No CrossRef data available.
Article contents
Is there any difference between attribute- and object-based psychophysics?
Review products
Commentary onLockheadGregory R. (1992) Psychophysical scaling: Judgments of attributes or objects?BBS 15:543–601.
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 04 February 2010
Abstract
An abstract is not available for this content so a preview has been provided. Please use the Get access link above for information on how to access this content.
- Type
- Article Commentary
- Information
- Copyright
- Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1994
References
Algom, D. (1992) Perception, apperception and psychophysics. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15:558–59. [CP]Google Scholar
Allik, J. (1989) Is n unified psychophysical law realistic? Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12:267–68. [JA]Google Scholar
Allik, J. & Tuulmets, T. (1991) Occupancy model of perceived numerosity. Perception and Psychophysics 49:303–14. [JA]Google Scholar
Arcnd, L. E., Buehler, N. & Lockhead, G. R. (1971) Difference information in brightness perception. Perception and Psychophysics 9:367–70. [rGRL]Google Scholar
Bingham, G. P. (1991) The identification problem in visual event perception. Part I: Rate structures in optic flow and the degrees of freedom problem. Indiana University Cognitive Science Research Report No. 52. [SR]Google Scholar
Bingham, G. P. & Runeson, S. (1983) On describing what is perceived: Seeing “velocity” versus seeing “push” in moving objects. Paper presented at the Autumn Meeting of the International Society for Ecological Psychology, Trinity College, Hartford, CT. 10 1983. [SR]Google Scholar
Binghnm, G. P., Schmidt, R. C. & Rosenblum, D. (1989) Hefting for a maximum distance throw: A smart perceptual mechanism. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 15:507–28. [SR]Google Scholar
Booth, D. A. & Conner, M. T. (1991) Characterisation and measurement of influences on food acceptability by analysis of choice differences: Theory and practice. Food Quality and Preference 2:75–85. [DAB]Google Scholar
Booth, D. A. & Freeman, R. P. J. (1993) Discriminative measurementof feature integration in object recognition. Ada Psychologica 84:1–16. [DAB]Google Scholar
Booth, D. A., Freeman, R. P. J. & Lähteenmäki, L. (1991) Likings for complex foods and meals. Appetite 17:156. [DAB]Google Scholar
Booth, D. A. & Shepherd, R. (1988) Sensory influences on food acceptance the neglected approach to nutrition promotion. BNF Nutrition Bulletin 13 (1): 39–54. [DAB]Google Scholar
Boring, E. G. (1950) A history of experimental psychology. Appleton-Century Crofts. [rGRL, CP]Google Scholar
Brunswik, E. (1956) Perception and representative design of psychological experiments. University of California Press. [rGRL]Google Scholar
Brysbaert, M. (1992) Accounting for an old inconsistency in the psychophysics of Plateau and Delboeuf. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15:562–63. [CP]Google Scholar
Bunge, M. (1977) Treatise on basic philosophy. Ontology I: the furniture of the toorld. Reidel. [SR]Google Scholar
Burton, G. & Turvey, M. T. (1990) Perceiving thelengths of rods that are held but not wielded. Ecological Psychology 2:295–324. [SR]Google Scholar
Chase, S. & Heinemann, E. G. (1972) Choices based on redundant information: An analysis of two-dimensional stimulus control. journal of Experimental Psychology 92:161–75. [VS[Google Scholar
Conner, M. T., Haddon, A. V., Pickering, E. S. & Booth, D. A. (1988) Sweet tooth demonstrated: Individual differences in preference for both sweet foods and foods highly sweetened. Journal of Applied Psychology 73:275–80. [DAB]Google Scholar
Coren, S. (1992) Psychophysical scaling: Context and illusion. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15:563–64. [CP]Google Scholar
Crick, F. (1990) What mad pursuit. A personal view of a scientific discovery. Penguin. [JA]Google Scholar
Dzhafarov, E. N. (1992) Can brightness be related to luminance by a meaningful function? Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15:565–66. [JA]Google Scholar
Ennis, D. M., Palen, J. J. & Mullen, K. (1988) A multidimensional stochastic theory of similarity. Journal of Mathmatical Psychology 32:449– 65. [DAB]Google Scholar
Falmagne, F.-C. (1985) Elements of psychophysical theory. Oxford University Press. [DAB]Google Scholar
Geschcider, C. A. (1988) Psychophysical scaling. Annual Review of Psychology 39:169–200. [VS]Google Scholar
Gibson, J. J. (1966) The senses considered as perceptual systems. Houghton Mifflin. [rGRL, SR]Google Scholar
Gibson, J. J. (1979) The ecological approach to visual perception Houghton Mifflin. [rGRL, SR]Google Scholar
Gravetter, F. & Lockhead, C. (1973) Critical range as a frame of reference for stimulus judgment. Psychological Review 80:203–16. [rGRL]Google Scholar
Heinemann, E. G. & Chase, S. (1970) Conditional stimulus control. Journal of Experimental Psychology 84:187–97. [VS]Google Scholar
Helson, H. (1964) Adaption-level theory: An experimental and systematic approach to behavior. Harper & Row. [rGRL]Google Scholar
Herrnstein, R. J. & Boring, E. G., eds. (1966) A source book in thehistory of psychology. Harvard University Press. [CP]Google Scholar
Hinson, J. & Lockhead, G. R. (1986) Range effects in successive discrimination procedures. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 12:270–76. [rGRL, VS]Google Scholar
Hofer, G. & Sarris, V. (1992) Test–series effects in psychophysical responses by chickens and humans. Paper presented to the 25th International Congress of Psychology, Brussels, Belgium, 07 [VS]Google Scholar
Holland, M. & Lockhead, G. (1968) Sequential effects in absolute judgments of loudness. Perception and Psychophysics. 3:409–14. [rGRL]Google Scholar
Hutchinson, J. W. & Lockhead, G. R. (1977) Similarity as distance: A structural principle for semantic memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory 3:660–78. [rGRL]Google Scholar
Indow, T. & Ida, M. (1977) Scaling of dot numerosity. Perception and Psychophysics 22:265–76. [JA]Google Scholar
Kanisza, G. (1974) Contours without gradients or cognitive contours? Italian Journal of Psychology 1(1) 92–112. [rGRL]Google Scholar
King, M. C. & Lockhead, G. (1981) Response scales and sequential effects in judgment. Perception and Psychophysics 30:599–603. [rGRL]Google Scholar
Knibestöl, M. & Vallbo, Å. B. (1980) Intensity of sensation related to activity of slowly adapting mechanoreceptive units in the human hand. Journal of Physiology 300:251–67. [rGRL, GSW]Google Scholar
Krueger, L. E. (1984) Perceived numerosity: A comparison of magnitude production, magnitude estimation, and discrimination judgments. Perception and Psychophysics 35:536–42. [JA]Google Scholar
Krueger, L. E. (1989) Reconciling Fechncr and Stevens: Toward a unified psychophysical law. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12:251–66. [DAB, GSW]Google Scholar
Krueger, L. E. (1992) Will the real stimulus please step forward? Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15:570–72. [SR]Google Scholar
Kugler, P. N., Kelso, J. A. S. & Turvey, M. T. (1982) On the control and coordination of naturally developing systems. The development of movement control and co–ordination, ed. Kelso, J. A. S. & Clark, J. E.. Wiley. [SR]Google Scholar
Laming, D.(1989) Experimental evidence for Fechner's and Stevens's laws. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12:277–81. [rGRL]Google Scholar
Land, E. H. (1959) Color vision and the natural image. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 45:115–29, 536–644. [rGRL]Google Scholar
Land, E. H. (1977) The retinex theory of color vision. Scientific American, 1977; reprinted in: The Perceptual World, ed. I. Rock. Freeman, 1990. [rGRL]Google Scholar
Lawless, H. T. (1991) Bridging the gap between sensory science and product evaluation. In: Sensory science theory and application in foods, ed. Lawless, H. T. and Klein, B. P.. Marcel Dekker. [DAB]Google Scholar
Lockhead, G. R. (1966) Effects of dimensional redundancy on visual discrimination. Journal of Experimental Psychology 72:95–104.Google Scholar
Lockhead, G. R. (1972) Processing dimensional stimuli: A note. Psychological Review 79:410–19. [rGRL]Google Scholar
Lockhead, G. R. (1992a) Psychophysical scaling: Judgments of attributes or objects? Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15:543–601. [rGRL, JA, DAB, CP, SR, VS, GSW]Google Scholar
Lockhead, G. R. (1992b) On identifying things: A case for context. In: Percepts, concepts, and categories: The representation and processing of information, ed. Burns, B., Advances in Psychology. North Holland. [rGRL]Google Scholar
Lockhead, G. R. (1993) A parallel view of the history of psychophysics. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16:154–55. [rGRL]Google Scholar
Lockhead, G. R. & King, M. C. (1977) Classifying integral stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 3:436–43. [rGRL]Google Scholar
Monahan, J. S. (1992) Attributes or objects: A paradigm shift in psychophysics. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15:577. [CP]Google Scholar
Monahan, J. S. & Lockhead, G. R. (1977) Identification of integral stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 106:94–110. [rGRL]Google Scholar
Morgan, M. J., Hole, G. J. & Glennerster, A. (1990) Biases and sensitivities in geometrical illusion. Vision Research 30:1793–1810. [JA]Google Scholar
Müller, G. E. (1896): Zur Psychophysik der Gesichtesempfindungen. Zeitschrift für Psychologic und Physiologic der Sinnesorgane 10:1–82. [CP]Google Scholar
Murray, D. J. (1993) A perspective for viewing the history of psychophysics. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16:115–37. [rGRL]Google Scholar
Niall, K. K. (1992) The evident object of inquiry. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15:578. [CP]Google Scholar
Nosofsky, R. M. (1988) On exemplar–based exemplar representations: Reply to Ennis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 117:412–14. [DAB]Google Scholar
Parducci, A. (1983) Category ratings and the relational character of judgment. In: Modern issues in perception, ed. Geissler, H.-G., Buffart, H. F. J. M., Leeuwenberg, E. L. J. & Sarris, V.. North-Holland. [VS]Google Scholar
Poulton, E. C. (1989) Uncertain size of exponent when judging without familiar units. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12:286–88. [rGRL]Google Scholar
Ramachandran, V. S. (1990) Visual perception in people and machines. In: Al and the eye, ed. Blake, A. & Troscianko, T.. Wiley. [rGRL, JA]Google Scholar
Richardson, N. J. & Booth, D. A. (1993) Multiple physical patterns in judgments of the creamy texture of milks and creams. Ada Psychologica 84:93–101. [DAB]Google Scholar
Rosch, E. (1975) Cognitive representations of semantic categories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 104:192–233. [VS]Google Scholar
Rosen, R. (1978) fundamentals of measurement and representation of natural systems. North–Holland. [SR]Google Scholar
Runeson, S. (1974) Constant velocity – not perceived as such. Psychological Research 37:3–23. [SR]Google Scholar
Runeson, S. (1975) Visual prediction of collision with natural and nonnatural motion functions. Perception and Psychophysics 18:261–66. [SR]Google Scholar
Runeson, S. (1977) On the possibility of “smart” perceptual mechanisms. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology 18:172–79. [JA, SR]Google Scholar
Runeson, S. & Vedeler, D. (1993) The indispensability of precollision kinematics in the visual perception of relative mass. Perception and Psychophysics 53:617–32. [rGRL, SR]Google Scholar
Sarris, V. (1975) Wahrnehmung und Urteil: Bezugssystemeffekte in der Psychophysik [Perception and judgment: Frame–of–reference effects in psychophysics]. 2d ed. Hogrefe. [VS]Google Scholar
Sarris, V. (1990) Contextual effects in animal psychophysics: A comparative analysis of the chicken's perceptual relativity. European Bulletin of Cognitive Psychology 10:475–89. [VS]Google Scholar
Sarris, V., Hofer, G. & Zoeke, B. (1990) The chicken's visual psychophysics: Two-dimensional effects. In: Proceedings of the sixth annual meeting of the International Society for Psychophysics, ed. Müller, F.. Würzburg, Germany. [VS]Google Scholar
Sarris, V. & Parducci, A. (1978) Multiple anchoring of category rating scales. Perception & Psychophysics 24:35–39. [VS]Google Scholar
Sarris, V. & Zoeke, B. (1985) Tests of a quantitative frame-of-referencc model: Practice effects in psychophysical judgments with different age groups. In: Cognition, information processing, and motivation, ed. d'Ydewalle, G.. North-Holland. [VS]Google Scholar
Shepard, R. N. (1991) Integrality versus separability of stimulus dimensions: Evolution of the distinction and a proposed theoretical basis. In: The perception of structure, ed. Lockhead, G. R. & Pomerantz., J. American Psychological Association. [rGRL]Google Scholar
Solomon, H. Y. & Turvey, M. T. (1988) Haptically perceiving the distances reachable with hand–held objects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 14:404–27. [SR]Google Scholar
Stebbins, W. C. (1990) Perception in animal behavior. In: Comparative perception, vol. I, ed. Berkeley, M. A. & Stebbins, W. C.. Wiley. [VS]Google Scholar
Stevens, S. S. (1975) Psychophysics: Introduction to its perceptual, neural, and social prospects. Wiley. [JA]Google Scholar
Teghtsoonian, R. (1971) On the exponents in Stevens's law and the constants in Ekman's law. Psychological Review 78:71–80. [rGRL]Google Scholar
Thomas, D. R. (1974) The role of adaptation-level in stimulus generalization. In: The psychology of learning and motivation, vol. 8, ed. Bower, G. H.. Academic Press. [VS]Google Scholar
Thomas, D. R., Lusky, M. & Morrison, S. (1992) Go/no–go choice: A comparison of generalization functions and franje of reference effects in two training paradigms. Perception and Psychophysics 51:529–40. [VS]Google Scholar
Thomas, D. R., Mood, K., Morrison, S. & Wiertelak, E. (1991) Peak shift revisited: A test of alternative interpretations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 17:130–40. [VS]Google Scholar
Turvey, M. T. (1986) Intentionality: A problem of multiple reference frames, specificational information, and extraordinary boundary conditions on natural law. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9:153–55. [SR]Google Scholar
Turvey, M. T. (1992) Affordances and prospective control: An outline of the ontology. Ecological Psychology 4:173–87. [SR]Google Scholar
Turvey, M. T. & Carello, C. (1986) The ecological approach to perceivingacting: A pictorial essay. Ada Psychologica 63:133–55. [SR]Google Scholar
van Brakel, J. (1992) Ceteris paribus laws. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15:584–85. [GSW]Google Scholar
Ward, L. M. & Lockhead, G. R. (1971) Response system processes in absolute judgment. Perception and Psychophysics 9:73–78. [rGRL]Google Scholar
Wasserman, G. S. (1991) Neural and behavioral assessments of sensory quantity. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14:192–93. [rGRL, GSW]Google Scholar
Wundt, W. (1902) Grundzüge der physiologischen Psychologic, vol. I, 5th edition. Engelmann. [CP]Google Scholar
Zoeke, B., Sarris, V. & Hofer, G. (1988) Psychophysical context effects in chickens (hubbards). International Journal of Comparative Psychology 1:167–78. [VS]Google Scholar
Zoeke, B. Sarris, V. (1989) Comparative psychophysics: Some contextual effects in birds and humans. International Journal of Comparative Psychology 2:151–63. [VS]Google Scholar