Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-lj6df Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-10T19:26:50.562Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The Role of Disgust and Threat in Contamination-Related Obsessive–Compulsive Disorder

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 February 2021

Leanne Mulheron
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, The University of Sydney, Sydney, New South Wales, Australia
Mairwen K. Jones*
Affiliation:
Sydney School of Health Sciences, The University of Sydney, Sydney, New South Wales, Australia
*
*Corresponding author: Mairwen K. Jones, Senior Lecturer in Psychology, Sydney School of Health Sciences, Faculty of Medicine and Health, University of Sydney, Sydney, NSW, Australia. Email: mairwen.jones@sydney.edu.au
Get access

Abstract

Theoretical models suggest that the emotion disgust or threat overestimates are important in the aetiology and maintenance of contamination-based obsessive–compulsive disorder. In the current study, both threat and disgust were manipulated and 115 non-clinical participants (mean age 20.46 years, 94 females) were randomly allocated to one of four conditions: high-disgust/low-threat (n = 29), high-disgust/high-threat (n = 29), low-disgust/low-threat (n = 27), and low-disgust/high-threat (n = 30). Participants completed a hierarchical Behavioural Avoidance Task (BAT). Those in the high-threat and high-disgust conditions completed less BAT steps and showed more latency to begin each step than those in the low-threat and low-disgust conditions. A significant interaction effect was observed for the high-disgust/high-threat condition as significantly more task avoidance was found. However, handwashing duration was not significantly different between the high and low-disgust conditions or the high and low-threat conditions. The overall low mean washing duration of 30 s possibly due to the testing conditions and/or the ethnic heterogeneity of the sample may account for these results. There were also no significant differences in the level of anxiety for participants in the high-threat compared with the low-threat conditions. It is possible that anxiety remained relatively low across conditions as a result of the graduated BAT. Future research and theoretical and clinical implications are discussed.

Type
Standard Paper
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abramowitz, J, Lackey, G and Wheaton, M (2009). Obsessive–compulsive symptoms: The contribution of obsessional beliefs and experiential avoidance. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 23, 160166. doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2008.06.003.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Health Disorders (5th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
Athey, A, Elias, J, Crosby, J, Jenike, M, Pope, H and Hudson, J (2015). Reduced disgust propensity is associated with improvement in contamination/washing symptoms in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Journal of Obsessive Compulsive Related Disorders, 4, 2024. doi:10.1016/j.jocrd.2014.11.001.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bhikram, T, Abi-Jaoude, E and Sandor, P (2017). OCD: Obsessive-compulsive … disgust? The role of disgust in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Journal of Psychiatry & Neuroscience, 42, 300306. doi:10.1503/jpn.160079.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cavanagh, K and Davey, G (2000). UCS expectancy biases in spider phobics: Underestimation of aversive consequences following fear-irrelevant stimuli. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 38, 641651. doi:10.1016/S0005-7967(99)00077-7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cisler, J and Koster, E (2010). Mechanisms of attentional biases towards threat in anxiety disorders: An integrative review. Clinical Psychology Review, 30, 203216. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2009.11.003.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cisler, J, Brady, RE, Olatunji, B and Lohr, J (2010). Disgust and obsessive beliefs in contamination-related OCD. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 34, 439448. doi:10.1007/s10608-009-9253-y.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cougle, Jesse R, Wolitzky-Taylor, Kate B., Lee, Han-Joo and Telch, Michael J (2007) Mechanisms of change in ERP treatment of compulsive hand washing: Does primary threat make a difference?. Behaviour Research and Therapy 45, 14491459. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2006.12.001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deacon, B and Olatunji, B (2007). Specificity of disgust sensitivity in the prediction of behavioral avoidance in contamination fear. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45, 21102120.10.1016/j.brat.2007.03.008CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Silva, P and Rachman, S (1992). Obsessive compulsive disorders. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Fan, Q and Olatunji, B (2013). Individual differences in disgust sensitivity and health-related avoidance: Examination of specific associations. Personal Individual Differences, 55, 454458. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2013.04.007.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Faul, F and Erdfelder, E (1992). GPOWER: A priori-, post hoc-, and compromise power analyses for MS-DOS [computer program]. Bonn, Germany: Bonn University.Google Scholar
Field, A (2013). Discovering statistics using IBM SPSS statistics: And sex and drugs and rock “n” roll (4th ed.). CA: Cram101 Incorporated.Google Scholar
Foa, E, Huppert, J, Leiberg, S, Langner, R, Kichic, R, Hajcak, G and Salkovskis, P (2002). The Obsessive-Compulsive Inventory: Development and validation of a short version. Psychological Assessment, 14, 485496.10.1037/1040-3590.14.4.485CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goetz, A, Lee, H, Cougle, J and Turkel, J (2013). Disgust propensity and sensitivity: Differential relationships with obsessive-compulsive symptoms and behavioral approach task performance. Journal of Obsessive-Compulsive and Related Disorders, 2, 412419. doi:10.1016/j.jocrd.2013.07.006.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Govender, S, Drummond, LM and Menzies, RG (2006). Danger ideation reduction therapy for the treatment of severe, chronic and resistant obsessive-compulsive disorder. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 34, 477480.10.1017/S1352465806003018CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haberkamp, A, Schmidt, F, Hansmeier, J and Glombiewski, JA (2019). Contamination fear in subclinical obsessive-compulsive disorder: A further proof for no preferential processing of disorder-related stimuli. Journal of Obsessive-Compulsive and Related Disorders, 21, 2636. doi:10.1016/j.jocrd.2018.11.004.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hambridge, J and Loewenthal, M (2003). Treating obsessive compulsive disorder: A new role for infectious diseases physicians? International Journal of Infectious Diseases, 7, 152155.10.1016/S1201-9712(03)90012-9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jones, MK and Krochmalik, A (2003). Obsessive-compulsive washing. In Menzies, RG and de Silva, P (Eds.), Obsessive compulsive disorder: Theory, research, and treatment (pp. 121137). Chichester, UK: Wiley.Google Scholar
Jones, MK and Menzies, RG (1997a). The cognitive mediation of obsessive-compulsive handwashing. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 35, 843850. doi:10.1016/S0005-7967(97)00035-1.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jones, MK and Menzies, RG (1997b). Danger ideation reduction therapy (DIRT): Preliminary findings with three obsessive-compulsive washers. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 35, 955960.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jones, MK and Menzies, RG (1998a). The role of perceived danger in the mediation of obsessive-compulsive washing. Depression and Anxiety, 8, 121125.10.1002/(SICI)1520-6394(1998)8:3<121::AID-DA4>3.0.CO;2-P3.0.CO;2-P>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jones, MK and Menzies, RG (1998b). Danger ideation reduction therapy (DIRT) for obsessive-compulsive washers. A controlled trial. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 36, 959970.10.1016/S0005-7967(98)00057-6CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Knowles, KA, Jessup, SC and Olatunji, BO (2018). Disgust in anxiety and obsessive-compulsive disorders: Recent findings and future directions. Current Psychiatry Reports, 20, 110. doi:10.1007/s11920-018-0936.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krochmalik, A, Jones, MK and Menzies, RG (2001). Danger ideation reduction therapy (DIRT) for treatment-resistant compulsive washing. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 39, 897912.10.1016/S0005-7967(00)00063-2CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krochmalik, A, Jones, MK, Menzies, RG and Kirkby, K (2004). The superiority of danger ideation reduction therapy (DIRT) over exposure and response prevention (ERP) in treating compulsive washing. Behaviour Change, 21, 251268.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Li, Y, Marques, L, Hinton, D, Wang, Y and Xiao, Z (2009). Symptom dimensions in Chinese patients with obsessive compulsive disorder. CNS Neuroscience & Therapeutics, 15, 276282. doi:10.1111/j.1755-5949.2009.00099.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lovibond, S and Lovibond, P (1995). Manual for the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (2nd ed.). Sydney, Australia: Psychology Foundation of Australia.Google Scholar
Ludvik, D, Boschen, M and Neumann, D (2015). Effective behavioural strategies for reducing disgust in contamination-related OCD: A review. Clinical Psychology Review, 42, 116129. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2015.07.001.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacDonald, ML and Jones, MK (2015). The role of danger expectancies and disgust in obsessive-compulsive washing. Journal of Psychology, 3, 127141.Google Scholar
Matchett, G and Davey, G (1991). A test of a disease-avoidance model of animal phobias. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 29, 9194. doi:10.1016/S0005-7967(09)80011-9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McKay, D, Abramowitz, JS, Calamari, JE, Kyrios, M, Radomsky, A, Sookman, D, Taylor, S and Wilhelm, S (2004). A critical evaluation of obsessive–compulsive disorder subtypes: Symptoms versus mechanisms. Clinical Psychology Review, 24, 283–231.10.1016/j.cpr.2004.04.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McNally, R (2002). Disgust has arrived. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 16, 561566. doi:10.1016/S0887-6185(02)00174-3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Melli, Gabriele, Moulding, Richard and Poli, Andrea (2016) Is it more disgusting if I think about it? Examining the interaction of obsessive beliefs and disgust propensity in clinical obsessive-compulsive disorder. Journal of Obsessive-Compulsive and Related Disorders 11, 7481. doi: 10.1016/j.jocrd.2016.09.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Merckelbach, H, de Jong, P, Arntz, A and Schouten, E (1993). The role of evaluative learning and disgust sensitivity in the etiology and treatment of spider phobia. Advances in Behaviour Research and Therapy, 15, 243255. doi:10.1016/0146-6402(93)90011-P.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Najmi, S and Amir, N (2010). The effect of attention training on a behavioral test of contamination fears in individuals with subclinical obsessive-compulsive symptoms. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 119, 136142. doi:10.1037/a0017549.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Obsessive–Compulsive Cognitions Working Group (1997). Cognitive assessment of obsessive-compulsive disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 35, 667681. doi:10.1016/S0005-7967(97)00017-X.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Obsessive–Compulsive Cognitions Working Group (2005). Psychometric validation of the obsessive belief questionnaire and interpretation of intrusions inventory — Part 2: Factor analyses and testing of a brief version. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 43, 15271542. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2004.07.010.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Olatunji, B and Cisler, J (2009). Disgust sensitivity: Psychometric overview and operational definition. doi:10.1037/11856-002.Google Scholar
Olatunji, B and Sawchuk, C (2005). Disgust: Characteristic features, social manifestations, and clinical implications. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 24, 932962. doi:10.1521/jscp.2005.24.7.932.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Olatunji, B, Cisler, J, Deacon, B, Connolly, K and Lohr, J (2007). The Disgust Propensity and Sensitivity Scale-Revised: Psychometric properties and specificity in relation to anxiety disorder symptoms. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 21, 918930. doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2006.12.005.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olatunji, Bunmi O, Moretz, Melanie W., Wolitzky-Taylor, Kate B, McKay, Dean, McGrath, Patrick B. and Ciesielski, Bethany G (2010) Disgust Vulnerability and Symptoms of Contamination-Based OCD: Descriptive Tests of Incremental Specificity. Behavior Therapy 41, 475490. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2009.11.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olatunji, B, Cisler, J, McKay, D and Phillips, M (2010). Is disgust associated with psychopathology? Emerging research in the anxiety disorders. Psychiatry Research, 175, 110. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2009.04.007.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rachman, S (1997). A cognitive theory of obsessions. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 35, 793802. doi:10.1016/S0005-7967(97)00040-5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rachman, S (1998). A cognitive theory of obsessions behavior and cognitive therapy today: Essays in honor of Hans J. Eysenck. (pp. 209222). England: Oxford; Elsevier Science Ltd.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rachman, S (2004). Fear of contamination. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 42, 12271255. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2003.10.009.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rozin, P, Haidt, J and McCauley, CR (1993). Disgust. In Lewis, M and Haviland, JM (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (pp. 575594). New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Salkovskis, P (1985). Obsessional-compulsive problems: A cognitive-behavioural analysis. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 23, 571583. doi:10.1016/0005-7967(85)90105-6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schnall, S, Haidt, J, Clore, G and Jordan, A (2008). Disgust as embodied moral judgment. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34, 10961109. doi:10.1177/0146167208317771.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shook, N, Thomas, R and Ford, C (2019). Testing the relation between disgust and general avoidance behavior. Personality and Individual Differences, 150, 109457. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2019.05.063.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thorpe, S, Barnett, J, Friend, K and Nottingham, K (2011). The mediating roles of disgust sensitivity and danger expectancy in relation to hand washing behaviour. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 39, 175190. doi:10.1017/S1352465810000676.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tolin, D, Woods, C and Abramowitz, J (2003). Relationship between obsessive beliefs and obsessive–compulsive symptoms. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 27, 657669. doi:10.1023/A:1026351711837.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Travis, R and Fergus, T (2015). The potentiating effect of disgust sensitivity on the relationship between disgust propensity and mental contamination. Journal of Obsessive Compulsive Related Disorders, 6, 114119. doi:10.1016/j.jocrd.2015.06.007.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Overveld, W, de Jong, P, Peters, M, Cavanagh, K and Davey, G (2006). Disgust propensity and disgust sensitivity: Separate constructs that are differentially related to specific fears. Personality and Individual Differences, 41, 12411252. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2006.04.021s.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Watson, D, Clark, L and Tellegen, A (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 10631070. Retrieved from: http://search.proquest.com/docview/78342057/CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Williams, M, Gomez Grajales, C and Kurkiewicz, D (2013). Assumptions of multiple regression: Correcting two misconceptions. Practical Assessment, Research & Evaluation, 18, 14.Google Scholar
Woody, SR and Teachman, BA (2000). Intersection of disgust and fear: Normative and pathological views. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 7, 291311. doi:10.1093clipsy7.3.291.Google Scholar