Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-ndw9j Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-13T08:22:37.771Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The case of the neglected alphasyllabary: Orthographic processing in Devanagari

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  29 August 2012

Chaitra Rao
Affiliation:
National Brain Research Centre, NH-8, Nainwal Mode, Manesar –122050, India. chaitlee@gmail.comshwetanbrc@gmail.comnandini@nbrc.ac.inhttp://www.nbrc.ac.in/faculty/nandini/mindinyourlanguage.blogspot.com
Shweta Soni
Affiliation:
National Brain Research Centre, NH-8, Nainwal Mode, Manesar –122050, India. chaitlee@gmail.comshwetanbrc@gmail.comnandini@nbrc.ac.inhttp://www.nbrc.ac.in/faculty/nandini/mindinyourlanguage.blogspot.com
Nandini Chatterjee Singh
Affiliation:
National Brain Research Centre, NH-8, Nainwal Mode, Manesar –122050, India. chaitlee@gmail.comshwetanbrc@gmail.comnandini@nbrc.ac.inhttp://www.nbrc.ac.in/faculty/nandini/mindinyourlanguage.blogspot.com

Abstract

We applaud Ram Frost for highlighting the need for multicultural perspectives while developing universal models of visual word recognition. We second Frost's proposal that factors like lexical morphology should be incorporated besides purely orthographic features in modeling word recognition. In support, we provide fresh evidence from Hindi (written in Devanagari), an example of hitherto under-represented alphasyllabic orthographies, in which flexible encoding of akṣara (character) position is constrained by the morphological structure of words.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2012 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Christianson, K., Johnson, R. L. & Rayner, K. I. (2005) Letter transpositions within and across morphemes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 31:1327–39.Google Scholar
Das, T., Padakannaya, P., Pugh, K. R. & Singh, N. C. (2011) Neuroimaging reveals dual routes to reading in simultaneous proficient readers of two orthographies. NeuroImage 54:1476–87.Google Scholar
Duñabeitia, J. A., Perea, M. & Carreiras, M. (2007) Do transposed-letter similarity effects occur at a morpheme level? Evidence for morpho-orthographic decomposition. Cognition 105(3):691703.Google Scholar
Gold, B. T. & Rastle, K. (2007) Neural correlates of morphological decomposition during visual word recognition. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 19:1983–93.Google Scholar
Grainger, J. (2008) Cracking the orthographic code: An introduction. Language and Cognitive Processes 23:135.Google Scholar
Kachru, Y. (2008) Hindi-Urdu-Hindustani. In: Language in South Asia, ed. Kachru, B. B., Kachru, Y. & Sridhar, S. N., pp. 81102. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Masica, C. (1991) The Indo-Aryan languages. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Perea, M. & Perez, E. (2009) Beyond alphabetic orthographies: The role of form and phonology in transposition effects in Katakana. Language and Cognitive Processes 24:6788.Google Scholar
Vaid, J. & Gupta, A. (2002) Exploring word recognition in a semi-alphabetic script: The case of Devanagari. Brain and Language 81:679–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar