The water-soluble micronutrients vitamin B12 (cobalamin) and folate are necessary for the rapid growth and development during the early years of life(Reference Obeid and Herrmann1). The relation between these micronutrients and cognitive and motor development is not fully understood, but the shared metabolism suggests that the status mutually affect the metabolism of the other(Reference Black2). Both are essential cofactors for RNA and DNA synthesis and required for development of and maintaining the nervous system. Vitamin B12 participate in the folate-dependent conversion of homocysteine to methionine and the conversion of methylmalonyl CoA to succinyl CoA(Reference Allen, Miller and de Groot3). Deficiency leads to increased homocysteine levels and reduced supply of methyl groups and succinyl CoA that affects the production of myelin. Myelin is an essential component of brain development and maturation and is related to psychomotor development. Thus, vitamin B12 deficiency may lead to disturbed myelin structure and demyelination of the central nervous system which may affect the process of learning and achieving motor abilities(Reference Black2,Reference Kvestad, Taneja and Kumar4,Reference Dror and Allen5) .
Vitamin B12 is synthesised by micro-organisms and the primary sources of vitamin B12 are animal source foods. Fruit and vegetables are key sources for folate(Reference Solvik, Strand and Kvestad6). In low- and middle-income regions with low intake of meat, milk and fish, poor vitamin B12 status is widespread, especially among pregnant women and young children(Reference Taneja, Bhandari and Strand7,Reference Finkelstein, Layden and Stover8) . Deficiency is less common among children with a typical Western diet, but vegetarians and especially vegans may be at risk(Reference Pawlak, Lester and Babatunde9,Reference Venkatramanan, Armata and Strupp10) . In future, this might be an issue to consider due to the increasing interest in sustainable food consumption in high-income countries. This will probably increase the intake of plant-based foods with a concurrent reduction of animal foods(Reference Aschemann-Witzel, Gantriis and Fraga11). Furthermore, the complementary feeding period with the transition from breast milk to the family diet may affect vitamin B12 status. During this period, the need for especially vitamin B12 is high, as the content in human breast milk and the child’s own depots are sparse(Reference Black2,Reference Greibe, Lildballe and Streym12) .
Few studies have focused on the impact of vitamin B12 status on psychomotor development among well-nourished children in high-income countries(Reference Louwman, van Dusseldorp and van de Vijver13–Reference Torsvik, Ueland and Markestad15) and not among representative healthy children during the first years of life. In low- and middle-income countries, the association between vitamin B12 status and cognitive function has been assessed in more studies(Reference Kvestad, Taneja and Kumar4,Reference Venkatramanan, Armata and Strupp10,Reference Strand, Taneja and Ueland16–Reference Sheng, Wang and Li19) . This study aims to address the associations between vitamin B12 and folate status in infancy and early childhood and cognitive and motor development in early childhood among healthy well-nourished children. We hypothesised that plasma vitamin B12 and folate concentrations would be positively associated with psychomotor development during the first 3 years of life among healthy children. Furthermore, the associations between intake of animal source foods and vitamin B12 and psychomotor development were examined as well.
Methods
Study design and participants
The present study used samples from the two comparable prospective observational cohorts SKOT I and SKOT-II. In SKOT-I, infants were randomly selected from the Copenhagen area using the National Danish Civil Registry(Reference Madsen, Schack-Nielsen and Larnkjaer20), whereas in SKOT-II, infants were born to obese mother participating in the intervention study ‘Treatment of obese pregnant women’ (TOP) at Hvidovre Hospital(Reference Renault, Nørgaard and Nilas21). The cohorts followed the same protocol from 9 months making it possible to pool data(Reference Andersen, Pipper and Trolle22). Both cohorts have been described in detail previously(Reference Madsen, Schack-Nielsen and Larnkjaer20,Reference Andersen, Pipper and Trolle22–Reference Engel, Tronhjem and Hellgren25) . Briefly, inclusion criteria for both cohorts were healthy singleton full-term infants aged 9 months ± 2 weeks at the first examination and Danish speaking parents. The infants were 36 ± 3 months when they were monitored at the third and last examination. At 9 months, 311 and 166 infants participated in the SKOT-I and SKOT-II cohorts, respectively, whereas at 36 months, the numbers were 266 and 130, respectively. Data collection was carried out from 2007 to 2010 for SKOT-I and from 2011 to 2014 for SKOT-II. Written informed consent was obtained from all parents and legal guardians of the children. SKOT-I (H-KF-2007-0003) and SKOT-II (H-3-2010-122) were approved by The Committees on Biomedical Research Ethics for the Capital Region of Denmark.
Examinations at 9 and 36 months
The examinations were performed at the Department of Nutrition, Exercise and Sports, University of Copenhagen, Denmark, as described in detail previously(Reference Andersen, Pipper and Trolle22,Reference Ejlerskov, Larnkjaer and Pedersen23,Reference Laursen, Andersen and Michaelsen26) . Length at 9 months and height at 36 months were calculated as the mean of three measurements and BMI as weight/length2 or weight/height2, respectively. Z-scores for weight, length, height and BMI were calculated using WHO growth standards as a reference and the WHO Anthro software(27). From background questionnaires and interviews, information regarding breast-feeding was obtained, and full breast-feeding was defined as only receiving breast milk, vitamins and water. Information regarding mothers’ education was also obtained and categorised as basic, short, medium or long.
Venous blood samples of 5 ml were taken after approximately 2 h fasting in lithium-heparin test tubes. Blood sampling was not successful in all children, so at 9 and 36 months, 401 and 331 blood samples were available, respectively. After preparation, blood samples were stored at −80°C. Vitamin B12 and folate were analysed on an Immulite 2000 Xpi System analyser (Siemens Healthcare Diagnostics) and determined by competitive immunoassays using Immulite®2000 vitamin B12 and Immulite®2000 Folic Acid kits with intra- and interassay CV of 4·6 and 9·6% for vitamin B12 and 4·5 and 5·1 % for folate, respectively. Due to limited plasma volume, some samples had to be diluted twice for analyses, which led to a higher limit of detection for vitamin B12 of 221 pmol/l for these samples. Samples below this value were coded as 111 pmol/l. Plasma ferritin was determined on an Immulite 1000 analyzer (Siemens Medical Solutions Diagnostics) using the chemiluminescent immunometric assay Immulite®Ferritin kit (Diagnostic Products Corporation) with an intra- and interassay CV of 3·4 and 3·7 %, respectively.
The definition of vitamin B12 deficiency was set to <148 pmol/l(Reference Allen, Miller and de Groot3) and low vitamin B12 status to <300 pmol/l(28). Low folate status was defined as <10 nml/l(Reference Bailey, Stover and McNulty29).
Diet
Dietary intake was recorded using a validated pre-coded food diary(Reference Gondolf, Tetens and Hills30). Parents and caregivers recorded for 7 d except for SKOT-II at 36 months where 4 d recording was applied. Household measures and photo booklet for estimation of portion sizes were used as described elsewhere(Reference Gondolf, Tetens and Hills30). Intake of energy and food items for each child was calculated using the software system GIES (version 1.000d; The National Food Institute). Intake of dairy products was calculated as the combined intake of milk, formula, other milk products and cheese. Human milk was not included, as the intake was not measured. Intake of meat products was calculated as the combined intake of meat, poultry and fish.
Assessment of psychomotor development
At 36 months, motor and cognitive development was assessed by the parents using the Ages and Stages Questionnaire, 3rd edition (ASQ-3) as described previously(Reference Andersen, Harsløf and Schnurr24,Reference Engel, Tronhjem and Hellgren25) . Briefly, the parents were instructed by the project staff at the visit how to complete the questionnaire at home. It was recommended that the parents filled it out when the child was fed and rested. ASQ-3 is a standardised developmental checklist, which for 36 months old children consists of thirty age-appropriate questions divided into five subscale categories: communication, gross motor, fine motor, problem-solving and personal-social skills. The total ASQ-3 score was calculated by adding the scores for the subscales using only cases with completed questionnaires for all subscales. The possible score range for each subscale was 0–60 and hence the range for total ASQ-3 was 0–300.
Statistics
Characteristics are given as means and standard deviations or medians and interquartile ranges (IQR) for continuous variables, and for categorical variables as counts and percentages. Comparison between sex, cohorts or multivitamin supplementation was performed by independent t test or Mann–Whitney for parametric and non-parametric variables. Bivariate correlations between values at 9 and 36 months for vitamin B12 and folate were conducted using Pearson’s correlation coefficient.
Associations between vitamin B12, folate and ASQ-3 were assessed by multiple linear regression analysis using general linear models (GLM). Vitamin B12 and folate at 9 and 36 months were exposures and total ASQ-3 and subscale scores were outcomes. The adjusted models included sex and age at ASQ-3 assessment. Fully adjusted models included adjustment for cohort, maternal education level and vitamin B12 or folate value at 9 months for the corresponding 36 months analyses. In addition, models with vitamin B12 were adjusted for ferritin and models with folate for BMI at 36 months as vitamin B12 was correlated with ferritin and folate with BMI. One model also examined the interaction between sex and vitamin B12 including a sex × vitamin B12 term, which was removed if not significant. Standardised effect estimates were calculated to compare estimates between the different exposures and outcomes. Vitamin B12 quartiles were applied in GLM models with vitamin B12 as a categorical variable. As the range for ASQ variables was rather narrow and often left skewed, logistic regression models were also accomplished to assess consistency. The ASQ-3 score variables were categorised at the 25th percentile (≤ 25th percentile v. >25th percentile) and logistic regression analyses were performed using the same adjustments as for GLM.
The associations between dietary intake and vitamin B12 and total ASQ-3, respectively, were investigated using GLM. Crude and models adjusted for sex, age and cohort were performed. At 9 months, many infants were still breastfed, which may have affected the milk intake. As the intake of human milk was not measured, the analyses of intake of dairy products at 9 months included a model with adjustment for current breast-feeding status as well. At 36 months, adjustment included intake of multivitamin supplements, which may affect plasma vitamin B12 concentration. Difference in intake of dairy or meat products across vitamin B12 quartiles was assessed by ANOVA.
Model assumptions for regression models and impact of outliers on estimates and significance were checked by residual plots and Cook’s distance, respectively. P values <0·05 were considered significant, whereas P values 0·05 to <0·10 as trends. All analyses were performed using IBM SPSS statistics (version 27.0; IBM).
Results
Of the 477 children included in the SKOT cohorts, 280 children had complete ASQ-3 and valid plasma vitamin B12 measurements at 36 months. As these variables were of major interest, they defined the sample size. Of these, 278 children had folate measurements at 36 months, and at 9 months, 221 and 226 children had vitamin B12 and folate measurements, respectively (online Supplementary Fig. 1). There was no significant difference in characteristics between children with or without ASQ-3 and vitamin B12 measurements except that children included in the analyses were about 1 week younger at the 36 months examination than those not included (mean age (sd): 36·4 (1·02) months v. 36·7 (1·03) months, P = 0·028).
The characteristics of the children are shown in Table 1. No children had vitamin B12 deficiency (<148 pmol/l) at 36 months. The number of children with vitamin B12 deficiency at 9 months was not possible to evaluate due to dilution of samples with limited amount of material resulting in a higher detection limit of 221 pmol/l. Eight (4 %) children had concentrations below this value. One undiluted sample was <148 pmol (138 pmol/l). There were 37 (17 %) and 6 (2 %) children with low vitamin B12 status (<300 pmol/l) at 9 and 36 months, respectively. Furthermore, no children had low folate status (<10 nmol/l) at 9 months and 2 (1 %) had at 36 months. The median (IQR) for total ASQ-3 was 270 (250–280), whereas medians for subscales ranged between 50 (40–60) and 60 (55–60). Girls had higher total ASQ-3 score compared with boys (275 (255–285) v. 260 (245–275); P ≤ 0·001), but there was no sex difference regarding vitamin B12 or folate status (P ≥ 0·16). For vitamin B12, there was a correlation between values at 9 and 36 months (r = 0·408, P ≤ 0·001) but not for folate (r = 0·052, P = 0·44).
ASQ-3, Ages and Stages Questionnaire, 3rd edition.
* Values are expressed as mean and standard deviation (sd); median and interquartile range (IQR); n and %, as appropriate.
