Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-v9fdk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-14T16:46:42.758Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Psychopathic personality in young people

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

This article is an overview of developments in psychopathy and their application to children and adolescents. A key question is whether or not psychopathy is stable throughout the lifespan. Some characteristics indicate phenotypic similarities with adult psychopathy, and current instruments appear to be measuring similar constructs across the age ranges. Although the literature on developmental aspects of psychopathy in young people is limited, a number of instruments have been designed to measure the construct. These tools appear to have reasonable construct, concurrent and predictive validity, but we cannot yet recommend their routine use in clinical practice or in the criminal justice system, given the limited evidence base on their predictive validity. At best, they should be viewed as a means of subtyping potentially high-risk groups with a view to treatment planning.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © The Royal College of Psychiatrists 2003 

Psychopathy is a personality disorder characterised by a constellation of interpersonal, affective and behavioural characteristics (Reference Hare, Cooke, Forth and HareHare, 1998). The early literature suggested that it was a uni-dimensional phenomenon, but subsequent studies revealed that measures of psychopathy had at least a two-factor structure, comprising an interpersonal/affective element (factor 1) and a social deviance component (factor 2). More recently, a three-factor structure has been proposed (Reference Cooke and MichieCooke & Michie, 2001), which includes:

  1. an arrogant, deceitful interpersonal style, involving dishonesty, manipulation, grandiosity and glibness;

  2. defective emotional experience, involving lack of remorse, poor empathy, shallow emotions and a lack of responsibility for one's own actions;

  3. behavioural manifestations of impulsiveness, irresponsibility and sensation-seeking.

Conduct disorder, antisocial personality disorder and psychopathy are often seen as developmental disorders that span the life course and the terms are sometimes used interchangeably. There are, however, significant differences between them and their associated correlates. Whereas conduct disorder and antisocial personality disorder primarily focus on behavioural problems, psychopathy, as described by Reference HareHare (1991), emphasises deficits in affective and interpersonal functioning. Psychopathy is seen as a higher-order construct, which can now be reliably be assessed in adults using the Psychopathy Checklist – Revised (PCL–R; Reference HareHare, 1991). A score of >30 on the PCL–R indicates prototypical psychopathy.

The estimated prevalence of adult psychopathy in the general population is 1%, rising to between 15% and 25% in incarcerated groups. The notion that individuals identified as PCL–R ‘psychopaths’ are different from people with a diagnosis of antisocial personality disorder comes from research showing that there are high rates (50–80%) of antisocial personality disorder in prison populations, but only 20% of these meet Hare's criteria for psychopathy (Reference Hare, Cooke, Forth and HareHare, 1998).

Aetiology of psychopathy

The biological and environmental factors responsible for the development and maintenance of antisocial behaviour and psychopathy are not well understood. Studies in children and adolescents (Reference Forth, Burke, Cooke, Forth and HareForth & Burke, 1998) indicate that several family background variables (e.g. parental rejection, inconsistent discipline, abuse) are associated with the development of conduct disorder and psychopathy. However, an independent literature also suggests that children with conduct disorder, particularly those with a history of repeated violent behaviour, exhibit neuropsychological deficits (Reference Moffitt, Henry and MilnerMoffitt & Henry, 1991) and have reduced levels of arousal, including reduced plasma cortisol levels (Reference RaineRaine, 1993; Reference McBurnett, Lahey, Fowles, Sutker and GoodmanMcBurnett & Lahey, 1994). These findings suggest a role for neurobiological factors, particularly executive (prefrontal) and temporolimbic (amygdala) dysfunction, in the aetiology of conduct disorder and psychopathy.

The prefrontal cortex and behaviour

There are a number of theories relating to the neurological basis of antisocial behaviour and psychopathy. Of those focusing on the prefrontal cortex, the most prominent are the somatic marker hypothesis (Reference DamasioDamasio, 1994) and the response modulation deficit hypothesis (for a review see Reference Newman, Cooke, Forth and HareNewman, 1998).

The somatic marker hypothesis suggests that damage to the ventromedial cortex results in a failure of the somatosensory structures to mark experiences as good or bad, and consequently there is no mechanism for learning to avoid aversive situations. This model receives some support from studies of acquired psychopathy, where subjects show an attenuated autonomic response to aversive social stimuli.

The response modulation deficit hypothesis, which also focuses on the ventromedial prefrontal cortex, accounts for the risk-taking behaviour and failure to learn from experience seen in criminals with psychopathic personality performing laboratory-based tasks of passive-avoidance learning. Some support for the response modulation deficit hypothesis comes from studies revealing that children with marked callous and unemotional traits show an insensitivity to punishment cues on a gambling task (Reference Frick, Cooke, Forth and HareFrick, 1998).

The amygdala and affect

There are also two main theories relating to the affective characteristics of psychopathy: the punishment/low-fear theory (Reference LykkenLykken, 1995) and the violence inhibition mechanism deficit hypothesis (Reference BlairBlair, 1995). Both models suggest a crucial role for the amygdala as the seat of dysfunction in psychopathic individuals. The low-fear model stresses the aspects of psychopathy related to sensation-seeking and insensitivity to punishment (Reference LykkenLykken, 1995; Reference PatrickPatrick, 1994), whereas the violence inhibition mechanism model accounts for the specific failure of basic emotions (e.g. fear) to result in autonomic arousal and the inhibition of ongoing behaviour in individuals with psychopathic personality.

To date, few of these theories have been extensively tested in child and adolescent samples. However, some support for the violence inhibition mechanism model has emerged in studies showing a selective impairment in the processing of sad and fearful faces in children with psychopathic tendencies (Reference Blair, Colledge and MurrayBlair et al, 2001).

The relationship between callous-unemotional traits and conduct disorder

Reference Frick, Cooke, Forth and HareFrick (1998) outlined a basic framework for conceptualising the relationship between what he termed callous-unemotional traits and conduct problems in children. In this model, callous-unemotional traits develop as part of a unique temperamental style, low behavioural inhibition, which makes the child poorly responsive to socialisation. Reference KochanskaKochanska (1993) suggested that behavioural inhibition is critical in the development of conscience in young children and reported that low-fear children did not respond to the type of socialisation (gentle, non-power, assertive discipline) that led to conscience development in more fearful children. Subsequent studies in children with conduct disorder indicated that the style parents use to socialise their child has less impact on the development of conduct problems in children with callous-unemotional traits, suggesting that genetic or neurodevelopmental factors make a more significant contribution in children who are notably callous.

Psychopathy in children and adolescents

The existence and assessment of psychopathy in children and adolescents is a contentious issue (Reference Edens, Skeem and CruiseEdens et al, 2001; Reference Hart, Watts and VincentHart et al, 2002; Reference Seagrave and GrissoSeagrave & Grisso, 2002). Key concerns centre on the reliability and validity of current assessment tools, the developmental appropriateness of these measures, how closely the construct mimics that in adulthood and the potentially negative impact of attaching a label of psychopathy to those who have not yet reached maturity (Box 1).

Box 1 Key concerns about psychopathy assessment in juveniles

  1. How early can psychopathic traits be reliability detected?

  2. Are there developmentally appropriate measures of psychopathy for use in children and adolescents?

  3. What is the prevalence of psychopathy in childhood and adolescence?

  4. Does its presentation mimic that in adulthood and has it construct and concurrent validity?

  5. What is the developmental stability of psychopathy over the lifespan?

  6. What is the predictive validity of current childhood and adolescent measures of psychopathy?

  7. What is the impact of attributing a label of psychopathy in childhood and adolescence?

Reference FrickFrick (2002) argues that psychopathy presents no more of a challenge than any other measure of psychopathology in children and adolescents, and suggests that psychopathy assessment in juveniles may be a means of early detection and intervention in high-risk groups.

Can psychopathic traits be reliably detected?

The notion that personality disorder and psychopathy can be diagnosed in childhood and adolescence has provoked debate in the literature. Some (e.g. Reference FrickFrick, 2002; Reference LynamLynam, 2002) argue that, theoretically, personality traits are relatively stable across adolescence into adulthood and that there remarkable similarities between the literature on psychopathy in adults and that emerging on children and adolescents. Others (e.g. Reference Seagrave and GrissoSeagrave & Grisso, 2002) suggest that psychopathy as a construct has a high false-positive rate in adolescence, as this is period of considerable developmental change. Reference CleckleyCleckley (1976) also noted that certain transient developmental behaviours and attributes that arise in childhood and adolescence resemble psychopathic traits but attenuate with normal development. For example, adolescents are known to be more impulsive and have less empathic understanding than adults, which might result in higher scores on these items on current psychopathy measures.

To date, there have been no studies looking at the stability of psychopathic traits across the younger age range, and such work is needed. In the adult literature it has been noted that the behavioural items are less stable than the interpersonal/affective items. This has led to recent proposals that psychopathy should focus primarily on callous-unemotional and affective traits, as these are the core components that characterise it and they have been found to be relatively stable, remaining apparent past the age of 40, when the more impulsive, aggressive and criminal behaviours tend to decline or ‘burn out’.

Are there developmentally appropriate measures of psychopathy?

Some researchers have modified adult measures of psychopathy to take account of developmental and social/peer-group influences in an attempt to deal with the above concerns. For example, Reference Forth, Kosson and HareForth et al(2004) modified PCL–R items for adolescents. Others have developed new measures with items that may be more applicable to children and adolescents (e.g. Reference LynamLynam, 1997; Reference Frick, Cooke, Forth and HareFrick, 1998). But although there are now measures of psychopathy in childhood and adolescence, four of which are listed in Box 2, there is a distinct lack of data on their temporal stability.

Box 2 Current intruments for assessing psychopathy in children and adolescents

Psychopathy Checklist: Youth Version (PCL–YV; Reference Forth, Kosson and HareForth et al (2004)

A clinician-rated scale for use with adolescents aged 13–18 years. The PCL–YV, like the adult PCL–R from which it was derived, has a two-factor structure, one reflecting the interpersonal and affective aspects of personality function and the other reflecting the chronic antisocial lifestyle.

Psychopathy Screening Device (PSD; Reference Frick, Cooke, Forth and HareFrick, 1998)

(Also becoming know as the Antisocial Process Screening Device.) This 20-item measure was designed to measure psychopathy in children aged 6–12 years. It is completed by teachers or parents, rather than by self-report or interview with the young people themselves, because of concerns about the validity of children's accounts of their own emotional and behavioural difficulties (Reference Kamphaus and FrickKamphaus & Frick, 1996).

Childhood Psychopathy Scale (CPS; Reference LynamLynam, 1997)

A parent/teacher-rated scale designed to assess psychopathy in late childhood and early adolescence. The 13-item CPS was derived from the Pittsburgh Youth study on 12–13-year-old boys and has a set of items that are similar to the behavioural items on the adult PCL–R.

Youth Psychopathic Traits Inventory (YPI; Reference Andershed, Kerr, Stattin, Blaauw and SheridanAndershed et al, 2002)

A self-report, 50-item scale designed for young people of 12 years or older. Its sub-scales include dishonest charm, grandiosity, lying, manipulation, callousness, unemotionality, remorselessness, irresponsibility and thrill-seeking. Respondents are asked to rate the degree to which individual statements or items apply to them. To avoid response distortion and social desirability effects, items are framed as potentially positive attributes. For example, for dishonest charm a sample item is ‘When I need to, I use my smile and my charm to use others’. The instrument primarily focuses on the core personality traits of the psychopathic personality constellation, rather than on the behavioural traits associated with it.

What are the psychometric properties of current psychopathy measures?

To date, there is relatively little literature on the psychometric properties of measures of psychopathy designed for use with children and adolescents. Preliminary data (see below) show that the external correlates of psychopathy in young people are similar to those seen in affected adults, suggesting some phenotypic similarities. For the Psychopathy Checklist: Youth Version (PCL–YV) (Boxes 2 & 3) there is evidence of adequate internal consistency and interrater reliability for total scores (Reference Forth, Burke, Cooke, Forth and HareForth & Burke, 1998). However, relatively little is known about the factor structure of this instrument across settings, genders and ethnic groups and there have been no studies of its test–retest reliability.

Box 3 Item content of the Hare Psychopathy Checklist: Youth Version (after Reference Forth, Kosson and Hare Forth et al, 2004 )

  1. 1 Impression management Conforms with notions of social desirability, presents him- or herself in a good light, is superficially charming

  2. 2 Grandiose sense of self-worth Is dominating, opinionated, has an inflated view of own ability

  3. 3 Stimulation-seeking Needs novelty, excitement, is prone to boredom and risk-taking behaviours

  4. 4 Pathological lying Exhibits pervasive lying, lies readily, easily and obviously

  5. 5 Manipulation for personal gain Is deceitful, manipulates, engages in dishonest or fraudulent schemes that can result in criminal activity

  6. 6 Lack of remorse Has no guilt, lacks concern about the impact of his or her actions on others; justifies and rationalise their abuse of others

  7. 7 Shallow affect Has only superficial bonds with others, feigns emotion

  8. 8 Callous or lacking empathy Has a profound lack of empathy, views others as objects, has no appreciation of the needs or feelings of others

  9. 9 Parasitic orientation Exploits others, lives at the expense of friends and family, gets others to do his or her schoolwork using threats

  10. 10 Poor anger control Is hotheaded, easily offended and reacts aggressively, is easily provoked to violence

  11. 11 Impersonal sexual behaviour Has multiple casual sexual encounters, indiscriminate sexual relationships, uses coercion and threats

  12. 12 Early behavioural problems Lying, thieving, fire-setting before 10 years of age

  13. 13 Lacks goals Has no interest or understanding of the need for education, lives day-to-day, has unrealistic aspirations for the future

  14. 14 Impulsivity Acts out frequently, quits school, leaves home on a whim, acts on the spur of the moment, never considers the consequences of impulsive acts

  15. 15 Irresponsibility Habitually fails to honour obligations or debts, shows reckless behaviour in a variety of settings, including school and home

  16. 16 Failure to accept responsibility Blames other for his or her problems, claims that he or she was ‘set up’, is unable and unwilling to accept personal responsibility for their actions

  17. 17 Unstable interpersonal relationships Has turbulent extrafamilial relationships, lacks commitment and loyalty

  18. 18 Serious criminal behaviour Has multiple charges of convictions for criminal activity

  19. 19 Serious violations of conditional release Has two or more escapes from security or breaches of probation

  20. 20 Criminal versatility Engages in at least six different categories of offending behaviour

The Psychopathy Screening Device (PSD) was initially reported to have a two-factor structure (Reference Frick, O'Brien and WoottonFrick et al, 1994). The first was labelled the impulsive–conduct problem scale, and it roughly corresponded to the social deviance (factor 2) scale of the adult PCL–R. The second was the callous-unemotional scale, which roughly corresponded to the interpersonal/affective (factor 1) scale of the PCL–R. As yet, there are no specific cut-off scores for the PSD, so a diagnosis of prototypical psychopathy cannot be made with this instrument.

Data on the psychometric properties of the Childhood Psychopathy Scale (CPS) is limited and Reference LynamLynam (1997) generally uses the total rather than sub-scale scores in data analyses.

The Youth Psychopathic Traits Inventory (YPI) has been shown to have acceptable internal consistency. Factor analyses have revealed a three-factor structure similar to that reported for adults (Reference Cooke and MichieCooke & Michie, 2001).

To date, there is relatively little information on whether the self-report (the YPI) and parent/teacher-rated (the PSD and CPS) measures are susceptible to response bias or positive impression management (social desirability), and none of these instruments has a validity scale to check response distortion.

Does the presentation mimic that in adults?

Several studies have shown parallels between patterns of offending behaviour in both adults and adolescents identified as psychopaths. Delinquent offenders with pronounced psychopathic traits have an earlier onset of offending (Reference Brandt, Kennedy and PatrickBrandt et al, 1997; Reference Forth, Burke, Cooke, Forth and HareForth & Burke, 1998), commit more crimes, and reoffend more often (Reference Forth, Burke, Cooke, Forth and HareForth & Burke, 1998; Reference Myers, Burket and HarrisMyers, 1995) and more violently (Reference Brandt, Kennedy and PatrickBrandt et al, 1997; Reference Spain, Douglas and PoythressSpain et al, 2004) than non-psychopathic criminal youth. Psychopathy scores have also been found to correlate significantly with the severity of conduct problems, antisocial behaviour and delinquency in adolescents (Reference Forth, Burke, Cooke, Forth and HareForth & Burke, 1998). In children, the CPS score correlates significantly with measures of impulsivity and adds incremental validity to the prediction of delinquency at ages 12 and 13 years. The PSD impulsive–conduct problem scale has been shown to correlate with traditional measures of conduct disorder (Reference Frick, O'Brien and WoottonFrick et al, 1994). Furthermore, Reference Christian, Frick and HillChristian et al(1997) have also demonstrated that children with callous-unemotional traits engage in more persistent antisocial behaviour. In addition, they exhibit insensitivity to punishment cues irrespective of whether or not they have conduct problems (Reference O'Brien and FrickO'Brien & Frick, 1996). These findings appear to fit with similar studies examining emotional and information-processing deficits in both adolescents and adults identified as psychopaths and assessed using similar measures (Reference Hare, Cooke, Forth and HareHare, 1998; Reference Newman, Cooke, Forth and HareNewman, 1998). Studies examining the validity of the YPI also indicate high correlations with early behavioural problems, poor behavioural control and hyperactivity–impulsivity attention difficulties (Reference Andershed, Kerr, Stattin, Blaauw and SheridanAndershed et al, 2002). However, this measure has not yet been compared with alternative measures of juvenile psychopathy.

Prevalence of psychopathy in forensic samples

At present, there are no data on the prevalence of psychopathy in the juvenile general population. Using a PCL–YV cut-off score of >30 in a population of adolescents already involved with criminal or psychiatric services, Reference Forth, Burke, Cooke, Forth and HareForth & Burke (1998) found rates of psychopathy of 3.5% in young people in community care, 12% in those on probation and 28.3% in those incarcerated. Reference Brandt, Kennedy and PatrickBrandt et al(1997) reported a prevalence of 37% in incarcerated youths, using a PCL–YV cut-off score of >28. As there are no recommended cut-off scores for diagnoses of psychopathy using the CPS, PSD or YPI, prevalence rates using these measures have not been reported.

Overall, there seems to be some evidence that the base rate of psychopathy in adolescence is higher (Reference Brandt, Kennedy and PatrickBrandt et al, 1997; Reference Forth, Burke, Cooke, Forth and HareForth & Burke, 1998) than that reported in adult samples (Reference HareHare, 1991, Reference Hare, Cooke, Forth and Hare1998). This has prompted questions about the validity of current ‘age-appropriate’ measures and the stability of psychopathy over the lifespan (Reference Edens, Skeem and CruiseEdens et al, 2001; Reference Seagrave and GrissoSeagrave & Grisso, 2002). If the construct of psychopathy were valid and stable then one would expect similar prevalence rates across the lifespan. It is possible that the inflated scores on adolescent psychopathy measures are due to higher ratings on items relating to impulsivity and irresponsibility in younger samples.

Comorbidity

The only published studies looking at comorbidity of psychopathy with other psychiatric disorders in adolescent populations have focused on groups with substance misuse disorders, where the prevalence of comorbid psychopathy is high (e.g. Reference Brandt, Kennedy and PatrickBrandt et al, 1997; Reference Mailloux, Forth and KronerMailloux et al, 1997). Although the social deviance component of psychopathy shows item overlap with the criteria for DSM–IV disruptive behaviour disorders and attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), no study has specifically addressed the nature of the relationship between psychopathic traits and these disorders in younger cohorts. This is likely to be an important area for further research, as deficits in executive function have been reported in individuals with these disorders as well as in people who score highly on the impulsive-aggressive domains of the psychopathy construct.

The callous-unemotional and interpersonal aspects of psychopathy share some features with the pervasive developmental disorders. To date, there have been no studies in forensic cohorts to explore the similarities and differences between these disorders or the level of comorbidity between them, despite evidence that autistic-spectrum disorders are prevalent in criminal samples (Reference Soderstrom, Sjodin and CarlstedtSoderstrom et al, 2004). As amygdala dysfunction has been implicated in the pathogenesis of both autism and psychopathy and both disorders are viewed as involving impairments in social functioning there is a need to clarify how these disorders can be differentiated so that appropriate treatment programmes can be developed.

Outcome

In the adult literature there is some evidence that psychopathy is a significant negative moderator of treatment outcome (Reference Losel, Cooke, Forth and HareLosel, 1998). To date, two studies have looked at the relationship between psychopathy and treatment outcome in adolescents, but none has considered this in children. Reference Rogers, Johansen and ChangRogers et al(1997) found modest correlations between psychopathy scores and ratings of non-compliance with treatment in 81 adolescent in-patients. Reference O'Neill, Lidz and HeilbrunO'Neill et al(2003) also reported that psychopathy score was negatively correlated with attendance rates, quality of participation and clinical improvement in adolescents in a substance misuse treatment programme. These findings suggest that psychopathy may be an important moderator of treatment outcome in adolescents as in adults, and specialised programmes may be needed for young people with psychopathic traits. Reference Forth, Burke, Cooke, Forth and HareForth & Burke (1998) also point out that juveniles identified as psychopathic may be more ‘malleable’ and benefit more from treatment than their older counterparts

Psychopathy as a predictor of violence?

Psychopathy has been shown to be a robust predictor of future institutional and post-discharge violence in adults (Reference Salekin, Rogers and SewellSalekin et al, 1996), and the results of studies in adolescent samples largely concur with this finding. Psychopathy scores in adolescents have been found to be associated with both the frequency of violent recidivism (Reference Forth, Hart and HareForth et al, 1990) and a shorter time to violent recidivism (Reference Brandt, Kennedy and PatrickBrandt et al, 1997). Studies of institutional violence indicate moderately strong correlations between psychopathy score and verbal and physical aggression (Reference Edens, Skeem and CruiseEdens et al, 2001; Reference Spain, Douglas and PoythressSpain et al, 2004).

None of the adolescent studies has examined which (if any) of the callous-unemotional or behavioural factors of the psychopathy subscales contribute most to predictive accuracy. As yet, there have been no risk prediction studies at all in children with psychopathic traits.

Are the neurocognitive markers similar in young people and adults?

From a neurocognitive perspective there is some evidence that children and adolescents with psychopathy display deficits on experimental tasks assessing impulse control and empathy (Reference BlairBlair, 1999; Reference Barry, Frick and DeShazoBarry et al, 2000) similar to those seen in adults (Reference Hare, Cooke, Forth and HareHare, 1998; Reference Newman, Cooke, Forth and HareNewman, 1998). In children with emotional and behavioural problems, PSD scores predicted electrodermal responses to distress cues and threatening stimuli (Reference BlairBlair, 1999) as well as differences in moral reasoning (Reference BlairBlair, 1997). This work lends support to Blair's violence inhibition mechanism model of psychopathy.

Use of psychopathy assessments in the criminal justice system

A number of researchers and clinicians in the field have advocated caution in the use of current psychopathy assessment tools as a means of informing the criminal justice system on sentence planning for young people, particularly in light of the fact that in adults a diagnosis of psychopathy can result in more severe sentences. Instruments developed for assessing psychopathy in child and adolescent populations require further validation in a variety of populations and settings before they can be fully accepted. Only then can they be used with confidence in the criminal justice system.

Conclusions

The assessment of psychopathy in children and adolescents is a very important area of research and it is still in its infancy. There seems to be reasonable evidence that juvenile psychopathy shows similar correlates (e.g. aggression, neurocognitive deficits, substance misuse) to adult psychopathy. Our knowledge about the nature, stability and consequences of juvenile psychopathy, however, is still very limited. There have been no published longitudinal studies of the stability of psychopathy as assessed by any of the current measures and it remains unclear to what degree the antisocial behavioural items that contribute to the psychopathy label change over time, given what we know about adolescent-limited antisocial behaviours. The limited data on the psychometric properties of current instruments, particularly information on recommended or specific cut-off scores for prototypical psychopathy, suggest that it is premature to assign this label to younger cohorts. For this reason many researchers in this field refer to juveniles with ‘psychopathic characteristics’ rather than using the term ‘psychopathy’.

However, these instruments do include items we know to be associated with high-risk behaviours, and they can inform clinical assessments of risk and treatment planning.

At present, there is no general agreement on whether or not psychopathy exists in childhood and adolescence. A consensus is likely to be reached only when we have longitudinal studies demonstrating the stability of psychopathic traits over the lifespan and evidence that the same aetiological factors contribute to this disorder at all ages. As there is significant overlap between the behavioural aspects of juvenile psychopathy and ADHD and between the callous-unemotional dimension of psychopathy and autistic-spectrum disorders, future work needs to disentangle these constructs from a phenomenological and aetiological perspective.

As yet, there are few treatment outcome studies in juveniles with psychopathic traits, although the limited data suggest that these traits might be a moderator of outcome. Most clinicians view youth psychopathy as a potentially treatable disorder, and there is some evidence that identification of psychopathic traits in young people has a number of benefits, which include:

  1. identifying high-risk offenders;

  2. reducing misclassifications that have negative ramifications for children and adolescents;

  3. improving and optimising treatment planning for young people with psychopathic traits, who may require more intensive and risk-focused therapeutic approaches.

MCQs

  1. 1 Which of the following best answer the question ‘Is psychopathy the same as conduct disorder and antisocial personality?’

    1. a yes

    2. b no

    3. c it overlaps with antisocial personality

    4. d it overlaps with conduct disorder

    5. e not all conduct disorder and antisocial personality disorders meet the criteria for psychopathy.

  2. 2 Regarding measures of psychopathy for those under 18 years old

    1. a there are no available measures of psychopathy for those under 18 years old

    2. b there are several self-report, informant-rated and interview measures for those under 18 years old

    3. c current instruments provide well-validated cut-off scores for making a prototypical diagnosis

    4. d the instruments for those under 13 years old are based on either self-report or informant-rated measures

    5. e the only current interview schedule is for those aged 13–18 years.

  3. 3 Which of the following measures have been independently validated in a variety of international settings and groups?

    1. a the Psychopathy Checklist – Youth Version

    2. b the Youth Psychopathic Traits Inventory

    3. c the Child Psychopathy Scale

    4. d all of the above

    5. e none of the above.

  4. 4 Psychopathy in childhood and adolescence is associated with:

    1. a earlier onset of antisocial behaviour

    2. b more criminal convictions

    3. c more violent offences

    4. d substance misuse

    5. e potentially poorer response to treatment.

  5. 5 Psychopathy in childhood and adolescence is:

    1. a well recognised and a stable diagnosis across age ranges and from childhood to adulthood

    2. b potentially useful in subtyping those who may be at high risk and need specialist interventions

    3. c associated with neurocognitive (empathy and impulse control) deficits similar to those seen in adults with this disorder

    4. d routinely assessed and used in sentence planning by the criminal justice system

    5. e a potentially stigmatising label for a child or adolescent.

MCQ answers

1 2 3 4 5
a F a F a F a T a F
b F b T b F b T b T
c T c F c F c T c T
d T d T d F d T d F
e T e T e T e T e T

Footnotes

This article continues the series entitled ‘Lifespan psychiatry’, which considers the developmental psychiatry of adulthood. Previous articles looked at attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (Reference Zwi and YorkZwi & York, 2004; Invited commentaries: Reference AshersonAsherson, 2004 and Reference CoghillCoghill, 2004) and Asperger syndrome (Reference BerneyBerney, 2004).

References

Andershed, H., Kerr, M., Stattin, H. et al (2002) Psychopathic traits in non-referred youths: a new assessment tool. In Psychopaths – Current International Perspectives (eds Blaauw, E. & Sheridan, L.) The Hague: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Asherson, P. P. (2004) Bridging the service divide. Invited commentary on Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder in adults. Advances in Psychiatric Treatment, 10, 257259.Google Scholar
Barry, C.T., Frick, P.J., DeShazo, T.M. et al (2000) The importance of callous-unemotional traits for extending the concept of psychopathy to children. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109, 335340.Google Scholar
Berney, T. (2004) Asperger syndrome from childhood into adulthood. Advances in Psychiatric Treatment, 10, 341351.Google Scholar
Blair, R. J. R. (1995) A cognitive developmental approach to morality: investigating the psychopath. Cognition, 57, 129.Google Scholar
Blair, R. J. R. (1997) Moral reasoning and the child with psychopathic tendencies. Personality and Individual Differences, 22, 731739.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blair, R. J. R. (1999) Responsiveness to distress cues in the child with psychopathic tendencies. Personality and Individual Differences, 27, 135145.Google Scholar
Blair, R. J. R., Colledge, E., Murray, L et al (2001) A selective impairment in the processing of sad and fearful expressions in children with psychopathic tendencies? Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 29, 491498.Google Scholar
Brandt, J. R., Kennedy, W. A., Patrick, C. J. et al (1997) Assessment of psychopathy in a population of incarcerated adolescent offenders. Psychological Assessment, 9, 429435.Google Scholar
Christian, R. E., Frick, P. J., Hill, N. et al (1997) Psychopathy and conduct problems in children: II. Sub typing children with conduct problems based on their interpersonal and affective style. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 36, 233241.Google Scholar
Cleckley, H. (1976) The Mask of Sanity (5th edn). St Louis, MI: Mosby Google Scholar
Coghill, D. (2004) Understudied and underrecognised. Invited commentary on Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder in adults. Advances in Psychiatric Treatment, 10, 338340.Google Scholar
Cooke, D.J. & Michie, C. (2001) Refining the construct of psychopathy: towards a hierarchical model. Psychological Assessment, 13, 171188.Google Scholar
Damasio, A. (1994) Descartes' Error: Emotion, Reason, and the Human Brian. New York: Putnam & Sons Google Scholar
Edens, J.F., Skeem, J. L., Cruise, K. R. et al (2001) Assessment of juvenile psychopathy' and its association with violence. A critical review. Behavioral Sciences and the Law, 19, 5358.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Forth, A. E. & Burke, H.C. (1998) Psychopathy in adolescence: assessment, violence and developmental precursors. In Psychopathy: Theory, Research and Implications for Society (eds Cooke, D., Forth, A. & Hare, R.) pp. 205230. Dordrecht: Kluwer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Forth, A. E., Hart, S. D. & Hare, R. D. (1990) Assessment of psychopathy in male young offenders. Psychological Assessment: A Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 2, 342344.Google Scholar
Forth, A. E., Kosson, D. S. & Hare, R.D. (2004) The Hare Psychopathy Checklist: Youth Version (PCL-YV) – Rating Guide. Toronto, Ontario: Multi Health Systems.Google Scholar
Frick, P. J. (1998) Callous-unemotional traits and conduct problems: applying the two-factor model of psychopathy to children. In Psychopathy: Theory, Research and Implications for Society (eds Cooke, D., Forth, A. & Hare, R.) pp. 161189. Dordrecht: Kluwer.Google Scholar
Frick, P. J. (2002) Juvenile psychopathy from a developmental perspective: Implications for construct development and use in forensic assessments. Law and Human Behaviour, 26, 247253.Google Scholar
Frick, P. J., O'Brien, B. S., Wootton, J. M. et al (1994) Psychopathy and conduct disorders in children. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103, 700707.Google Scholar
Hare, R. D. (1991) The Hare Psychopathy Checklist - Revised. Toronto: Multi Health Systems.Google Scholar
Hare, R. D. (1998) Psychopathy, affect and behaviour. In Psychopathy: Theory, Research and Implications for Society (eds Cooke, D., Forth, A. & Hare, R.) pp. 105139. Dordrecht: Kluwer.Google Scholar
Hart, S. D., Watts, K. & Vincent, G. M. (2002) Commentary on Seagrave and Grisso: Impressions of the state of the art. Law and Human Behavior, 26, 241245.Google Scholar
Kamphaus, R. W. & Frick, P. J. (1996) Clinical Assessment of Child and Adolescent Personality and Behavior. Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon.Google Scholar
Kochanska, G. (1993) Towards a synthesis of parental socialisation and child temperament in early development of conscience. Child Development, 64, 228240.Google Scholar
Kosson, D., Cyterski, T., Steuerwald, B. et al (2001) The reliability and validity of the Psychopathy Checklist: Youth Version (PCL: VY) in non-incarcerated adolescent males. Psychological Assessment, 14, 97109.Google Scholar
Losel, F (1998) Treatment and management of psychopaths. In Psychopathy: Theory, Research, and Implications for Society (eds Cooke, D. J., Forth, A. E. & Hare, R. D.) pp. 303355. Dordrecht: Kluwer.Google Scholar
Lykken, D. T. (1995) The Antisocial Personalities. Hillsdale. NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Lynam, D.R. (1997) Pursuing the psychopath: capturing the fledgling psychopath in a nomological net. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 106, 425438.Google Scholar
Lynam, D.R. (2002) Fledgling psychopathy: a view from personality theory. Law and Human Behavior, 26, 255259.Google Scholar
Mailloux, D. L., Forth, A. E. & Kroner, D. G. (1997) Psychopathy and substance use in adolescent male offenders. Psychological Reports, 80, 529530.Google Scholar
McBurnett, K. & Lahey, B. B. (1994) Psychophysiological and neuroendocrine correlates of conduct disorder and antisocial behaviour in children and adolescents. In Progress in Experimental Personality & Psychopathology Research (eds Fowles, D. C., Sutker, P. & Goodman, S.) pp. 199231. New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Moffitt, T. E. & Henry, B. (1991) Neuropsychological studies of juvenile delinquency and violence: a review. In Neuropsychology of Aggression (ed. Milner, J. S.) pp. 131140. Norwell: Kluwer Academic.Google Scholar
Myers, W. C., Burket, R. C. & Harris, H. E. (1995) Adolescent psychopathy in relation to delinquent behaviours, conduct disorder, and personality disorder. Journal of Forensic Sciences, 40, 436440.Google Scholar
Newman, J. P. (1998) Psychopathic behaviour: An information processing perspective. In Psychopathy: Theory, Research, and Implications for Society (eds Cooke, D. J., Forth, A. E. & Hare, R. D.) pp. 81104. Dordrecht: Kluwer.Google Scholar
O'Brien, B. S. & Frick, P. J. (1996) Reward dominance: Associations with anxiety conduct problems and psychopathy in children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 24, 223240.Google Scholar
O'Neill, M., Lidz, V. & Heilbrun, K. (2003) Adolescents with psychopathic characteristics in a substance-abusing cohort. Treatment process and outcomes. Law and Human Behavior, 27, 299313.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Patrick, C. J. (1994) Emotion and psychopathy: startling new insights. Psychophysiology, 31, 319330.Google Scholar
Raine, A. (1993) The Psychopathology of Crime: Criminal Behavior as a Clinical Ddisorder. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Rogers, R., Johansen, J., Chang, J. J. et al (1997) Predictors of adolescent psychopathy: oppositional and conduct-disordered symptoms. Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law, 25, 261271.Google Scholar
Salekin, R., Rogers, R. & Sewell, K. (1996) A review and meta-analysis of the Psychopathy Checklist and Psychopathy Checklist – Revised: predictive validity of dangerousness. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 3, 203215.Google Scholar
Seagrave, D. & Grisso, T. (2002) Adolescent development and the measurement of juvenile psychopathy. Law and Human Behavior, 26, 219239.Google Scholar
Soderstrom, H., Sjodin, A.-K. & Carlstedt, A. F. (2004) Adult psychopathic personality with childhood-onset hyperactivity and conduct disorder: a central problem constellation in forensic psychiatry. Psychiatry Research, 121, 271280.Google Scholar
Spain, S. E., Douglas, K. S., Poythress, N. G. et al (2004) The relationship between psychopathic features, violence and treatment outcomes: the comparison of three youth measures of psychopathic features. Behavioural Sciences and the Law, 22, 85102.Google Scholar
Zwi, M. & York, A. (2004) Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder in adults: validity unknown. Advances in Psychiatric Treatment, 10, 248256.Google Scholar
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.