Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-7cvxr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T19:16:58.040Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Motor-sensory feedback and geometry of visual space: an attempted replication

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  19 May 2011

John Gyr
Affiliation:
Mental Health Research Institute, Department of Psychiatry, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, Mich. 48109
Richmond Willey
Affiliation:
Mental Health Research Institute, Department of Psychiatry, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, Mich. 48109
Adele Henry
Affiliation:
Mental Health Research Institute, Department of Psychiatry, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, Mich. 48109

Abstract

This study involved a partial replication of an experiment performed by Held & Rekosh (1963) on the relation between prism-induced changes in visuomotor feedback and shifts in the perception of straightness. The Held & Rekosh study is important because its methodology makes it possible to clearly separate changes in perception due to visuomotor feedback from those due to other perceptual factors. Thus it bears unambiguously on the hypothesis that the neurological processing of sensory input is dependent on the organization of motor activity. The present paper consists of a brief review of theory and experiment on sensorimotor phenomena in perception, followed by a detailed consideration of the Held & Rekosh study and the present replication. For greater effectiveness, Held & Rekosh should have used base-up and base-down prism orientations rather than the base-left and base-right orientations that they employed, but in any case our replication yielded negative results for both base-up and base-right. It is concluded that, given the minimally effective prism orientation and the attentional loads characterizing the Held & Rekosh study, there is need for further experimentation with these two variables. A more drastic suggestion is that there may be a need to confront anew the evidence for sensorimotor processes in perception.

Type
Target Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1979

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Bischof, N. & Kramer, E.Untersuchungen und Ueberlegungen zur Richtungswahrnehmung bei willkurlichen sakkadischen Augenbewegungen. Psychologische Forschung. 32: 185218, 1968.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Epstein, W.Varieties of Perceptual Learning. McGraw-Hill, New York, 1967.Google Scholar
Festinger, L., Burnham, C. A., Ono, H., & Bamber, D.Efference and the conscious experience of perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology Monographs. 74(4, No. 637), 1967.Google Scholar
Freedman, S. J. (ed.) The Neuropsychology of Spatially Oriented Behavior. Dorsey, Homewood, Ill., 1968.Google Scholar
Gibson, E. J.Principles of Perceptual Learning and Development. Appleton-Century-Crofts, New York, 1969.Google Scholar
Gibson, J. J.Adaptation, after-effect and contrast in the perception of curved lines. Journal of Experimental Psychology. 16: 131, 1933.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
The Senses Considered as Perceptual Systems. Houghton Mifflin, Boston, 1966.Google Scholar
Gourlay, K., Gyr, J. W., Walters, S., & Willey, R.Instrumentation designed to simulate the effects of prisms used in studies of visual arrangement. Behavior Research Methods and Instrumentation. 7: 294300, 1975.Google Scholar
Gyr, J. W.is a theory of direct visual perception adequate? Psychological Bulletin. 77: 246261, 1972.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
The relationship between motor and visual-sensory processes in perception. In: Cruickschank, W. M. & Hallahan, D. P. (eds.) Perceptual Learning Disabilities in Children. Vol. 2. Syracuse University Press, Syracuse, 1975.Google Scholar
Gyr, J. W. & Willey, R.The effects of efference to the arm on visual adaptation to curvature: a replication. Psychonomic Science. 21: 8991, 1970.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harris, C. S.Perceptual adaptation to inverted, reversed and displaced vision. Psychological Review. 72: 419444, 1965.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hein, A. & Held, R. A neural model for labile sensori-motor coordinations. In: Biological Prototypes and Synthetic Systems. Vol. 1. Plenum Press, pp. 7174, 1962.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Held, R.Exposure-history as a factor in maintaining stability of perception coordination. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease. 132: 2632, 1961.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
The role of movement in the origin and maintenance of visual perception. In: Procedures XVII International Congress of Psychology. North Holland, Amsterdam, 1964.Google Scholar
Held, R. & Hein, A.Movement-produced stimulation in the development of visually guided behavior. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology. 56: 872876, 1963.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Held, R. & Rekosh, J.Motor-sensory feedback and geometry of visual space. Science. 141: 722723, 1963.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hochberg, J. Space and Movement. In: Kling, J. W. and Riggs, L. A. (eds.) Woodworth and Schlosberg's Experimental Psychology, Holt, Rinehart and Winston, New York; 1971.Google Scholar
Howard, I. P., Craske, B., and Templeton, W. B.Visuo-motor adaptation to discordant exafferent stimulation. Journal of Experimental Psychology. 70: 189191, 1965.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Howard, I. P. & Templeton, W. B.Human Spatial Orientation. John Wiley, New York, 1966.Google Scholar
Konorski, J.Integrative Activity of the Brain. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, 1967.Google Scholar
Meyers, B.Discrimination of visual movement in perceptually deprived cats. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology. 57: 152153, 1964.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mott, F. W. & Sherrington, C. S.Experiments upon the influence of sensory nerves upon movement and nutrition of the limbs. Proceedings of the Royal Society. 57: 481488, 1895.Google Scholar
Riesen, A. H. & Aarons, L.Visual movement and intensity discrimination in cats after early deprivation of pattern vision. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology. 52: 142149, 1959.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rock, I.The Nature of Perceptual Adaptation. Basic Books, New York, 1966.Google Scholar
Sherrington, C. S.Quantitative management of contraction in lowest level coordination. Brain. 54: 128, 1931.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sirigatti, S. The effects of saccadic recalibration upon the visual perception of extent. Dissertation, Xerox University Microfilms, (752325), Ann Arbor, 1974.Google Scholar
Slotnick, R. S.Adaptation to curvature distortion. Journal of Experimental Psychology. 81: 441448, 1969.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sperry, R. W.Neural basis of the spontaneous optokinetic response produced by visual inversion. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology. 43: 482489, 1950.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taub, E. Prism compensation as a learning phenomenon: a phylogenetic perspective. pp. 77109. In: Freedman (1968).Google Scholar
Taub, E. & Berman, A. J. Avoidance conditioning in the absence of sensory feedback. pp. 173–93. In: Freedman (1968).Google Scholar
Teuber, H. L. The riddle of frontal lobe function in man. In: Warren, J. M. & Akert, K. (eds.) The Frontal Granular Cortex and Behavior. McGraw-Hill, New York, 1964.Google Scholar
Victor, J. The role of movement in perceptual adaptation to curvature. Dissertation, Xerox University Microfilms, (pp 6877, 173) Ann Arbor, 1968.Google Scholar
Von Holst, E.Relations between the central nervous system and the peripheral organs. British Journal of Animal Behavior. 2: 8994, 1954.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Von Holst, E. & Mittelstaedt, H.Das Reafferenz-Prinzip. Die Naturwissen-schaften. 20: 464476, 1950.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walk, R. D. & Gibson, E. J.A comparative and analytical study of visual depth perception. Psychological Monographs. 75(15, No. 519). 1961.Google Scholar
Welch, R. B.Research on adaptation to rearranged vision, 1966–1974. Perception. 3: 367393, 1974.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed