Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-dzt6s Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-27T22:39:10.715Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Stimulating understanding: Making the example fit the question

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 February 2010

Thomas Edelson
Affiliation:
10114 Fleming Avenue, Bethesda, Md. 20014

Abstract

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Continuing Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1982

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Chomsky, N. (1980) Rules and representations. Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 3:161. [HR]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cullingford, R. E. (1978) Script application: Computer understanding of newspaper stories. Yale Computer Science Department Research Report 116. [DM]Google Scholar
Cummins, R. (1977) Programs in the explanation of behavior. Philosophy of Science 44:269–87. [WES]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dennett, D. (1980) The milk of human intentionality. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3:428–30. [TE]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fodor, J. A. (1981) The mind-body problem. Scientific American 244:114–23. [WES]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haugeland, J. (1980) Programs, causal powers, and intentionality. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3:432–33. [JS, SFS, JRS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kolers, P. A. (1980) Confusions of symbolization in mental representation. Paper presented at the meeting of the Philosophy of Science Association, University of Toronto, October. [WES]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kolers, P. A. & Smythe, W. E. (1979) Images, symbols, and skills. Canadian Journal of Psychology 33:158–84. [WES]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lycan, W. (1980) The functionalist reply (Ohio State). Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3:434–35. [SFS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Minsky, M. (1968) Matter, mind, and models. In: Semantic information processing, ed. Minsky, M.. Cambridge: MIT Press [DM]Google Scholar
Newell, A. (1980) Physical symbol systems. Cognitive Science 4:135–83. [WES]Google Scholar
Pylyshyn, Z. W. (1980) Computation and cognition: Issues in the foundations of cognitive science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3:111–69. [WES]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Searle, J. R. (1979) What is an intentional state? Mind 88:7492. [WES]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Searle, J. R. (1980a) Intrinsic intentionality. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3:450–57. [TE, HR, JS, SFS]Google Scholar
Searle, J. R. (1980b) Minds, brains, and programs. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3:417–50. [TE, DM, JS, SFS, WES]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sloman, A. & Croucher, M.How to turn an information processor into an understander. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3:447–48. [JS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smythe, W. E. (1980a) Simulation games. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3:448–49. [JRS, WES]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smythe, W. E. (1980b) What kind of symbol is a mental image? An inquiry concerning imagery as a personal symbol system. Paper presented at the 4th American Imagery Conference, San Francisco. [WES]Google Scholar
Smythe, W. E. (1981) Personal symbol systems: A target for cognitive psychology. Paper presented at the 7th annual meeting of the Society for Philosophy and Psychology, University of Chicago, April. [WES]Google Scholar
Stich, S. P. (1978) Autonomous psychology and the belief-desire thesis. Monist 6:573–91. [WES]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilks, Y. (1975) Putnam and Clarke and mind and body. British Journal of the Philosophy of Science 26:213–25. [YW]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wittgenstein, L. (1958) Philosophical investigations. Oxford: Blackwell. [YW]Google Scholar