Vitamin B12 and ASQ-3
Linear regression analyses showed that vitamin B12 at 36 months was positively associated with total ASQ-3 score corresponding to an increase of 100 pmol/l vitamin B12 per 1·5 increase in total ASQ-3 score (Table 2). Thus to obtain an increase of about 1 sd in total ASQ-3 score, an increase of approximately 2000 pmol/l vitamin B12 would be needed. Adjustment attenuated the significance, but the associations remained significant also after adjustment for vitamin B12 at 9 months, which was not significant in any of the models. Although girls had higher total ASQ-3 score than boys, there was no interaction between sex and vitamin B12 (P = 0·12).
ASQ-3, Ages and Stages Questionnaire, 3rd edition.
* Values are β-coefficients, 95 % CI, P-values.
† Model 1 is adjusted for sex and age at 36 months.
‡ Model 2 is adjusted for sex, age at 36 months, cohort, maternal education and value at 9 months for analyses at 36 months. Furthermore, vitamin B12 analyses were adjusted for ferritin and folate analyses for BMI at 36 months.
§ n 221 and 224 for vitamin B12 and folate both at 36 months, respectively.
In the ASQ-3 subscale analyses, vitamin B12 at 36 months was significantly positively associated with problem-solving score and it remained significant after adjustment. Further, there was a positive trend for an association with the subscales communication and personal-social scores for vitamin B12 at 36 months in the crude model, which became significant or vanished after full adjustment, respectively. Standardised effect estimates showed comparable estimates of the total ASQ-3 score and significant subscale scores (standardised estimates (CI)); total ASQ-3:0·17 (0·006; 0·33), problem-solving: 0·18 (0·011; 0·35) and communication: 0·19 (0·027; 0·35), fully adjusted models. There were no associations with the personal-social, gross or fine motor subscales.
Vitamin B12 at 9 month was not associated with total ASQ-3 scores or subscale scores except for a positive association with personal-social score in the crude model.
To further investigate the impact of a low vitamin B12 status at 36 months on total ASQ-3 score, vitamin B12 was divided into quartiles in the GLM analyses. Overall, there was a trend for a difference between vitamin B12 quartiles in relation to ASQ-scores (P = 0·072). Pairwise comparisons showed that the lowest vitamin B12 quartile had lower ASQ score compared with the other quartiles ((first (lowest) v. second quartile: P = 0·036; first v. third quartile: P = 0·039 and first v. fourth (highest) quartile, P = 0·022)) (Fig. 1). When adjusting for sex and age, the overall trend for a difference between vitamin B12 quartiles vanished (P = 0·160), but total ASQ-3 score in the lowest vitamin B12 quartile was still lower compared with the highest quartiles (P = 0·036). Equivalent results were obtained after full adjustment.
Logistic models for the total ASQ-3 scores using total ASQ-3 below or above the 25th percentile as outcome showed similar results. Per 100 pmol/l increase in vitamin B12 concentration, the odds of not being in the lower quartile of total ASQ-3 score was 1·24 ((95 % CI: 1·07, 1·43), P = 0·004). Adjustment did not change the associations significantly (OR = 1·28 (95 % CI 1·05, 1·56), P = 0·016 for the full adjusted model). Contrary to the linear regression analyses, there were no associations between vitamin B12 at 36 months and the subscales scores though a trend was seen for fine motor subscale in the fully adjusted model (OR = 1·21 (95 % CI 0·99, 1·47) for not being in the lower total ASQ-3 quartile per 100 pmol/l increase in vitamin B12, P = 0·061). Vitamin B12 at 9 months showed no associations with total ASQ-3 scales or any of the subscales.
Folate and ASQ-3
Folate was not associated with total ASQ-3 score or subscales at any time points in any of the models except for an association between folate at 9 months and communication subscale in the fully adjusted logistic regression model (OR = 1·02 (95 % CI 1·00, 1·03)) for not being in the lower total ASQ-3 quartile, per nmol/l increase in folate, P = 0·040).
Dietary intake and vitamin B12
At 9 and 36 months, dietary intake was available for 272 and 262 children, respectively. The energy intake and intake of milk- and meat-related products are shown in Table 3. At 9 months, no infants were fully breastfed, while almost half of the children were still breastfed (47 %) (Table 1). Multivitamin supplements were only given to one child (0·4 %) at 9 month, whereas 226 children (81%) had received multivitamin supplements at 36 months. The vitamin B12 concentration was significantly higher in the children receiving multivitamin supplement than those not receiving supplement (680 (250) pmol/l v. 540 (141) pmol/l, P ≤ 0·001.)
At both 9 and 36 months, the intake of cheese was minimal compared with the milk intake, which included formula (Table 3). Likewise was the intake of poultry and fish low compared with the intake of meat. However, to investigate the associations between dietary intake and serum vitamin B12 concentration, dairy products included all relevant milk products and meat products included all meat-related products. Intake of dairy products was positively associated with current vitamin B12 concentration at 36 months. A 100 g increase in dairy products per d was associated with 30 pmol/l higher vitamin B12 concentration ((95 % CI 0·11, 0·48), P = 0·002) (online Supplementary Table 1), equivalent to an increase of about 5 %. Adjustment for sex, age, intake of multivitamin supplements and cohort did not change the result significantly (P = 0·003). At 9 months, there was a trend for an association (β (95 % CI): 0·16 pmol/l per g/d (-0·02, 0·33), P = 0·082), but adjustment attenuated the association (P = 0·13) also when adjusting for breast-feeding status at 9 months (P = 0·18). Intake of meat products was not associated with vitamin B12 at any time point (P ≥ 0·57). The intake of milk products across the vitamin B12 quartiles was different (P = 0·011). The lowest vitamin B12 quartile had lower intake of milk products than third and the fourth quartile ((mean (sd); P) 348 (141) g/d v. 421 (156) g/d, P = 0·007 and 348 (141) g/d v. 424 (140) g/d, P = 0·005, respectively). The intake of meat products did not differ across the vitamin B12 quartiles (P = 0·64).
Dietary intake and ASQ
None of the diet variables were associated with total ASQ-3 at any time point (P ≥ 0·11). However, duration of full breast-feeding and breast-feeding status at 9 months were associated with total ASQ-3 score (P = 0·021, P = 0·033, respectively), but it disappeared after adjustment for sex, age, cohort and maternal education (P ≥ 0·11).
Discussion
In this study, including healthy Danish children with adequate vitamin B12 concentrations, we found that plasma vitamin B12 was positively associated with psychomotor development measured as total ASQ-3 at 36 month also after adjustment for essential confounders. The relation was consistent using logistic regression or vitamin B12 concentrations categorised into quartiles. Furthermore, the total ASQ-3 score for children in the lowest vitamin B12 quartile was about 10 scores lower compared the other quartiles. This could indicate that even though these children were not vitamin B12-deficient, attention should be paid to children with low vitamin B12 concentration. Though the effect size was relatively small, it is interesting that a positive association could be observed in this sample of well-nourished children which has not been reported previously. For the ASQ-3 subscales, positive associations were observed for the communication and problem-solving scores in the linear regression analyses but not in the logistic regression analyses. This may be due to the low range and variability of the responses and risk of ceiling effect in the subscales.
Although there was a highly significant correlation between vitamin B12 at 9 and 36 months, vitamin B12 status at 9 months was not associated with ASQ-3 at 36 months. Hence, in this study, only concurrent B12 values were associated with psychomotor development though tracking from 9 months cannot be excluded.
Several studies have investigated the association between vitamin B12 status and psychomotor development in children and adolescents but mainly in low- and middle-income countries(Reference Kvestad, Taneja and Kumar4,Reference Venkatramanan, Armata and Strupp10,Reference Strand, Taneja and Ueland16–Reference Sheng, Wang and Li19) or in special populations with risk of low vitamin B12 status due to diet or medical issues in high-income countries(Reference Louwman, van Dusseldorp and van de Vijver13–Reference Torsvik, Ueland and Markestad15). In line with our results, a higher vitamin B12 status was associated with improved cognitive and motoric development in many studies, but studies have also shown no associations(Reference Strand, Ulak and Hysing18,Reference Nguyen, Gracely and Lee31) . A Dutch observational study measured cognitive functions in adolescents who had been on a macrobiotic diet, which is close to a vegan diet and low in vitamin B12, for the first 6 years of life(Reference Dagnelie and van Staveren32). In early childhood, the children had reduced plasma vitamin B12 concentration, impaired growth and psychomotor development compared with children on omnivorous diets. Follow-up examinations in adolescence revealed that these children underperformed in cognitive tests compared with adolescents fed on an omnivorous diet(Reference Louwman, van Dusseldorp and van de Vijver13). In a larger observational study from the USA (NHANES III), plasma vitamin B12 was not associated with cognitive tests scores in children aged 6–16 years(Reference Nguyen, Gracely and Lee31). Compared with our study the children were older, the age range broader and tests for cognitive function were different, which may contribute to the different results.
Two Norwegian randomised studies investigated the effect of vitamin B12 injection to high-risk groups with impaired vitamin B12 function in infancy(Reference Torsvik, Ueland and Markestad14,Reference Torsvik, Ueland and Markestad15) . After 1 month, infants treated with vitamin B12 showed improved motor function. In our study, we did not observe any associations of motor development ASQ-3 subscales and vitamin B12, but the difference in age might be important as associations between nutrition and motor development might be difficult to observe in later childhood(Reference Kvestad, Hysing and Shrestha17).
In a Nepalese observational study, vitamin B12 status in infancy (2–12 months) was associated with cognitive development 5 years later(Reference Kvestad, Hysing and Shrestha17). ASQ-3 scores were assessed and in accordance with our results, the strongest association was found for total ASQ-3 score but also associations for problem-solving scores were found. In contrast, we did not find any long-term association between vitamin B12 in infancy at 9 months and ASQ-3 scores at 36 months. Contrary to our study, different assessments of vitamin B12 status were applied and Kvested et al. found most associations for other markers, that is, total homocysteine and methylmalonic acid, than total vitamin B12 concentration. Total homocysteine and methylmalonic acid have been recognised as sensitive markers of low levels and mild deficiency reflecting status for metabolic function(Reference Allen, Miller and de Groot3,Reference Bjørke Monsen and Ueland33) . In infancy, differences in breast-feeding might influence the vitamin B12 status as some studies have showed lower vitamin B12 status in breastfed infants(Reference Taneja, Bhandari and Strand7,Reference Kvestad, Hysing and Shrestha17,Reference Bjørke-Monsen, Torsvik and Saetran34,Reference Hay, Johnston and Whitelaw35) . Studies from populations with a low vitamin B12 status have shown associations between vitamin B12 concentration and cognitive development among 12–18 months old children(Reference Strand, Taneja and Ueland16,Reference Sheng, Wang and Li19) .
In addition, a few randomised trials have investigated the effect of vitamin B12 on psychomotor development showing inconsistent results. A group of 6–30 months old children receiving vitamin B12 supplementation for 6 months showed improved gross motor scores compared with placebo but, contrary to our study, there was no effect on total ASQ-3(Reference Kvestad, Taneja and Kumar4). However, in the same study, where one of the four randomised groups received both folate and vitamin B12, an effect was observed for gross motor and problem-solving and a trend for total ASQ-3 compared with placebo(Reference Kvestad, Taneja and Kumar4). Contrary, no effect on psychomotor development of vitamin B12 supplementation of 6–11 months old infants for 1 year was found in a Nepalese study(Reference Strand, Ulak and Hysing18). However, the study group consisted of mildly stunted children who might suffer from insufficient levels of other nutrients and the highly selected group might reduce the generalisability of the results to populations with lower prevalence of micronutrient deficiencies.
Generally, the studies are challenging to compare as they vary in study design, motor and cognitive development tests, B12 status assessment, age, sample size and setting of participants. Different specific developmental domains vary between tests and may not be directly comparable. In addition, the rapid development of the brain with different timing of regional brain growth spurts complicate comparison of results on developmental domains across ages.
We found no associations between folate and total ASQ-3 score. The association in the fully adjusted logistic model for the communication subscale might be a chance finding as no associations were noticed for the other models or for total ASQ-3, which has a broader range and variability, which increase the ability to detect associations as observed for the vitamin B12 analyses. In the literature, divergent results have been reported. In the study by Kvestad et al., supplementation with folate alone did not improve total ASQ-3 and subscale scores(Reference Kvestad, Taneja and Kumar4). However, a positive association between folate and cognitive scores in a cross-sectional study in 6–16 years old children from the USA was observed(Reference Nguyen, Gracely and Lee31) as well as in 12–18 months old Indian children provided that their vitamin B12 concentration was above the 25th percentile(Reference Strand, Taneja and Ueland16).
We found a positive association between the intake of dairy products and vitamin B12 at 36 months but no association with meat intake. Though the intake of dairy products in the lowest vitamin B12 quartile was around the recommended intake of 350 g/d(36), it was still possible to observe a difference in intake across the B12 quartiles. The association was independent of intake of multivitamin supplements. This may indicate that dairy products especially milk, which constituted the main part of the dairy intake, probably is the key contributor to vitamin B12 intake. This also conforms to the higher intake of dairy products compared with intake of meat products. At 9 months, many infants were still breastfed, which may underlie the missing association between milk intake and vitamin B12 at this age. In many studies, animal source food and plasma vitamin B12 concentrations were positively correlated(Reference Allen, Miller and de Groot3,Reference Vogiatzoglou, Smith and Nurk37–Reference Hay, Trygg and Whitelaw39) , but in a study among healthy Norwegian 4–6 years old children, no associations between dietary intake and biomarkers for vitamin B12 were found(Reference Solvik, Strand and Kvestad6). In accordance with our results, dairy products but not meat products were correlated with plasma vitamin B12 concentration among 2 years old Norwegian children(Reference Hay, Trygg and Whitelaw39). The difference in results between dairy products and meat products may be that dairy products are the principal source of vitamin B12 and that meat intake is low at this age. Furthermore, the bioavailability of vitamin B12 from dairy products might also be higher as previously suggested in a study in adults(Reference Vogiatzoglou, Smith and Nurk37).
A strength of the study is that the SKOT cohorts are healthy well-nourished children with detailed information on cognitive development and dietary intake of the first years of life. A limitation of the study is that only plasma vitamin B12 (cobalamin) was measured as biomarker of vitamin B12 status due to limited amount of material. It reflects long-term status and is not affected by recent intake. Additional biomarkers would have increased the sensitivity and specificity of vitamin B12 status and increased the validity of the results. Another limitation is the reduced sample size as not all children had complete ASQ-3 assessment or blood samples. Nevertheless, as the only difference in baseline characteristics between children with or without valid B12 measurements and total ASQ-3 at 36 months was a difference in age at 36 months of 7 d. It is unlikely that this has affected the results. Furthermore, we did not have measurement of intake of human milk at 9 months. There is a risk of residual confounding as this is an observational study and no causative conclusions can be made as well as associations should be interpreted with carefulness. We did not adjust for multiplicity as this was an explorative study and the risk of chance findings may occur. The results should therefore be confirmed in other studies preferentially including other biomarkers for vitamin B12 status.
In summary, this study indicates that even in well-nourished children, a low vitamin B12 concentration is associated with a lower score for psychomotor development. The growing concern for sustainable food production might increase the interest for plant-based diet and reduced intake of animal source foods, which may compromise the vitamin B12 status. The current study indicates that special attention should be paid to the association between vitamin B12 status and psychomotor development. Future studies should address how a sufficient vitamin B12 status can be achieved, so optimal psychomotor development is secured.
Acknowledgements
We thank the participating children, families and project staff of the SKOT cohorts, and Vivian Anker and Inge Rasmussen for technical assistance.
This work was supported by grants from Arla Foods amba, and the Milk Levy Fund. The SKOT-I study was funded by The Directorate for Food, Fisheries and Agri Business. The SKOT-II study was supported by grants from the Aase and Ejnar Danielsens Foundation, the Augustinus foundation and by contributions from the research programme ‘Governing Obesity’ by the University of Copenhagen Excellence Program for Interdisciplinary Research (www.go.ku.dk). The funders had no role in the design, analysis or writing of the article.
The authors’ contributions were as follows: M. V. L., K. F. M. and C. M. conceptualised the research and were responsible for funding acquisition; A. L., S. H. C., M. V. L., C. M. and K. F. M. designed the study; M. V. L. conducted the research; A. L. and M. V. L. analysed the data and performed the statistical analyses; A. L. wrote the original draft of manuscript. All authors contributed to the interpretation of the result, commented on the drafts and approved the final version of the manuscript.
The authors (A. L., S. H. C. and M. V. L) have no conflicts of interest to disclose. C. M. and K. F. M. has also received grants from Arla Food for Health and Danish Dairy Research Foundation to other research projects.
Supplementary material
For supplementary material/s referred to in this article, please visit https://doi.org/10.1017/S0007114521004888