Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-jn8rn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-28T15:51:20.531Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

On the nature of morphological awareness in Japanese–English bilingual children: A cross-linguistic perspective*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  31 July 2012

YUKO HAYASHI*
Affiliation:
University of Oxford
VICTORIA A. MURPHY
Affiliation:
University of Oxford
*
Address for correspondence: Yuko Hayashi, 15 Norham Gardens, Oxford OX2 6PY, UKyuko.hayashi@education.ox.ac.uk

Abstract

While morphological awareness has received much attention to date, little is understood about how morphological awareness develops within bilingual children learning typologically different languages. Therefore, we investigated children's knowledge of inflections and derivations in Japanese and English, and also asked whether morphological awareness in one language predicted morphological awareness in the other. To that end, 24 Japanese learners of L2 English (ESL) and 21 English learners of Japanese as a heritage language (JHL) were recruited and participated in a range of tasks assessing both vocabulary and morphological knowledge. Cross-linguistic contributions of morphological awareness were identified in both directions (Japanese ↔ English), after controlling for age, IQ, and vocabulary knowledge. This bidirectional transfer was, however, identified only in the ESL group. The group-specific and reciprocal transfer observed is discussed in terms of morphological complexities and relative competence in each language. The potential role of different types of L2 instruction in morphological development is also discussed.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2012

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

*

The study is part of a larger-scale research project funded by the Japan Students Organisation Service (Grant No. L 09172010001). We would like to express our sincere gratitude to all the participating children for their hard work in the testing sessions and to their parents and teachers for their continued support and encouragement. We are very grateful to the anonymous reviewers for their valuable feedback and Lars-Erik Malmberg for statistical support. Special thanks are also extended to Mairéad McKendry for her assistance with the testing. The first author would also like to thank Hiroshi Ono, Shoichi Tanaka and Matthew Clarke for their stimulating discussions and valuable encouragement throughout the study.

References

Anglin, J. M. (1993). Vocabulary development: A morphological analysis. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E. (2001). Metalinguistic aspects of bilingual processing. Annual Review of Applied Linguistics, 21 (1), 169181.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bloomfield, L. (1935). Language. London: Allen & Unwin.Google Scholar
Bowers, P. N. (2006). Adding transparency to morphologically opaque words through instruction. Masters dissertation, Queen's University, Canada.Google Scholar
Bowers, P. N., & Kirby, J. R. (2010). Effects of morphological instruction on vocabulary acquisition. Reading and Writing, 23 (5), 515537.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Byrne, B. M. (2001). Structural equation modeling with AMOS: Basic concepts, applications, and programming (3rd edn.). Mahwah, NJ & London: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Carlisle, J. F. (1996). An exploratory study of morphological errors in children's written stories. Reading and Writing, 8 (1), 6172.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carlisle, J. F. (2000). Awareness of the structure and meaning of morphologically complex words: Impact on reading. Reading and Writing, 12 (3), 169190.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carlisle, J. F., & Fleming, J. (2003). Lexical processing of morphologically complex words in the elementary years. Scientific Studies of Reading, 7 (3), 239253.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cook, V. J. (1991). The poverty-of-the-stimulus argument and multicompetence. Second Language Research, 7 (2), 103117.Google Scholar
Cook, V. J. (2003). Introduction: The changing L1 in the L2 user's mind. In Cook, V. J. (ed.), Effects of the second language on the first, pp. 118. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coulmas, F. (1989). The writing systems of the world. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Cummins, J. (1984). Wanted: A theoretical framework for relating language proficiency to academic achievement among bilingual students. In Rivera, C. (ed.), Language proficiency and academic achievement, pp. 219. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Daniel, L. G. (1998). Statistical significance testing: A historical overview of misuse and misinterpretation with implications for the editorial policies of educational journals. Research in the Schools, 5 (2), 2332.Google Scholar
Deacon, S. H., Wade-Woolley, L., & Kirby, J. (2007). Crossover: The role of morphological awareness in French immersion children's reading. Developmental Psychology, 43 (3), 732746.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Diebold, A. R. (1964). Language in culture and society. New York: Harper Collins.Google Scholar
Duncan, L. G., Casalis, S., & Colé, P. (2009). Early metalinguistic awareness of derivational morphology: Observations from a comparison of English and French. Applied Psycholinguistics, 30 (3), 405440.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunn, L., Dunn, L., Whetton, C., & Burley, J. (1997). British picture vocabulary scale (2nd edn.). Windsor: NFER–Nelson.Google Scholar
Field, A. P. (2009). Discovering statistics using SPSS (3rd edn.). London: Sage.Google Scholar
García, O. (2009). Bilingual education in the 21st century: A global perspective. Malden, MA & Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
Green, L., McCutchen, D., Schwiebert, C., Quinlan, T., Eva-Wood, A., & Juelis, J. (2003). Morphological development in children's writing. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95 (4), 752761.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Guglielmi, R. S. (2008). Native language proficiency, English literacy, academic achievement, and occupational attainment in limited-English-proficient students: A latent growth modeling perspective. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100 (2), 322342.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hamers, J. F., & Blanc, M. H. A. (2000). Bilinguality and bilingualism (2nd edn.). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hayashi, Y., & Murphy, V. A. (2011). An investigation of morphological awareness in Japanese learners of English. Language Learning Journal, 39 (1), 105120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hofland, K., & Johansson, S. (1982). Word frequencies in British and American English. Bergen: Norwegian Computing Centre for the Humanities.Google Scholar
Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1998). Fit indices in covariance structure modeling: Sensitivity to underparameterized model misspecification. Psychological Methods, 3 (4), 424453.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Iwasaki, S. (2002). Japanese. Amsterdam & Philadelphia, PA: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jean, M., & Geva, E. (2009). The development of vocabulary in English as a second language children and its role in predicting word recognition ability. Applied Psycholinguistics, 30 (1), 153185.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johansson, S., & Hofland, K. (1989). Frequency analysis of English vocabulary and grammar (based on the LOB Corpus) (vol. I): Tag frequencies and word frequencies. Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Kageyama, T. (1982). Word formation in Japanese. Lingua, 57 (1), 215258.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kataoka, Y., Koshiyama, Y., & Shibata, F. (2005). Japanese and English language ability of students in supplementary Japanese school in the U.S. Kokusai Kyoiku Hyoron, (2), 119.Google Scholar
Kieffer, M. J., & Lesaux, N. K. (2008). The role of derivational morphology in the reading comprehension of Spanish-speaking English language learners. Reading and Writing, 21 (8), 783804.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kline, R. B. (2011). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling (3rd edn.). New York & London: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Konishi, T., & Minamide, K. (2007). Genius English–Japanese dictionary (4th edn.). Tokyo: Taishūkanshoten.Google Scholar
Kuo, L., & Anderson, R. C. (2006). Morphological awareness and learning to read: A cross-language perspective. Educational Psychologist, 41 (3), 161180.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Larson-Hall, J. (2010). A guide to doing statistics in second language research using SPSS. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Leech, G., Rayson, P., & Wilson, A. (2001). Word frequencies in written and spoken English: Based on the British National Corpus. Harlow: Longman.Google Scholar
MacCallum, R. C., Browne, M. W., & Sugawara, H. M. (1996). Power analysis and determination of sample size for covariance structure modeling. Psychological Methods, 1 (2), 130149.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Macnamara, J. (1967). The bilingual's linguistic performance – a psychological overview. Journal of Social Issues, 23 (2), 5877.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mahony, D. L. (1994). Using sensitivity to word structure to explain variance in high school and college level reading ability. Reading and Writing, 6 (1), 1944.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mahony, D., Singson, M., & Mann, V. (2000). Reading ability and sensitivity to morphological relations. Reading and Writing, 12 (3), 191218.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marsh, H. W., Hau, K. T., & Wen, Z. (2004). In search of golden rules: Comment on hypothesis-testing approaches to setting cutoff values for fit indexes and dangers in overgeneralizing Hu and Bentler's (1999) findings. Structural Equation Modeling, 11 (3), 320341.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McBride-Chang, C., Cho, J. R., Liu, H., Wagner, R. K., Shu, H., Zhou, A., Cheuk, C. S-M., & Muse, A. (2005). Changing models across cultures: Associations of phonological awareness and morphological structure awareness with vocabulary and word recognition in second graders from Beijing, Hong Kong, Korea, and the united states. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 92 (2), 140160.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meisel, J. M. (2004). The bilingual child. In Bhatia, T. K. & Ritchie, W. C. (eds.), The handbook of bilingualism, pp. 91113. Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
Menard, S. (1995). Applied logistic regression analysis (Quantitative Applications in the Social Sciences 106). Thousand Oaks, CA & London: Sage.Google Scholar
Menyuk, P., & Brisk, M. (2005). Language development and education: Children with varying language experiences. New York: Palgrave Macmillan.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ministry of Foreign Affairs of Japan (Gaimushō). (2011). Kaigaizairyū houjinsū toukei [The annual report on statistics on Japanese nationals overseas]. http://www.mofa.go.jp/mofaj/toko/tokei/hojin_sj/index.html (retrieved January 5, 2012).Google Scholar
Murphy, V. A. (2010). The relationship between age of learning and type of linguistic exposure in children learning a second language. In Macaro, E. (ed.), Continuum companion to second language acquisition, pp. 158178. London: Continuum.Google Scholar
Myers, R. H. (1990). Classical and modern regression with applications. Boston, MA: Duxbury.Google Scholar
Nagy, W., Berninger, V. W., & Abbott, R. D. (2006). Contributions of morphology beyond phonology to literacy outcomes of upper elementary and middle-school students. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98 (1), 134147.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
National Institute for Japanese Language and Linguistics (NINJAL) (2009). Basic research on the fundamental vocabulary used for educational purposes (revised and expanded edition). Tokyo: Meijishoin.Google Scholar
Nomura, M. (1973). On the uses of the negative prefixes “mu”, “hu”, “mi” and “hi”. Kotoba no Kenkyu, (4), 3150.Google Scholar
Nozaki, Y. (2002). Japanese politics and the history textbook controversy, 1982–2001. International Journal of Educational Research, 37 (6–7), 603622.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nunes, T., Bryant, P., & Bindman, M. (1997). Morphological spelling strategies: Developmental stages and processes. Developmental Psychology, 33 (4), 637649.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nunes, T., Bryant, P., & Bindman, M. (2006). The effects of learning to spell on children's awareness of morphology. Reading and Writing, 19 (7), 767787.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ono, H. (1988). Kaigaizaijū⋅kikokushijono nihongo⋅eigoryoku-to nihonno eigokyouiku [Japanese and English language abilities in Japanese children abroad and returnees and English teaching in Japan]. Acoustical Society of Japan, 44 (7), 531536.Google Scholar
Ono, H. (1994). Bilingual no kagaku: Doushitara nareru noka [The science of bilingualism]. Tokyo: Koudansha.Google Scholar
Ono, H., Shigemasu, K., Hayashibe, H., Okazaki, T., Ichikawa, M., Kinoshita, H., & Makino, Y. (1989). Nihongo nouryokukensa no kaihatsu [The development of test of Japanese language abilities]. Tokyo: Tokyo Gakugei University.Google Scholar
Paradis, J. (2007). Second language acquisition in childhood. In Hoff, E. & Shatz, M. (eds.), Blackwell handbook of language development, pp. 387405. Malden, MA & Oxford: Blackwell.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pasquarella, A., Chen, X., Lam, K., Luo, Y. C., & Ramirez, G. (2011). Cross-language transfer of morphological awareness in Chinese–English bilinguals. Journal of Research in Reading, 34 (1), 2342.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Proctor, C. P., Uccelli, P., Dalton, B., & Snow, C. E. (2009). Understanding depth of vocabulary online with bilingual and monolingual children. Reading & Writing Quarterly, 25 (4), 311333.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ramirez, G., Chen, X., Geva, E., & Kiefer, H. (2010). Morphological awareness in Spanish-speaking English language learners: Within and cross-language effects on word reading. Reading and Writing, 23 (3), 337358.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Raven, J., Rust, J., & Squire, A. (2008). Manual: Standard progressive matrices plus and mill hill vocabulary scale. London: NCS Pearson.Google Scholar
Roman, A., Kirby, J., Parrila, R., Wade-Woolley, L., & Deacon, S. H. (2009). Toward a comprehensive view of the skills involved in word reading in Grades 4, 6, and 8. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 102 (1), 96113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rushton, J. P., Čvorović, J., & Bons, T. A. (2007). General mental ability in south Asians: Data from three Roma (gypsy) communities in Serbia. Intelligence, 35 (1), 112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schumacker, R. E., & Lomax, R. G. (2004). A beginner's guide to structural equation modeling (2nd edn.). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shibatani, M. (1990). The languages of Japan. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Singson, M., Mahony, D., & Mann, V. (2000). The relation between reading ability and morphological skills: Evidence from derivational suffixes. Reading and Writing, 12 (3), 219252.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snow, C. E., & Kim, Y. S. (2007). Large problem spaces: The challenge of vocabulary for English language learners. In Wagner, R. K., Muse, A. E. & Tannenbaum, K. R. (eds.), Vocabulary acquisition: Implication for reading comprehension, pp. 123139. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Stage, F. K., Carter, H. C., & Nora, A. (2004). Path analysis: An introduction and analysis of a decade of research. The Journal of Educational Research, 98 (1), 513.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tsujimura, N. (2007). An introduction to Japanese linguistics (2nd ed.). Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Tyler, A., & Nagy, W. (1989). The acquisition of English derivational morphology. Journal of Memory and Language, 28 (6), 649667.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wang, M., Cheng, C., & Chen, S. (2006). Contribution of morphological awareness to Chinese–English biliteracy acquisition. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98 (3), 542553.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wang, M., Ko, I. Y., & Choi, J. (2009). The importance of morphological awareness in Korean–English biliteracy acquisition. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 34 (2), 132142.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Webb, S. (2008). Receptive and productive vocabulary sizes of L2 learners. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 30 (1), 7995.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wiig, E. H., & Secord, W. (1992). TOWK: The Test of Word Knowledge. Toronto: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar