No CrossRef data available.
Article contents
Classical conditioning: The hegemony is not ubiquitous
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 04 February 2010
Abstract
An abstract is not available for this content so a preview has been provided. Please use the Get access link above for information on how to access this content.
- Type
- Continuing Commentary
- Information
- Copyright
- Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1993
References
Boelens, H. & Smeets, P. (1990) An analysis of emergent simple discrimination in children. Quarterly journal of Experimental Psychology 428:135–52. [FT]Google Scholar
Bolles, R. C. (1984) On the status of causal modes. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7:482–83. [FT]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bolles, R. C. (1985) A cognitive, nonassociative view of inhibition. In: Information processing in animals: Conditioned inhibition, ed. Miller, R. R. & Spear, N. E.. Erlbaum. [FT]Google Scholar
Branch, M. N. (1977) On the role of “memory” in the analysis of behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 28:171–79. [FT]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, B. L. (1985) Pavlovian factors in choice behavior. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8:333. [FT]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Catania, A. C., Horne, P. & Lowe, C. F. (1988) Transfer of function across members of an equivalence class. Presented at the second European Meeting on the Experimental Analysis of Behaviour, Liège, Belgium, July. [FT]Google Scholar
Costello, C. G. (1982) Fears and phobias in women: A community study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 4:280–89. [HM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crowne, D. P. & Marlowe, D. (1964) Approval motive: Studies in evaluative dependence. Wiley, ed.[JST]Google Scholar
Davey, G. C. (1987) An integration of human and animal models of Pavlovian conditioning: Associations, cognitions, and attributions. In: Cognitive processes and Pavlovian conditioning in humans, ed. Davey, G.. Wiley. [rJST, HM]Google Scholar
Davey, G. C. (1989) UCS revaluation and conditioning models of acquired fears. Behavior Research and Therapy 27:521–28. [HM]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Denny, M. R. (1986) “Retention” of S-R in the midst of the cognitive invasion. In: Theories of animal memory, ed. Kendrick, D. F., Rilling, M. R. & Denny, M. R.. Erlbaum. [rJST]Google Scholar
de Rose, J. C., McIlvane, W. J., Dube, W. V., Galpin, V. C. & Stoddard, L. T. (1988) Emergent simple discrimination established by indirect relation to differential consequences. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 50:1–20. [rJST, FT]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dickinson, A. (1980) Contemporary animal learning theory. Cambridge University Press, ed.FT]Google Scholar
Dickinson, A. & Mackintosh, N. J. (1978) Classical conditioning in animals. Annual Review of Psychology 29:587–612. [FT]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dinsmoor, J. A. (1985) The integrative power of the CS-US interval in other contexts. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8:336–37. [FT]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dohrenwend, B., Pearlin, L., Clayton, P., Riley, M., Hamburg, B. & Rose, R. M. (1982) Panel report on stress and life events. In: Stress and human health: Analysis and implications of research, ed. Elliott, G. R. & Eisdorfer, C.. Springer. [rJST]Google Scholar
Dube, W. V., Mellvane, W. J., Mackay, H. A. & Stoddard, L. T. (1987) Stimulus class membership established via stimulus-reinforcer relations. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 47:159–75. [FT]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eysenck, H. J. (1979) The conditioning model of neurosis. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2:155–99. [HM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fantino, E. (1977) Conditioned reinforcement: Choice and information. In: Handbook of operant behavior, ed. Honig, W. K. & Staddon, J. E. R.. Prentice-Hall. [FT]Google Scholar
Fields, C. (1989) Explaining classical conditioning: Phenomenological unity conceals mechanistic diversity. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12:141–42. [FF]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fields, L., Adams, B. J., Verhave, T. & Newman, S. (1990) The effects of nodality on the formation of equivalence classes. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 53:345–58. [FT]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fields, L., Verhave, T. & Fath, S. (1984) Stimulus equivalence and transitive associations: A methodological analysis. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 42:143–57. [FT]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Furedy, J. J. (1979) Teaching self-regulation of cardiac function through imaginational Pavlovian and biofeedback conditioning. Remember the response. In: Biofeedback and self-regulation, ed. Birnabaumer, N. & Kimniel, H. D.. Lawrence Erlbaum. [RBM]Google Scholar
