No CrossRef data available.
Article contents
Where are nature's joints? Finding the mechanisms underlying categorization
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 29 June 2010
Abstract
Machery argues that concepts are too heterogeneous to be a natural kind. I argue that the book does not go far enough. Theories of concepts assume that the task of categorizing warrants a unique set of cognitive constructs. Instead, cognitive science must look across tasks to find a fundamental set of cognitive mechanisms.
- Type
- Open Peer Commentary
- Information
- Copyright
- Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2010
References
Ashby, F. G., Alfonso-Reese, L. A., Turken, A. U. & Waldron, E. M. (1998) A neuropsychological theory of multiple systems in category learning. Psychological Review
105(3):442–81.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chin-Parker, S. & Ross, B. H. (2002) The effect of category learning on sensitivity to within-category correlations. Memory and Cognition
30(3):353–62.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garrod, S. & Anderson, A. (1987) Saying what you mean in dialogue: A study in conceptual and semantic co-ordination. Cognition
27:181–218.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garrod, S. & Doherty, G. (1994) Conversation, co-ordination and convention: An empirical investigation of how groups establish linguistic conventions. Cognition
53:181–215.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Knowlton, B. J., Squire, L. R. & Gluck, M. A. (1994) Probabilistic classification learning in amnesia. Learning and Memory
1:106–20.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Logan, G. D. (1988) Toward an instance theory of automaticity. Psychological Review
95:492–527.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Logan, G. D. (2002) An instance theory of attention and memory. Psychological Review
109(2):376–400.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maddox, W. T. & Ashby, F. G. (2004) Dissociating explicit and procedure-learning based systems of perceptual category learning. Behavioral Processes
66(3):309–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Malt, B. C., Sloman, S. A. & Gennari, S. P. (2003) Universality and language specificity in object naming. Journal of Memory and Language
49:20–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Markman, A. B. & Dietrich, E. (2000) In defense of representation. Cognitive Psychology
40(2):138–71.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Markman, A. B. & Makin, V. S. (1998) Referential communication and category acquisition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General
127(4):331–54.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Markman, A. B. & Ross, B. (2003) Category use and category learning. Psychological Bulletin
129:592–613.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morris, C. D., Bransford, J. D. & Franks, J. J. (1977) Levels of processing versus transfer appropriate processing. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior
16:519–33.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yamauchi, T., Love, B. C. & Markman, A. B. (2002) Learning nonlinearly separable categories by inference and classification. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition
28(3):585–93.Google ScholarPubMed
Yamauchi, T. & Markman, A. B. (1998) Category learning by inference and classification. Journal of Memory and Language
39(1):124–48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar