Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-94fs2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-11T03:38:23.285Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Language and analogy in conceptual change

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  19 May 2011

Dedre Gentner
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Northwestern University, Evanston, IL 60208. gentner@northwestern.eduhttp://groups.psych.northwestern.edu/gentner/ninasimms@northwestern.edu
Nina Simms
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Northwestern University, Evanston, IL 60208. gentner@northwestern.eduhttp://groups.psych.northwestern.edu/gentner/ninasimms@northwestern.edu

Abstract

Carey proposes that the acquisition of the natural numbers relies on the interaction between language and analogical processes: specifically, on an analogical mapping from ordinal linguistic structure to ordinal conceptual structure. We suggest that this analogy in fact requires several steps. Further, we propose that additional analogical processes enter into the acquisition of number.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2011

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Carey, S. (2004) Bootstrapping and the origin of concepts. Daedalus 133:5968.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carey, S. (2009) The origin of concepts. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Everett, D. (2005) Cultural constraints on grammar and cognition in Pirahã: Another look at the design features of human language. Current Anthropology 76:621–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frank, M. C., Everett, D. L., Fedorenko, E. & Gibson, E. (2008) Number as cognitive technology: Evidence from Pirahã language and cognition. Cognition 108:819–24.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fuson, K. C. (1988) Children's counting and concepts of number. Springer Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gentner, D. (2003) Why we're so smart. In: Language in mind: Advances in the study of language and cognition, ed. Gentner, D. and Goldin-Meadow, S., pp. 195236. MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gentner, D. (2010) Bootstrapping children's learning: Analogical processes and symbol systems. Cognitive Science 34(5):752–75.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gentner, D. & Christie, S. (2008) Relational language supports relational cognition in humans and apes. Behavioral & Brain Sciences 31:136–37.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gentner, D. & Medina, J. (1998) Similarity and the development of rules. Cognition 65:263–97.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gordon, P. (2004) Numerical cognition without words: Evidence from Amazonia. Science 306(5695):496–99.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Loewenstein, J. & Gentner, D. (2005) Relational language and the development of relational mapping. Cognitive Psychology 50:315–53.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mix, K. S., Sandhofer, C. M. & Baroody, A. J. (2005) Number words and number concepts: The interplay of verbal and nonverbal quantification in early childhood. In: Advances in child development and behavior, vol. 33, ed. Kail, R. V., pp. 305–46. Academic Press.Google Scholar
Pica, P., Lemer, C., Izard, V. & Dehaene, S. (2004) Exact and approximate arithmetic in an Amazonian indigene group. Science 306(5695):499503.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Russell, B. (1920) Introduction to mathematical philosophy. Macmillan.Google Scholar
Wynn, K. (1990) Children's understanding of counting. Cognition 36:155–93.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed