, Jones and ClarkBlair et al, 1997), and disturbed functional connectivity of brain areas related to emotion processing (Reference Müller, Sommer and WagnerMüller et al, 2003). Clearly, the final word has not been said on this issue.
Implications for treatment
Essentially, all therapeutic interventions in psychiatry seek to regulate affects by various means (Reference BradleyBradley, 2000: p. 146), and it is no different for personality disorders. Cognitive psychotherapies are likely to engage the dorsal prefrontal system, which is involved in the use of reason, logic and foresight, to influence affect regulation. Relationship-based therapies (including individual and group psychodynamic therapies), which are based on emotional experiences, are likely to be processed in the ventral prefrontal cortices. This is consistent with evidence that mild to moderate degrees of personality disorder can be treated using a combination of psychotherapies (Reference Bateman and TyrerBateman & Tyrer, 2004).
An affect-regulation model of personality disorder also helps to explain the use of polypharmacy to treat it. This includes all classes of psychotropic drugs, which are often used on a trial and error basis (Reference Tyrer and BatemanTyrer & Bateman, 2004). It has been proposed that, although most psychotropics have some specificity for psychiatric disorders, most have a generic affect- (anxiety-) regulating function. Antipsychotic medications are most effective for the most intense and disorganising anxiety (psychotic reactions), whereas antidepressants and sedatives have an anxiolytic effect in the less disorganising types (Reference LeDouxLeDoux, 1996). This may explain the efficacy of mood-stabilising agents in the management of personality disorders. Given the prevalence of substance misuse in personality disorder, it is hardly surprising that any prescribed drugs that reduce arousal or regulate affect will be as effective (or ineffective) as illicit drugs; nor is it surprising that people with personality disorders may misuse prescribed drugs.
Affect regulation is also relevant in group processes such as therapeutic communities, which are clearly effective for mild to moderate personality disorder (Reference Lees and ManningLees & Manning, 1999). Community members report feeling more confident in dealing with their own (insight) and others’ negative feelings (empathy), especially hostility and rage. The therapeutic benefit of such communities for people with personality disorders may arise from the secure attachment to the community that they can make, which allows them to develop a greater capacity to manage negative affect internally.
Conclusions
Our heuristic model is based on an integrative synthesis of recent empirical evidence from the fields of attachment and neurobiology, relating it to current strategies for treating personality disorders. It proposes a biologically grounded system that is nevertheless sufficiently based on clinical findings to be clinically relevant.
Affect regulation is only one, but arguably the most critical, aspect of personality disorder. Given its developmental origins, it is a key foundation on which other aspects of personality – thoughts, perceptions and behaviour – are built.
Our model is purposely limited in scope and does not incorporate neuroendocrine regulation or the involvement and interaction of various neurotransmitters and neuromodulators. Neither does it deal with the problem of comorbid mental illness and the fact that personality disorders rarely occur singly. Finally, the model does not explain all types of personality disorder, especially psychopathy and schizotypy. We suggest that there are fundamental differences in the brain mechanisms underlying the latter disorders, which may be the result of altered patterns of neural connectivity and responses that are largely genetically based rather than a product of gene–environment interaction.
Our key conclusion is that a personality disorder is like many other complex medical conditions. It has degrees of severity and can manifest with varying levels of behavioural dysfunction and symptomatic distress. Mild degrees of personality disorder are probably compatible with reasonable mental health and functioning; more severe disorder or comorbid psychiatric conditions will cause more dysfunction and result in referral to mental health services. If there is to be a national strategy for personality disorder services, clinical teams will need explanatory models to help them understand their patients’ problems and plan treatment accordingly.
Declaration of interest
None.
MCQs
-
1 Personality disorders:
-
a are common psychiatric disorders
-
b are associated with behavioural problems
-
c do not co-exist with other disorders
-
d invite punitive or rejecting responses from others
-
e are associated with subjective distress.
-
-
2 Affect regulation:
-
a is the same as emotional regulation
-
b involves both mental and physical experience
-
c is an essential part of social relationships
-
d is a function of several complex neurological systems
-
e does not influence behaviour.
-
-
3 Affects are produced in the following brain areas:
-
a amygdala
-
b insula
-
c orbitofrontal cortex
-
d occipital cortex
-
e parietal lobes.
-
-
4 Secure attachment in mammals:
-
a regulates affects through modulation of the noradrenergic system
-
b affects the development of right orbitofrontal cortex
-
c promotes exploratory behaviour and learning
-
d is associated with successful social relationships with peers
-
e is associated with successful parenting in adulthood.
-
-
5 People with personality disorders:
-
a are affectless
-
b do not feel distress or anxiety
-
c have increased capacity for down-regulation of affect
-
d have decreased capacity for recognition of affective signals in others
-
e are unresponsive to all therapies.
-
1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
a | T | a | T | a | T | a | T | a | F |
b | T | b | T | b | T | b | T | b | F |
c | F | c | T | c | T | c | T | c | T |
d | T | d | T | d | F | d | T | d | T |
e | T | e | F | e | F | e | T | e | F |
eLetters
No eLetters have been published for this article.