Furedy, J. J. (1989) Flights of teleological fancy about classical conditioning do not produce valid science or useful technology. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12:142–43. [RBM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Furedy, J. J. & Riley, D. M. (1987) Human Pavlovian autonomic conditioning and the cognitive paradigm. In: Cognitive processes and Pavlovian conditioning in humans, ed. Davey, G.. Wiley. [RBM]Google Scholar
Furedy, J. J., Shulhan, D. J. & Randall, D. C. (1989) Human Pavlovian HR decelerative conditioning with negative tilt as US: A review of some S-R, stimulus-substitution evidence. International Journal of Psychophysiology 7:19–23. [RBM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gardner, L. & Malmo, R. B. (1969) Effects of low-level septal stimulation on escape: Significance for limbic-midbrain interactions in pain. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 68:65–73. [RBM]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gatch, M. B. & Osborne, J. G. (1989) Transfer of contextual stimulus function via equivalence class development. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 51:369–78. [FT]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldiamond, I. (1966) Perception, language, and conceptualization rules. In: Problem solving: Research, method, and theory, ed. Kleinmuntz, B.. Wiley. [FT]Google Scholar
Gormezano, I., Kehoe, E. J. & Marshall, B. S. (1983) Twenty years of classical conditioning with the rabbit. Progress in Psychobiology and Physiological Psychology 10:198–264. [rJST]Google Scholar
Hall, G. (1988) Reasoning and associative learning. Presented at the second European Meeting on the Experimental Analysis of Behaviour, Liège, Belgium, July. [FT]Google Scholar
Hayes, S. C. (1989) Nonhninans have not yet shown stimulus equivalence. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 51:385–92. [FT]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hayes, S. C., Devany, J. M., Kohlenberg, B. S., Brownstein, A. J. & Shelby, J. (1987) Stimulus equivalence and the symbolic control of behavior. Revista Mexicana de Análisis dc la Conducta 13:361–74. [FT]Google Scholar
Hayes, S. C., Hayes, L. J. & Reese, H. W. (1988) Finding the philosophical core: A review of Stephen C. Pepper's World hypotheses: A study in evidence. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of behavior 50:97–111. [FT]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hineline, P. N. (1984) Can a statement in cognitive terms be a behavior-analytic interpretation? Behavior Analyst 7:97–100. [FT]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hineline, P. N. (1986) Re-tuning the operant-respondent distinction. In: Analysis and integration of behavioral units, ed. Thompson, T. & Zeiler, M. D.. Erlbaum. [FT]Google Scholar
Hineline, P. N. (1990) The origins of environment-based psychological theory. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 53:305–20. [FT]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hitchcock, J. M., Reed, T. L., McCabe, P. M. & Schneiderman, N. (1989) Effect of lesions of the spinal trigeminal tract on acquisition of classically conditioned bradycardia in the rabbit. Society for Neuroscience Abstracts 15:888. [RBM]Google Scholar
Holland, P. C. (1981) Acquisition of representation-mediated conditioned food aversions. Learning and Motivation 12:1–18. [FT]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holland, P. C. & Forbes, D. T. (1982) Control of conditional discrimination performance by CS-evokcd event representations. Animal Learning & Behavior 10:249–56. [FT]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jacobs, W. J. (1989) What is classical conditioning? Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12:146. [FT]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jenkins, H. M., Barnes, R. A. & Barrera, F. J. (1981) Why autoshaping depends on trial spacing. In: Autoshaping and conditioning theory, ed. Locurto, C. M., Terrace, H. S. & Gibbon, J.. Academic Press. [FT]Google Scholar
Jenkins, H. M. & Moore, B. R. (1973) The form of the auto-shaped response with food or water reinforcers. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 20:163–81. [FT]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kapp, B. S., Frysinger, R. C., Gallagher, M. & Haselton, J. R. (1979) Amygdala central nucleus lesions: Effect on heart rate conditioning in the rabbit. Physiology and behavior 23:1109–17. [RBM]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kasl, S. V. (1983) Pursuing the link between stressful life experiences and disease: A time for reappraisal. In: Stress research, ed. Cooper, C. L.. Wiley. [rJST]Google Scholar
Kehoe, E. J., Marshall-Goodell, B. & Gormezano, I. (1987) Differential conditioning of the rabbit's nictitating membrane response to serial compound stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 13:17–30. [FT]Google ScholarPubMed
Keller, F. S. & Schoenfeld, W. N. (1950) Principles of psychology: A systematic text in the science of behavior. Appleton-Century-Crofts, [FT]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lal, H. & Emmett-Oglesby, M. W. (1983) Behavioral analogues of anxiety. Neuropharmacology 22:1423–41. [rJST]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lazar, R. (1977) Extending sequence-class membership with matching to sample. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 27:381–92. [FT]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lazar, R. M. & Kotlarchyk, B. J. (1986) Second-order control of sequenceclass equivalences in children. Behavioural Processes 13:205–15. [FT]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, V. L. (1990) Methodological commitments and other philosophic necessities. Behavior Analyst 13:91–94. [FT]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Malmo, R. B. (1963) On central and autonomic nervous system mechanisms in conditioning, learning and performance. Canadian Journal of Psychology 17:1–36. [rJST, RBM]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Malmo, R. B. (1964) Heart rate reactions and locus of stimulation within the septal area of the rat. Science 144:1029–30. [rJST, RBM]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Malmo, R. B. (1965) Classical and instrumental conditioning with septal stimulation as reinforcement. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 60:1–8. [RBM]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Malone, J. C. Jr. (1981) A fourth approach to the study of learning: Are “processes” really necessary? Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4:151–52. [rJST]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marks, I. M. (1977) Phobias and obsessions. Clinical phenomena in search of laboratory models. In: Psychopathology: Experimental models, ed. Maser, J. D. & Seligman, M. E. P.. Freeman. [HM]Google Scholar
McNally, R. J. (1987) Preparedness and phobias: A review. Psychological Bulletin 101:283–303. [HM]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McNally, R. J. & Steketee, G. S. (1985) The etiology and maintenance of severe animal phobias. Behaviour Research and Therapy 23:431–35. [HM]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Merckelbach, H., de Ruiter, C., van den Hout, M. A. & Hoekstra, R. (1989a) Conditioning experiences and phobias. Behavior Research and Therapy 27:658–62. [rJST, HM]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Merckelbach, H., van den Hout, M. A. & van der Molen, G. M. (1989b) The phylogenetic origin of phobias: A review of the evidence. In: Fresh perspectives on anxiety disorders, ed. Emmelkamp, P. M. G.. Swets & Zeitlinger. [HM]Google Scholar
Nevin, J. A. (1983) Pavlovian contingencies and conditioned reinforcement. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6:711–12. [FT]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nicholaichuk, T. P., Quesnel, L. J. & Tait, R. W. (1982) Eysenek's theory of inculation: An empirical test. Behaviour Research and Therapy 20:329–38. [HM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pearce, J. M. & Hall, G. (1980) A model for Pavlovian learning: Variations in the effectiveness of conditioned but not of unconditioned stimuli. Psychological Review 87:532–52. [FT]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rachlin, H., Logue, A. W., Gibbon, J. & Frankel, M. (1986) Cognition and behavior in studies of choice. Psychological Review 93:33–45. [FT]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rachman, S. (1977) The conditioning theory of fear-acquisition: A critical examination. Behaviour Research and Therapy 15:375–87. [HM]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rashotte, M. E. (1981) Second-order autoshaping: Contributions to the research and theory of Pavlovian reinforcement by conditioned stimuli. In: Autoshaping and conditioning theory, ed. Locurto, C. M., Terrace, H. S. & Gibbon, J.. Academic Press. [FT]Google Scholar
Reiss, S. (1980) Pavlovian conditioning and human fear: An expectancy model. Behavior Therapy 11:380–96. [HM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rescorla, R. A. (1974) Effects of inflation of the unconditioned stimulus value following conditioning. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 86:101–6. [FT]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rescorla, R. A. (1988) Pavlovian conditioning: It's not what yon think it is. American Psychologist 43:151–60. [RBM]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ross, A. R. & Malmo, R. B. (1979) Cardiovascular responses to rewarding brain stimulation. Physiology and Behavior 22:1005–13. [RBM]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schnaitter, R. (1986) Behavior as a function of inner states and outer circumstances. In: Analysis and integration of behavioral units, ed. Thompson, T. & Zeiler, M. D.. Erlbaum. [FF]Google Scholar
Seligman, M. E. P. (1971) Phobias and preparedness. Behavior Therapy 2:307–20. [HM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seligman, M. E. P. & Maier, S. F. (1967) Failure to escape traumatic shock. journal of Experimental Psychology 74:1–9. [rJST]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shimp, C. P. (1989) Contemporary behaviorism versus the old behavioral straw man in Gardner's The mind's new science: A history of the cognitive revolution. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 51:163–71. [FT]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sideroff, S., Elster, A. J. & Schneiderman, N. (1972) Cardiovascular classical conditioning in rabbits (Oryctolagus cuniculus) using appetitive or aversive hypothalamic stimulation as the US. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 81:501–8. [RBM]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sideroff, S., Schneiderman, N. & Powell, D. A. (1971) Motivational properties of septal stimulation as the US in classical conditioning of heart rate in rabbits, Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 74:1–10. [RBM]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sidman, M. & Tailby, W. (1982) Conditional discrimination vs. matching to sample: An expansion of the testing paradigm. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 37:5–22. [FT]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Terrace, H. S. (1973) Classical conditioning. In: The study of behavior: Learning, motivation, emotion, and instinct, ed. Nevin, J. A. & Reynolds, G. S.. Scott, Foresman. [rJST]Google Scholar
Tonneau, F. (1990) From reflex to memory: Molar sequences in Pavlovian and instrumental conditioning. Psychological Record 40:587–607. [FT]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Turkkan, J. S. (1989) Classical conditioning: The new hegemony. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12:121–79. [rJST, HM, RBM, FT]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Dercar, D., Elster, A. & Schneiderman, N. (1970) Classical heart rate conditioning motivated by hypothalamic or septal US stimulation, Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 72:145–52. [RBM]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wagner, A. R. (1981) SOP: A model of automatic memory processing in animal behavior. In: Information processing in animals: Memory mechanisms, ed. Spear, N. E. & Miller, R. R.. Erlbaum. [FT]Google Scholar
Williams, B. A. (1983) Revising the principle of reinforcement. Behaviorism 11:63–88. [FT]Google Scholar
Williams, B. A. (1984) Stimulus control and associative learning. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 42:469–83. [FT]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wixted, J. T. (1989) Nonhuman short-term memory: A quantitative reanalysis of selected findings. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 52:409–26. [FT]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wixted, J. T. & McDowell, J. J. (1989) Contributions to the functional analysis of single-trial free recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 15:685–97. [FT]Google Scholar
Wolpe, J. (1989) The derailment of behavior therapy: A tale of conceptual misdirection. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry 20:3–15. [HM]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wolpe, J. & Rachman, S. (1960) Psychoanalytic evidence: A critique based on Freud's case of little Hans. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 131:135–47. [HM]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed