Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-8bhkd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-15T01:37:15.787Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Nonword repetition, phonological memory, and vocabulary: A reply to Snowling, Chiat, and Hulme

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 November 2008

Susan E. Gathercole*
Affiliation:
Lancaster University
Cath Willis
Affiliation:
Lancaster University
Alan D. Baddeley
Affiliation:
MRC Applied Psychology Unit, Cambridge, England
*
Dr. Susan Gathercole, Department of Psychology, Lancaster University, Lancaster LA1 4YF, England

Extract

The opportunity taken by Snowling, Chiat, and Hulme to step into the debate concerning the nature of the relationship between nonword repetition abilities and vocabulary acquisition in young children should be welcomed. Vocabulary size is strongly associated with a range of abilities, including general intelligence scores, reading ability, reading comprehension, and school success (e.g., Anderson & Freebody, 1981) and as a consequence, vocabulary knowledge provides the major index of verbal intelligence in many standardized ability tests used with both children and adults. Given the weight attached by psychologists to vocabulary knowledge, it seems surprising that until recently the cognitive processes underpinning word learning had been largely neglected. Any progress in understanding the psychological constraints in vocabulary development, whether it takes the form of informed theoretical debate or further empirical work, should therefore be encouraged.

Type
Dialogue
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1991

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Anderson, R. C., & Freebody, P. (1981). Vocabulary knowledge. In Guthrie, J. T. (Ed.), Comprehension and teaching: Research reviews. Newark, DE: International Reading Association.Google Scholar
Baddeley, A. D., Papagno, C., & Vallar, G. (1988). When long-term learning depends on short-term storage. Journal of Memory and Language, 27, 586596.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D. (1990 a). Phonological memory deficits in language disordered children: Is there a causal connection? Journal of Memory and Language, 29, 336360.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D. (1990b). The role of phonological memory in vocabulary acquisition: A study of young children learning arbitrary names of toys. British Journal of Psychology, 81, 439454.Google Scholar
Gathercole, S. E., Willis, C., Emslie, H., & Baddeley, A. (1991). The influences of number of syllables and wordlikeness on children’s repetition of nonwords. Applied Psycholinguistics, 12, 349367.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hulme, C., Thomson, N., C., Muir, & Lawrence, A. (1984). Speech rate and the development of short-term memory span. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 38, 241253.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Papagno, C., Valentine, T., & Baddeley, A. (1991). Phonological short-term memory and foreign-language vocabulary learning. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 331347.Google Scholar
Service, E. (1989). Phonological coding in working memory and foreign-language learning. University of Helsinki General Psychology Monographs, B9.Google Scholar
Snowling, M., Chiat, S., & Hulme, C. (1991). Words, nonwords, and phonological processes: Some comments on Gathercole, Willis, Emslie, and Baddeley. Applied Psycholinguistics, 12, 369373.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stark, R., & Tallal, P. (1981). Selection of children with specific language deficits. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 46, 114122.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed