We use cookies to distinguish you from other users and to provide you with a better experience on our websites. Close this message to accept cookies or find out how to manage your cookie settings.
An abstract is not available for this content so a preview has been provided. Please use the Get access link above for information on how to access this content.
Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)
References
Beck, B. (1980) Animal tool behavior. Garland.Google Scholar
Beck, C. M. H. & Barton, R. L. (1972) Deviation and laterally of hand preference in monkeys. Cortex8:339–63.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boesch, C. & Boeseh, H. (1981) Sex differences in the use of natural hammers by wild chimpanzees: A preliminary report. Journal of Human Evolution10:585–93.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brésard, B. & Bresson, F. (1987) Reaching or manipulation. Behavioral and Brain Sciences10:265–66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chapman, J. P., Chapman, J. J. & Allen, J. J. (1987) The measurement of foot preference. Neuropsychologia25:579–84.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cicchetti, D. V. (1987). On viewing the evidence for primate handedness: Some biostatistical considerations. Behavioral and Brain Sciences10:268–69.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Denenberg, V. H. & Yutzey, D. A. (1985) Hemispheric laterality, behavioral asymmetry, and the effects of early experience in rats. In: Cerebral lateralization in nonhuman species, ed. Glick, S. D.. Academic Press.Google Scholar
Dhall, U. & Singh, I. (1977) Anatomical evidence of one-sided forelimb dominance in the rhesus monkey. Anatomischer Anzeiger141:420–25.Google ScholarPubMed
Fagot, J. & Vauclair, J. (1988a) Handedness and manual specialization in the baboon. Neuropsychologia26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fagot, J. & Vauclair, J. (1988b) Handedness and bimanual coordination in the lowland gorilla. Brain Behavior and Evolution29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Falk, D., Pyne, L., Helmkamp, C. & De Rousseau, C. (1988) Skeletal asymmetry in forelimb of Macaca mulatta. American Journal of Physical Anthropology70.Google Scholar
Glick, S. D., Ross, D. A. & Hough, L. B. (1982) Lateral asymmetry of neurotransmitters in the human brain. Brain Research234:53–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glick, S. D. & Shapiro, R. M. (1985) Functional and neurochemical mechanisms of cerebral lateralization in rats. In: Cerebral lateralization in nonhuman species, ed. Glick, S. D.. Academic Press.Google Scholar
Healey, J. M., Liederman, J. & Geschwind, N. (1986) Handedness is not a unidimensional trait. Cortex22:33–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heilman, K. M., Coyle, J. M., Gonyea, E. F. & Geschwind, N. (1973) Apraxia and agraphia in a left-hander. Brain96:21–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hopkins, W. D. & Morris, R. D. (1988) Laterality for visual-processing in two language-trained chimpanzees. Behavioral Neuroscience102.Google Scholar
Hopkins, W. D., Washburn, D. A. & Rumbaugh, D. M. (1988) A note on hand use in the manipulation of joysticks by two rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) and three chimpanzees (Pan. troglodytes). Journal of Comparative Psychology102.Google Scholar
King, J. E. & Landau, V. (in preparation) Manual preferences in varieties of reaching in squirrel monkeys. In: New evidence of primate behavioral asymmetries, ed. Ward, J..Google Scholar
Kooistra, C. A. & Heilman, K. M. (1988) Motor dominance and lateral asymmetry of the globus pallidus. Neurology38:388–90.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krmpotic, J. (1959) Donees anatomiques et histologiques relatives aux effecteurs laryngo-pharyngo-buccaux. Revue de Laryngologie (Bordeaux) 11:829–48.Google Scholar
Larsen, C. S. (1985) Dental modifications and tool use in the Western Great Basin. American Journal of Physical Anthropology67:393–402.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lenneberg, E. H. (1967) Biological foundations of language. Wiley.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacNeilage, P. F. (1987) The evolution of hemispheric specialization for manual function and language. In: Higher brain functions: Recent explorations of the brain's emergent properties, ed. Wise, S.. Wiley.Google Scholar
MacNeilage, P. F. (in preparation) The “Postural Origins” theory of primate neurobiological asymmetries. In: Biobehavioral foundations of language development, ed. Krasnegor, N..Google Scholar
McKain, K. S., Studdert-Kennedy, M. G., Spieker, S. & Stern, D. (1983) Infant intermodal speech perception is a left hemisphere function. Science219:1347–49.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mayr, E. (1982) The growth of biological thought. Belknap Press.Google Scholar
Peters, M. (1988) Footedness: Asymmetries in foot performance and skill and neuropsychological assessment of foot movement. Psychological Bulletin103:179–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Post, D. G., G., Hausfater & McCuskey, S. A. (1980) Feeding behavior of yellow baboons (Papio cynocephalus): Relationship to age, gender, and dominance rank. Folia Primatologia34:170–95.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schaller, G. B. (1963) The mountain gorilla. Chicago University Press.Google Scholar
Searleman, A. (1980) Subject variables and cerebral organization for language. Cortex16:239–54.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Robinson, T. E., Becker, J. B., Camp, D. M. & Mansour, A. (1985) Variation in the pattern of behavioral and brain asymmetries due to sex differences. In: Cerebral lateralization in nonhuman species, ed. Click, S. D.. Wiley.Google Scholar
Terborgh, J. (1983) Five new world primates. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Trevarthen, C. (1978) Manipulative strategies of baboons and the origins of cerebral asymmetries. In: Asymmetrical function of the brain, ed. Kinsbourne, M. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar