No CrossRef data available.
Article contents
Neural reuse implies distributed coding
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 22 October 2010
Abstract
Both distributed coding, with its implication of neural reuse, and more specialized function have been recognized since the beginning of brain science. A controversy over imageless thought threw introspection into disrepute as a scientific method, making more objective methods dominate. It is known in information science that one element, such as a bit in a computer, can participate in coding many independent states; in this commentary, an example is given.
- Type
- Open Peer Commentary
- Information
- Copyright
- Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2010
References
Broca, P. (1861) Remarques sur le siège de la faculté du langage articulé, suivies d'une observation d'aphémie (perte de la parole). Bulletin de la Société Anatomique
6:330–57.Google Scholar
Fritsch, G. & Hitzig, E. (1870/1960) On the electrical excitability of the cerebrum. In: Brain and behaviour: Vol. 2. Perception and action, ed. Pribram, K. H.. Penguin. (Original work published in 1870).Google Scholar
Glenberg, A. M. & Kaschak, M. P. (2002) Grounding language in action. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review
9:558–65.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hurley, S. L. (2008) The shared circuits model (SCM): How control, mirroring, and simulation can enable imitation, deliberation, and mindreading. Behavioral and Brain Sciences
31(1):1–58.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stroop, J. (1935) Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology
18:643–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weiskopf, D. (2007) Concept empiricism and the vehicles of thought. The Journal of Consciousness Studies
14:156–83.Google Scholar