Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-p9bg8 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T17:36:04.144Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Maternal fish consumption and child neurodevelopment in Nutrition 1 Cohort: Seychelles Child Development Study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 February 2023

Marie C. Conway*
Affiliation:
Nutrition Innovation Centre for Food and Health (NICHE), Ulster University, Coleraine, Northern Ireland
Alison J. Yeates
Affiliation:
Nutrition Innovation Centre for Food and Health (NICHE), Ulster University, Coleraine, Northern Ireland
Tanzy M. Love
Affiliation:
School of Medicine and Dentistry, University of Rochester, Rochester, NY, USA
Daniel Weller
Affiliation:
School of Medicine and Dentistry, University of Rochester, Rochester, NY, USA
Emeir M. McSorley
Affiliation:
Nutrition Innovation Centre for Food and Health (NICHE), Ulster University, Coleraine, Northern Ireland
Maria S. Mulhern
Affiliation:
Nutrition Innovation Centre for Food and Health (NICHE), Ulster University, Coleraine, Northern Ireland
Maria Wesolowska
Affiliation:
Nutrition Innovation Centre for Food and Health (NICHE), Ulster University, Coleraine, Northern Ireland
Gene E. Watson
Affiliation:
School of Medicine and Dentistry, University of Rochester, Rochester, NY, USA
Gary J. Myers
Affiliation:
School of Medicine and Dentistry, University of Rochester, Rochester, NY, USA
Conrad F. Shamlaye
Affiliation:
The Ministry of Health, Mahé, Republic of Seychelles
Juliette Henderson
Affiliation:
The Ministry of Health, Mahé, Republic of Seychelles
Philip W. Davidson
Affiliation:
School of Medicine and Dentistry, University of Rochester, Rochester, NY, USA
Edwin van Wijngaarden
Affiliation:
School of Medicine and Dentistry, University of Rochester, Rochester, NY, USA
J. J. Strain
Affiliation:
Nutrition Innovation Centre for Food and Health (NICHE), Ulster University, Coleraine, Northern Ireland
*
*Corresponding author: Dr M. C. Conway, email conway-m7@ulster.ac.uk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Maternal fish consumption exposes the fetus to beneficial nutrients and potentially adverse neurotoxicants. The current study investigated associations between maternal fish consumption and child neurodevelopmental outcomes. Maternal fish consumption was assessed in the Seychelles Child Development Study Nutrition Cohort 1 (n 229) using 4-day food diaries. Neurodevelopment was evaluated at 9 and 30 months, and 5 and 9 years with test batteries assessing twenty-six endpoints and covering multiple neurodevelopmental domains. Analyses used multiple linear regression with adjustment for covariates known to influence child neurodevelopment. This cohort consumed an average of 8 fish meals/week and the total fish intake during pregnancy was 106·8 (sd 61·9) g/d. Among the twenty-six endpoints evaluated in the primary analysis there was one beneficial association. Children whose mothers consumed larger quantities of fish performed marginally better on the Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test (a test of nonverbal intelligence) at age 5 years (β 0·003, 95 % CI (0, 0·005)). A secondary analysis dividing fish consumption into tertiles found no significant associations when comparing the highest and lowest consumption groups. In this cohort, where fish consumption is substantially higher than current global recommendations, maternal fish consumption during pregnancy was not beneficially or adversely associated with children’s neurodevelopmental outcomes.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of The Nutrition Society

Fish and seafood are dietary staples for many populations worldwide and globally represent a major source of dietary protein(Reference Nesheim, Oria and Yih1). The Food and Agriculture Organization of the United Nations (FAO) estimates that aquatic foods account for at least 20 % of average per capita intake of animal protein for 3·3 billion people(2). Fish is also a rich source of nutrients known to be essential for fetal neurodevelopment, in particular long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acids (LCPUFA), iodine and vitamin D(Reference Weichselbaum, Coe and Buttriss3). The LCPUFA docosahexanoic acid (DHA) is critical for optimal visual and brain development and deficiencies during fetal growth may have lifelong adverse consequences for brain function(Reference Innis4). Women who consume fish throughout pregnancy are more likely to achieve optimal intakes of these essential nutrients(Reference Bonham, Duffy and Robson5). A large body of evidence supports the nutritional benefits of fish consumption throughout pregnancy(Reference Starling, Charlton and McMahon6Reference Spiller, Hibbeln and Myers8). However, fish also contains small amounts of methylmercury (MeHg) and public health consumption guidelines have been formulated with the central aim of limiting possible risk from this naturally occurring environmental pollutant.

Public health advice to pregnant women has been variable. In their 2014 Opinion, the European Food Safety Authority concluded that three to four servings of fish/week (equivalent to >450 g or 16 oz/week) has nutritional benefits for neurodevelopment compared with no fish consumption(9). Similar guidance in the USA recommends that pregnant women should consume 8–12 oz (equivalent to approximately 227–340 g) of fish/week(1012). The UK advice, last updated in 2004, recommends consuming two portions of fish/week (equivalent to ∼280 g or 10 oz./week) with at least one of these being oily (or fatty) fish(13). Each of these guidelines recommends on a precautionary basis that fish with a high MeHg content (such as shark or swordfish) should be limited or avoided altogether. In many countries, fish consumption in women of childbearing age is significantly below the recommended amounts(Reference Jahns, Raatz and Johnson14,15) . Public confusion about the benefits and risks of fish consumption in the USA contributed to some women avoiding fish altogether when pregnant(Reference Taylor, Emmett and Emond16). Limiting fish consumption during pregnancy has possible long-term adverse consequences given its nutritional contribution to the diet.

In 2019, an expert panel conducted a systematic review to evaluate the risks and benefits of seafood consumption (excluding sea mammals) during pregnancy(Reference Hibbeln, Spiller and Brenna7). That study reported finding no evidence of an upper limit of intake at which adverse neurodevelopmental outcomes were present. The authors emphasised the benefits of consuming adequate amounts of a wide range of seafood for the greatest cognitive benefits to neurodevelopment, as well as the effect of beneficial nutrients to outweigh potential adverse effects of MeHg exposure(Reference Hibbeln, Spiller and Brenna7,Reference Spiller, Hibbeln and Myers8) . Fish advisories in the USA are based on epidemiological studies of individuals consuming whales (Faroe Islands) and shark (New Zealand) with co-exposure to multiple other neurotoxicants and the precautionary principle(Reference Lipfert, Morris and Sullivan17). However, findings from the multi-cohort Seychelles Child Development Study (SCDS) support the conclusion that the beneficial effects of nutrients in fish outweigh the possible adverse effects of MeHg(Reference Davidson, Myers and Cox18Reference Strain, Davidson and Bonham22). The SCDS has studied a population that consumes on average more than eight fish meals/week, several times higher than global recommendations(9,1113,Reference Davidson, Strain and Myers19) . The population has one of the highest prenatal MeHg exposures from fish consumption ever studied (> 5 ppm measured in maternal hair), consumes fish with MeHg concentrations similar to those in commercial fish in the UK and USA, and does not consume sea mammals(Reference Robinson and Shroff23). The study has followed three independent longitudinal cohorts over 24 years and found no consistent evidence of adverse associations between MeHg exposure and child neurodevelopmental outcomes(Reference Davidson, Myers and Cox18Reference Strain, Love and Yeates21). The SCDS has found beneficial associations between maternal LCPUFA status during pregnancy and early childhood neurodevelopment of offspring, with evidence that n-3 and n-6 PUFA may ameliorate negative outcomes from MeHg, if any are present, at this level of exposure(Reference Strain, Yeates and Van Wijngaarden20,Reference Strain, Davidson and Bonham22) .

Previous analyses of the SCDS cohorts focused on individual biomarkers of MeHg exposure and LCPUFA status. The aim of the current study is to investigate associations between maternal fish consumption (consumed as a whole food during pregnancy) and children’s neurodevelopmental outcomes at 9 and 30 months, and 5 and 9 years. The advantage of this approach, as advised by the FDA in their 2014 report on net effects(10), is that it allows both the beneficial contributions of nutrients and potential adverse contributions of MeHg to be considered concurrently. Consequently, results should prove more meaningful for formulating accurate public health guidance.

Subjects and methods

Population and location

The SCDS is a longitudinal observational study being conducted in the Republic of Seychelles. The primary aim of the study is to investigate the influence of prenatal MeHg exposure from fish consumption during pregnancy on child neurodevelopmental outcomes(Reference Davidson, Myers and Cox18). The Nutrition Cohort 1 (NC1) has the most comprehensive assessment of fish consumption during pregnancy of any SCDS maternal–child cohort to date and additionally comprehensive assessments of the children’s neurodevelopment. In 2001, we enrolled a total of 300 healthy pregnant women(Reference Strain, Davidson and Bonham22). A power calculation determined 250 participants were required to detect a five-point difference on the Bayley Scales of Infant Development II (BSID II) (primary outcome) between the low and high MeHg exposure groups(Reference Davidson, Strain and Myers19). Mothers were recruited during their first antenatal appointment (from 14 weeks of gestation) across the Island of Mahé, the main island of Seychelles. Inclusion criteria were over 16 years of age, native-born Seychellois and having a normal, healthy pregnancy.

Among the 300 women recruited to NC1, there were several exclusions owing to miscarriage/abortion (n 12), not being pregnant (n 4), illness (n 1), relocation (n 2) and noncompliance (n 8). Additionally, forty-four participants had incomplete dietary data and are not included in this analysis (online Supplementary Fig. 1).

Ethical approval

This study was conducted according to the guidelines laid down in the Declaration of Helsinki, and all procedures involving participants were reviewed and approved by the Seychelles Ethics Board and the Research Subjects Review Board at the University of Rochester. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants.

Fish intake data

Dietary data were available at 28 weeks gestation for 229 mothers as detailed in Bonham et al. (Reference Bonham, Duffy and Wallace24) Mothers completed a 4-day semi-quantitative food diary for two consecutive weekdays and two weekend days. The food diaries were available in both English and the native Kreol language and dietitians provided mothers with detailed information on how to complete them. Women were asked to record the amount and types of foods and beverages consumed. Diaries were reviewed locally by dietitians within 1 week of completion. Subsequently, nutritionists from Ulster University, Coleraine reviewed them for any errors or omissions and requested clarification from participants. Food diary data were converted to weight in grams and analysed using dietary analysis software (WISP version 2.0; Tinuviel Software, Warrington, UK) allowing for quantitative food and nutrient intakes to be determined. WISP software was updated with recipe and food composition data for foods commonly eaten in Seychelles using a variety of food composition tables including The Composition of South African Foods (Reference Sayed, Frans and Schonfeldt25) and The Concise New Zealand Food Composition Tables (Reference Ather, McLaughlin and Taylor26). The food diaries provide data on the amount (g/d) of a range of fish consumed during pregnancy. Each fish meal (g/d) was categorised into: fatty fish, lean fish, crustaceans, molluscs and fish products and dishes. Owing to a large number of non-consumers for the categories of crustaceans, molluscs and fish products and dishes in this cohort, these variables were excluded from analysis. Our analysis focused on the variable of fish consumption (g/d), calculated as the sum of fatty fish and lean fish consumed.

Developmental assessment

Seychellois maternal child health nurses specially trained at the University of Rochester administered all neurodevelopmental tests. Children completed testing at ages 9 and 30 months, and 5 and 9 years. All tests were translated into Kreol. At 9 and 30 months children completed the BSID II (Reference Bayley27) as described in Davidson et al. (Reference Davidson, Strain and Myers19) At age 5-years, the test battery included the following as described by Strain et al. (Reference Strain, Davidson and Thurston28): Finger Tapping (Dominant and Non-Dominant hand)(Reference Schatz, Kreutzer, DeLuca and Caplan29), the Preschool Language Scale (PLS) (Auditory Comprehension, Verbal Ability and Total Language)(Reference Zimmerman, Steiner and Pond30), the Woodcock–Johnson (WJ) Tests of Achievement (Applied Problems and Letter-Word Recognition)(Reference Schrank31), the Achenbach Child Behaviour Checklist (CBCL) (Total score)(Reference Achenbach and Rescorla32) and the Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test (KBIT) (Verbal Knowledge and Matrices)(Reference Kaufman and Kaufman33). At age 9 years, the Children’s test battery included the following: CBCL(Reference Achenbach and Rescorla32), Bender Visual Motor Gestalt(Reference Woltmann, Abt and Bellak34), Conners’ Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) Index(Reference Conners, Pitkanen, Rzepa, Kreutzer, DeLuca and Caplan35), Expressive Vocabulary Test (EVT)(Reference Williams36), KBIT (Verbal Knowledge and Matrices)(Reference Kaufman and Kaufman33), Peabody Picture Vocabulary (PPV) test(Reference Dunn37), Stroop(Reference Scarpina and Tagini38), Trail Making Time (Part A and B)(Reference Heller, Skinner, Tomiyama, Gellman and Turner39) and the WJ Tests of Achievement (Applied Problems and Letter-Word Recognition)(Reference Schrank31).

Covariates

Consistent with our previous work(Reference Davidson, Myers and Cox18,Reference Strain, Yeates and Van Wijngaarden20Reference Strain, Davidson and Bonham22) , multivariable regression analyses controlled for covariates already known to be associated with child neurodevelopment including: maternal age and IQ (KBIT), child sex, birthweight, and age at testing, socio-economic status (the Hollingshead four-factor SES modified for use in Seychelles), family status (the presence of both parents living with the child), and the home environment (the Paediatric Review of Children’s Environmental Support and Stimulation (PROCESS)).

Statistical analysis

Descriptive analysis was performed, and all data were expressed as mean ± sd, median, interquartile range and minimum and maximum values. The primary analysis was a series of multiple linear regressions where we separately examined associations between total fish consumption on a continuous scale (g/d) and child neurodevelopmental outcomes at each testing time point, while controlling for maternal age and KBIT, child sex, birthweight, and age at testing, family status, socio-economic status and PROCESS. To examine for any nonlinearity in the association of fish intake and endpoints, we conducted a secondary set of analyses using tertiles of fish consumption, with the lowest tertile as the reference group. Owing to the high levels of fish consumption in our cohort, it was not possible to categorise fish intakes with reference to the current FDA advice, above or below the lower cut point of 8 oz/week (equivalent to 32·4 g/d), as only eleven women reported consumption < 8 oz (227 g/week) of seafood, the lower FDA recommendation and three reported no seafood consumption. Therefore, we divided fish consumption into tertiles and examined their relationship with endpoints. Mothers in the lowest tertile consumed up to 74·5 g/d (median 55 g/d; equivalent to 14 oz/week) total fish. Mothers in the middle tertile consumed 74·6–118·6 g/d (median 97·3 g/d; equivalent to 24 oz/week) and mothers in the highest tertile consumed 118·7–413·3 g/d (median 156·6 g/d; equivalent to 39 oz/week). Analysis was performed with R statistical software, and statistical significance in all analyses was considered a two-sided P value <0·05.

Results

A total of n 229 mother–child pairs had complete dietary, neurodevelopmental and covariate data available. The average (sd) maternal age was 27·69 (5·88) years. The cohort comprised n 116 girls and n 113 boys. The average (sd) maternal total fish consumed in this cohort was 106·8 (61·9) g/d measured at 28 weeks’ gestation as shown in Table 1. As different numbers of children completed each cognitive test, the n for each model differs and is shown within Table 2, which also displays summary statistics for the child outcomes at each time point.

Table 1. Maternal characteristics of Nutrition Cohort 1 (NC1) with maternal fish consumption and any completed outcomes (n 229)

IQR, interquartile range; SES, socio-economic status; KBIT, Kaufmann brief intelligence test; PROCESS, Paediatric Review of Children’s Environmental Support and Stimulation.

Table 2. Summary statistics for Nutrition Cohort 1 (NC1) child cognitive outcomes at each time point

NC1, Nutrition Cohort 1; MDI, mental developmental index; PDI, psychomotor developmental index; FT, finger tapping; PLS, Preschool Language Scale; WJ, Woodcock–Johnson; CBCL, Child Behaviour Checklist; KBIT, Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test; ADHD, attention-deficient hyperactivity disorder; EVT, Expressive Vocabulary Test; PPV, peabody picture vocabulary; TM, trail making.

The primary analysis using total fish consumption as a continuous variable and its association with child neurodevelopmental endpoints at each time point is presented in Table 3. Total fish consumption was positively associated with the KBIT Matrices score, a measure of non-verbal intelligence at age 5 years (β = 0·003, 95 % CI (0·000, 0·005), P = 0·03). There were no adverse associations with child neurodevelopmental outcomes. However, if we had applied the Bonferroni correction for multiple testing and set P values at less than 0·002 as statistically significant, then no associations would have met that conservative threshold in primary analysis.

Table 3. Associations between maternal fish consumption (continuous) and child cognitive outcomes at each time point adjusted for maternal age and KBIT, child sex, birthweight, and age at testing, family status, socio-economic status and PROCESS

PROCESS, Paediatric Review of Children’s Environmental Support and Stimulation; MDI, mental developmental index; PDI, psychomotor developmental index; FT, finger tapping; PLS, Preschool Language Scale; WJ, Woodcock–Johnson; CBCL: Child Behaviour Checklist; KBIT: Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test; ADHD, attention-deficient hyperactivity disorder; EVT, Expressive Vocabulary Test; PPV, peabody picture vocabulary; TM, trail making.

Multiple regression models were fit separately and adjusted for maternal age at birth, child age at testing, child sex, birthweight, socio-economic status, family status, home environment and maternal IQ.

A secondary analysis examined fish consumption using tertiles (see Table 4). Among the fifty-two comparisons, there were no significant associations between the highest and the lowest tertiles. At age 5 years, children of mothers in the middle tertile showed a statistically significant adverse difference in score on the WJ Applied Problems scores (a test of mathematical reasoning) from mothers in the lowest tertile. Scores were 1·16 points lower on average (95 % CI (−2·309, −0·007)) than those of mothers in the lowest tertile (P = 0·049). We consider this a spurious finding because it was one of fifty-two comparisons, and there was no association between the highest and lowest tertile on this test. In all models, reported associations did not meaningfully change when comparing the associations from models controlling for covariates to those from unadjusted models (see Supplementary Tables). No associations would have been statistically significant if Bonferroni correction for multiple testing and a resultant P-value threshold of < 0·002 used.

Table 4. Associations between maternal total fish consumption (tertiles of intake) and child neurodevelopmental outcomes at each time point adjusted for maternal age and KBIT, child sex, birthweight, and age at testing, family status, socio-economic status and PROCESS

PROCESS, Paediatric Review of Children’s Environmental Support and Stimulation; MDI, mental developmental index; PDI, psychomotor developmental index; FT, finger tapping; PLS, Preschool Language Scale; WJ, Woodcock–Johnson; CBCL, Child Behaviour Checklist; KBIT, Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test; ADHD, attention-deficient hyperactivity disorder; EVT, Expressive Vocabulary Test; PPV, peabody picture vocabulary; TM, trail making.

*Tertile median g/d (tertile range g/d); range of fish intake for each tertile at each time point is as follows: 9 months: low (n 77) = 55·0 g/d (0–74·5), middle (n 76) = 97·3 g/d (74·6–118·6), high (n 76) = 156·6 g/d (118·7–413·3); 30 months: low (n 76) = 55·0 g/d (0–74·3), middle (n 76) = 97·3 g/d (74·4–118·8), high (n 76) = 156·6 g/d (118·9–413·3); 5 years: low (n 74) = 55·0 g/d (0–74·7), middle (n 74) = 96·8 g/d (74·8–118·4), high (n 74) = 155·3 g/d (118·5–413·3); 9 years: low (=72) = 55·4 g/d (0–74·3), middle (n 72) = 97·6 g/d (74·4–118·8), high (n 72) = 155·3 g/d (118·9–413·3).

Discussion

In the primary analysis examining the association of maternal fish consumption as a continuous variable with the twenty-six neurodevelopmental endpoints, we found one positive association. The children’s KBIT matrices, a test of nonverbal intelligence, at age 5 years improved as fish consumption increased. In a secondary analysis categorising fish consumption by tertiles, we found no significant associations between the highest and lowest tertiles. However, there was a statistically significant adverse difference in score on the WJ Applied Problems scores in children from mothers in the middle tertile when compared with children from mothers in the lowest tertile. We interpret our study as providing no clear evidence in either the primary or secondary analysis of beneficial or adverse associations between maternal fish consumption and children’s neurodevelopment. These results are consistent with our earlier findings in this cohort and findings of two recent systematic reviews which showed no adverse associations of fish consumption.

In our earlier assessment of this cohort, we found the mothers’ total n-3 PUFA status (a proxy for fatty fish consumed during pregnancy) was positively associated with the PDI in this age group(Reference Strain, Davidson and Bonham22). This finding suggested that higher n-3 PUFA may be contributing to the improved psychomotor development of infants at this age. The guidance from fish advisories differs worldwide, but the most common advice during pregnancy is to consume fish 2 to 3 times/week, with at least one portion being fatty fish(912). The suggested benefits are believed to be mainly attributable to DHA, a crucial nutrient in pregnancy for brain neurodevelopment(Reference Innis4). The benefits of DHA for neurodevelopment are well established(Reference Innis4), but the evidence for prenatal DHA supplementation remains inconclusive(Reference Lehner, Staub and Aldakak40).

In contrast, there is convincing evidence of the benefits of fish consumption in pregnancy for infant neurodevelopment from multiple studies that have evaluated fish as a whole food. Two rigorous scientific reviews of the evidence in this field concluded that there were no adverse associations of fish consumption with children’s neurodevelopment(Reference Hibbeln, Spiller and Brenna7,Reference Spiller, Hibbeln and Myers8) . The reviews evaluated data from forty-four publications where the range of beneficial outcomes included improved visual acuity, early language and communication skills, IQ and social skills in children(Reference Hibbeln, Spiller and Brenna7,Reference Spiller, Hibbeln and Myers8) . In these studies, fish consumption ranged from ∼4 oz (113 g) per week up to > 100 oz (2835 g or ≥405 g/d) per week(Reference Hibbeln, Spiller and Brenna7,Reference Spiller, Hibbeln and Myers8) ). Women in the SCDS NC1 consumed on average approximately 106 g/d (3·7 oz) fish, which is equivalent to 26 oz/week; these quantities are substantially more than the FDA advice to consume 8 to 12 oz/week in pregnancy.

As the Seychellois are such a high fish-consuming population, exposure to MeHg is several times higher than in the USA or UK. However, it is important to note that MeHg concentrations in fish in the Seychelles(Reference Robinson and Shroff23) are the same as in countries such as USA(41); therefore, it is the high levels of fish consumption, rather than Seychelles fish containing higher MeHg that leads to higher MeHg exposure for the Seychellois population. Our results add further evidence to the existing reports which found no adverse associations with high fish consumption during pregnancy(Reference Hibbeln, Spiller and Brenna7). We have previously reported that the nutrients, mainly LCPUFA, present in fish are likely to overcome any potential adverse toxic effects of prenatal MeHg exposure(Reference Strain, Yeates and Van Wijngaarden20Reference Strain, Davidson and Bonham22). Our findings add to the evidence supporting the safety of consuming fish that has only naturally acquired amounts of MeHg.

Strengths of our study include its prospective longitudinal double-blind exposure design and neurodevelopmental evaluations by specially trained nurse evaluators at multiple time points using a comprehensive battery of tests including measures of IQ and verbal development. Also, detailed dietary data collected prospectively through the completion of 4-d food diaries, a method which minimises some of the errors typically associated with interviewer technique and memory recall(Reference Biró, Hulshof and Ovesen42). The dietary data were further strengthened by our update of the WISP dietary analysis software with food composition data for foods specific to Seychelles and extensive review of the data by dietitians in Seychelles and nutritionists at Ulster University. Additionally, in Seychelles, consuming sea mammals is prohibited and there is no co-exposure to other pollutants which could potentially be detrimental to fetal neurodevelopment. Limitations of the study include it being an observational epidemiology study and unmeasured covariates might have been omitted, and the sample size is relatively small.

Conclusion

In this cohort, where fish consumption is substantially higher than current global recommendations, maternal fish consumption during pregnancy was not beneficially or adversely associated with children’s neurodevelopmental outcomes in primary or secondary analyses across numerous time points up to 9 years of age.

Acknowledgements

The authors thank the Seychelles Child Development Nutrition Cohort 1 participants and the health team in Seychelles for data collection and all their work with the implementation of dietary assessment. This work was supported by grants from the USA National Institute of Environmental Health Sciences, National Institutes of Health (grants R01 ES10219, P30 ES01247, T32ES0007271 and R01 ES015578), the European Union (contract EU FP6-2004-FIID-3-A PHIME, Public Health Impact of long term, low-level Mixed Element Exposure in susceptible population strata) and by the Government of Seychelles.

The authors G. J. M., C. F. S., P. W. D., G. E. W., E. V. W. and J. J. S. collaboratively designed the SCDS NC1 study (project conception, development of overall research plan and study oversight), with the concept for the present paper conceptualised by G. J. M., E. V. W. and J. J. S. C. F. S., M. S. M., E. M. C. S., G. E. W. and J. H. conducted the research (hands-on conduct of the experiments and data collection). D.W. and T. L. analysed the data and helped draft the manuscript. M. C. C., M. W. and A. J. Y. assisted with analysing the data, interpretation of data and co-drafted the manuscript. All authors reviewed, edited and approved the final article. M. C. C. and A. J. Y. had full access to all the data in the study and accepted final responsibility for the decision to submit for publication. The funders had no involvement or restrictions in relation to publication of this manuscript.

All authors declare no conflicts of interest.

Supplementary material

For supplementary material/s referred to in this article, please visit https://doi.org/10.1017/S0007114523000375

References

Nesheim, MC, Oria, M & Yih, PT (2015) A Framework for Assessing Effects of the Food System. Washington, DC: National Academies Press.Google Scholar
FAO (2022) The State of World Fisheries and Aquaculture 2022. Towards Blue Transformation. Rome: FAO.Google Scholar
Weichselbaum, E, Coe, S, Buttriss, J, et al. (2013) Fish in the diet: a review. Nutr Bull 38, 128177.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Innis, SM (2007) Dietary (n-3) fatty acids and brain development. J Nutr 137, 855859.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bonham, MP, Duffy, EM, Robson, PJ, et al. (2009) Contribution of fish to intakes of micronutrients important for fetal development: a dietary survey of pregnant women in the Republic of Seychelles. Public Health Nutr 12, 13121320.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Starling, P, Charlton, K, McMahon, AT, et al. (2015) Fish intake during pregnancy and foetal neurodevelopment – a systematic review of the evidence. Nutrients 7, 20012014.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hibbeln, CJR, Spiller, P, Brenna, JT, et al. (2019) Relationships between seafood consumption during pregnancy and childhood and neurocognitive development: two systematic reviews. Prostaglandins Leukot Essent Fat Acids 151, 1436.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spiller, P, Hibbeln, JR, Myers, G, et al. (2019) An abundance of seafood consumption studies presents new opportunities to evaluate effects on neurocognitive development. Prostaglandins Leukot Essent Fat Acids 151, 813.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
EFSA (2014) Scientific opinion on health benefits of seafood (fish and shellfish) consumption in relation to health risks associated with exposure to methylmercury. EFSA J 12, 3761.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Food & Drug Administration (2014) Quantitative-Assessment-of-the-Net-Effects-from-Eating-Commercial-Fish-on-Fetal-Neurodevelopment-(As-Measured-by-IQ-and-also-by-Early-Age-Verbal-Development-in-Children).pdf. https://www.fda.gov/food/environmental-contaminants-food/quantitative-assessment-net-effects-fetal-neurodevelopment-eating-commercial-fish-measured-iq-and (accessed March 2023).Google Scholar
Food & Drug Administration (2019) Advice about Eating Fish: For Women Who Are or Might Become Pregnant, Breastfeeding Mothers, and Young Children. https://www.fda.gov/food/consumers/advice-about-eating-fish (accessed March 2023).Google Scholar
U.S. Department of Agriculture & U.S. Department of Health and Human Services (2020) Dietary Guidelines for Americans, 2020–2025.https://www.dietaryguidelines.gov/ (accessed March 2023).Google Scholar
SACN (2004) Advice on Fish Consumption: Benefits & Risks 2004. London: TSO.Google Scholar
Jahns, L, Raatz, SK, Johnson, LAK, et al. (2014) Intake of seafood in the US varies by age, income, and education level but not by race-ethnicity. Nutrients 6, 60606075.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Food Standards Agency (2016) National diet and nutrition survey results from years 5 and 6 (combined) of the rolling programme (2012/2013–2013/2014). https://www.gov.uk/government/statistics/ndns-results-from-years-5-and-6-combined (accessed March 2023).Google Scholar
Taylor, CM, Emmett, PM, Emond, AM, et al. (2018) A review of guidance on fish consumption in pregnancy: is it fit for purpose? Public Health Nutr 21, 21492159.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lipfert, F, Morris, S, Sullivan, T, et al. (2005) Methylmercury, fish consumption, and the precautionary principle. J Air Waste Manag Assoc 55, 388398.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davidson, PW, Myers, GJ, Cox, C, et al. (1998) Effects of prenatal and postnatal methylmercury exposure from fish consumption on neurodevelopment: outcomes at 66 months of age in the Seychelles child development study. J Am Med Assoc 280, 701707.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davidson, PW, Strain, JJ, Myers, GJ, et al. (2008) Neurodevelopmental effects of maternal nutritional status and exposure to methylmercury from eating fish during pregnancy. Neurotoxicology 29, 767775.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strain, JJ, Yeates, AJ, Van Wijngaarden, E, et al. (2015) Prenatal exposure to methyl mercury from fish consumption and polyunsaturated fatty acids: associations with child development at 20 months of age in an observational study in the Republic of Seychelles. Am J Clin Nutr 101, 530537.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Strain, JJ, Love, TM, Yeates, AJ, et al. (2020) Associations of prenatal methylmercury exposure and maternal polyunsaturated fatty acid status with neurodevelopmental outcomes at 7 years of age: results from the Seychelles child development study nutrition cohort 2. Am J Clin Nutr 113, 304313.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Strain, JJ, Davidson, PW, Bonham, MP, et al. (2008) Associations of maternal long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acids, methyl mercury, and infant development in the Seychelles child development nutrition study. Neurotoxicology 29, 776782.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Robinson, J & Shroff, J (2020) Observations on the levels of total mercury (Hg) and selenium (Se) in species common to the artisanal fisheries of Seychelles. Neurotoxicology 81, 277281.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bonham, MP, Duffy, EM, Wallace, JMW, et al. (2008) Habitual fish consumption does not prevent a decrease in LCPUFA status in pregnant women (the Seychelles child development nutrition study). Prostaglandins Leukot Essent Fat Acids 78, 343350.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sayed, N, Frans, Y & Schonfeldt, H (1999) Composition of South African Foods: Milk and Milk Products; Eggs; Meat and Meat Products. Cape Town: Medical Research Council.Google Scholar
Ather, N, McLaughlin, J & Taylor, G (2003) The Concise New Zealand Food Composition Tables, 6th ed. Palmerston North: New Zealand Institute for Crop and Food Research Limited.Google Scholar
Bayley, N (1993) Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development II,. San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Brace & Co.Google Scholar
Strain, JJ, Davidson, PW, Thurston, SW, et al. (2012) Maternal PUFA status but not prenatal methylmercury exposure is associated with children’s language functions at age 5 years in the Seychelles. J Nutr 142, 19431949.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schatz, P (2011) Finger tapping test. In Encyclopedia of Clinical Neuropsychology, pp. 10501051 [Kreutzer, JS, DeLuca, J and Caplan, B, editors]. New York, NY: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zimmerman, IL, Steiner, VG & Pond, R (2002) Preschool Language Scale, 4th ed. APA PsycTests.Google Scholar
Schrank, FMKMN (2014) Woodcock-Johnson IV. Rolling Meadows, IL: Riverside Publishing.Google Scholar
Achenbach, TM & Rescorla, LA (2010) ASEBA Child Behavior Checklist. Burlington, VT: ASEBA.Google Scholar
Kaufman, AS & Kaufman, NL (2004) Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test, 2nd ed. Circle Pines, MN: AGS Publishing.Google Scholar
Woltmann, AG (1950) The bender visual-motor gestalt test. In Project Psychology Clinical Approaches to Total Personal, pp. 322356 [Abt, LE and Bellak, L, editors]. New York: Alfred A. Knopf.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Conners, CK, Pitkanen, J & Rzepa, S (2011) Conners 3rd edition (Conners 3; Conners 2008). In Encyclopedia of Clinical Neuropsychology, 3rd ed. pp. 675–678 [Kreutzer, JS, DeLuca, J and Caplan, B, editors]. New York, NY: Springer.Google Scholar
Williams, K (2007) Expressive Vocabulary Test, 2nd ed. APA PsycTests.Google Scholar
Dunn, L (1997) PPVT-III : Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test. Circle Pines, MN: AGS Publishing.Google Scholar
Scarpina, F & Tagini, S (2017) The Stroop color and word test. Front Psychol 8, 557.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heller, LJ, Skinner, CS, Tomiyama, AJ, et al. (2013) Trail-making test. In Encyclopedia of Behavioral Medicine, pp. 19861987 [Gellman, MD and Turner, J, editors]. New York, NY: Springer.Google Scholar
Lehner, A, Staub, K, Aldakak, L, et al. (2020) Impact of n-3 fatty acid DHA and EPA supplementation in pregnant or breast-feeding women on cognitive performance of children: systematic review and meta-analysis. Nutr Rev, 114.Google Scholar
FDA (2022) Mercury Levels in Commercial Fish and Shellfish (1990–2012). https://www.fda.gov/food/metals-and-your-food/mercury-levels-commercial-fish-and-shellfish-1990–2012 (accessed December 2022).Google Scholar
Biró, G, Hulshof, KF, Ovesen, L, et al. (2002) Selection of methodology to assess food intake. Eur J Clin Nutr 56, S25S32.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Maternal characteristics of Nutrition Cohort 1 (NC1) with maternal fish consumption and any completed outcomes (n 229)

Figure 1

Table 2. Summary statistics for Nutrition Cohort 1 (NC1) child cognitive outcomes at each time point

Figure 2

Table 3. Associations between maternal fish consumption (continuous) and child cognitive outcomes at each time point adjusted for maternal age and KBIT, child sex, birthweight, and age at testing, family status, socio-economic status and PROCESS

Figure 3

Table 4. Associations between maternal total fish consumption (tertiles of intake) and child neurodevelopmental outcomes at each time point adjusted for maternal age and KBIT, child sex, birthweight, and age at testing, family status, socio-economic status and PROCESS

Supplementary material: File

Conway et al. supplementary material

Figure S1

Download Conway et al. supplementary material(File)
File 24.6 KB
Supplementary material: File

Conway et al. supplementary material

Tables S1-S2

Download Conway et al. supplementary material(File)
File 30.1 KB