Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-g7gxr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-10T05:49:14.223Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The growing interdisciplinarity of developmental psychopathology: Implications for science and training

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 March 2024

Ian H. Gotlib*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Stanford University, Stanford, CA, USA
Jessica L. Buthmann
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Stanford University, Stanford, CA, USA
Jessica P. Uy
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Stanford University, Stanford, CA, USA
*
Corresponding author: I. H. Gotlib; Email: ian.gotlib@stanford.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

The field of developmental psychopathology has grown exponentially over the past decades, and has become increasingly multifaceted. The initial focus on understanding abnormal child psychology has broadened to the study of the origins of psychopathology, with the goals of preventing and alleviating disorder and promoting healthy development. In this paper, we discuss how technological advances and global events have expanded the questions that researchers in developmental psychopathology can address. We do so by describing a longitudinal study that we have been conducting for the past dozen years. We originally planned to examine the effects of early adversity on trajectories of brain development, endocrine function, and depressive symptoms across puberty; it has since become an interdisciplinary study encompassing diverse domains like inflammation, sleep, biological aging, the environment, and child functioning post-pandemic, that we believe will advance our understanding of neurobehavioral development. This increase in the breadth in our study emerged from an expansion of the field; we encourage researchers to embrace these dynamic changes. In this context, we discuss challenges, opportunities, and institutional changes related to the growing interdisciplinarity of the field with respect to training the next generation of investigators to mitigate the burden of mental illness in youth.

Type
Special Issue Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press

Fifty years ago, Achenbach (Reference Achenbach1974) described developmental psychopathology as a field that “hardly exists yet” (p. 3). Indeed, if the term “developmental psychopathology” was used at all, it was synonymous with Abnormal Child Psychology. Recognizing that this equivalence was likely to limit research in developmental psychopathology to the study of children, Sroufe and Rutter (Reference Sroufe and Rutter1984) offered a definition of developmental psychopathology as “the study of the origins and course of individual patterns of behavioral maladaptation, whatever the age of onset, whatever the causes, whatever the transformations in behavioral manifestation, and however complex the course of the developmental pattern may be” (p. 18). Sroufe and Rutter noted further that the focus of developmental psychopathology is on understanding processes that underlie both continuity and change in patterns of adaptation. Just over 20 years later, Masten (Reference Masten2006) extended Sroufe and Rutter’s definition, adding that a goal of developmental psychopathology is “to prevent or reduce psychopathology and alleviate the burden of suffering it brings to individuals, families, and communities, while at the same time promoting healthy behavior and development” (p. 52).

To be sure, we have made significant progress in developmental psychopathology since Achenbach’s (Reference Achenbach1974) bleak assessment of the field; indeed, researchers have worked diligently to address Masten’s (Reference Masten2006) objectives concerning prevention and promotion. Importantly, however, as in other areas of science, advances in technology and data analysis over the past two decades have expanded the questions and the range of issues researchers are able to address in the field of developmental psychopathology. In particular, advances in genetics and neuroimaging, in multivariate statistical and analytic techniques, in diagnostic approaches, including new versions of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual and new developments involving the National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH)’s Research Domain Criteria (RDoC), combined with the recent availability of large datasets that allow researchers to take a ‘big-data’ approach to address critical issues in developmental psychopathology, have informed (and have broadened) both the questions that investigators can ask and the methods they can use to address these new questions. In addition, recent events such as the COVID-19 pandemic and a renewed focus on climate-related issues have also led researchers in developmental psychopathology to raise and address novel questions.

At the same time that these advances and events have provided the impetus for investigators to pursue new research directions within developmental psychopathology, it is important to recognize that they also have significant implications for training in this field. Specifically, the stronger interdisciplinary focus in developmental psychopathology driven by this movement means that, as Masten (Reference Masten2006) foreshadowed, individual investigators are unlikely to be able to master the extensive background, approaches, methods, and analytic techniques that are becoming increasingly important in the integrative study of development across both disciplines and diverse domains of functioning, or to fully apply the obtained knowledge to practice and policy.

Our goal in this article is to articulate what we believe are important issues in developmental psychopathology; we do so in the context of describing the multidisciplinary evolution of a project that we have been conducting in our laboratory over the past ten years. We initially designed this project to examine the effects of early life stress (ELS) on trajectories of brain function/structure and of clinical symptoms in boys and girls over adolescence. Since beginning to conduct this study, however, we broadened our focus to address important questions, issues, and opportunities that arose and captured our interest mid-study. We describe these specific extensions to the initial aims of the project in greater detail below, discussing the impetus for these additions and presenting our belief that it is critical to the development of this field that researchers be open to leveraging and extending existing data to address emerging scientific questions.

We begin by describing our longitudinal study of the effects of ELS on psychobiological functioning across adolescence, briefly presenting the rationale and justification for conducting this investigation. We use this description of our project as a foundation for contextualizing important goals, issues, and methodological approaches in the field of developmental psychopathology, including the study of individuals through sensitive developmental periods, the use of longitudinal designs with repeated assessments to assess developmental changes and the emergence of psychopathology, and the integrative multi-domain examination of mechanisms and risk and protective factors for disorder across adolescence, in the service of generating and testing hypotheses relevant to human development. We discuss the ways in which this study has allowed us to examine a number of distinct questions in developmental psychopathology and, perhaps more importantly for this article, how we leveraged the longitudinal data we had collected to address new questions that emerged during the course of our study as relevant data became available in the public domain and as both the COVID-19 pandemic and a renewed focus on climate-related issues became salient; we describe how we responded to these events and issues by expanding the scope of our investigation in what we believe are important new directions. Finally, we conclude by describing challenges presented by the increasingly interdisciplinary nature of the field of developmental psychopathology for both mentors and mentees.

Our ELS project

About ten years ago, in 2013, we initiated a longitudinal study designed to examine the effects of ELS on trajectories of brain structure, function, and connectivity, and their possible mediation of the association of ELS with depressive symptoms, in boys and girls over the transition through puberty. A major impetus for this study was the increasing prevalence of both early adversity and depressive symptomatology in adolescents over the past three decades. Although researchers had demonstrated that ELS is a significant risk factor for the development of a range of psychiatric symptoms in adolescents (LeMoult et al., Reference LeMoult, Humphreys, King, Colich, Price, Ordaz and Gotlib2019; McLaughlin et al., Reference McLaughlin, Greif Green, Gruber, Sampson, Zaslavsky and Kessler2012), the mechanisms through which early adversity confers risk for psychopathology were (and still are) not well understood. Investigators also were unable to adequately explain the reasons underlying the well-documented sex difference in rates of depression during adolescence (Breslau et al., Reference Breslau, Gilman, Stein, Ruder, Gmelin and Miller2017). At the time we were designing our study, there were small but growing literatures indicating that ELS affects not only brain structure and function, particularly in regions involved in the generation and regulation of affect (Gee et al., Reference Gee, Gabard-Durnam, Flannery, Goff, Humphreys, Telzer, Hare, Bookheimer and Tottenham2013; Goff & Tottenham, Reference Goff and Tottenham2015), but also neurobiological responses to stress (Hanson et al., Reference Hanson, Albert, Iselin, Carré, Dodge and Hariri2016; Lupien et al., Reference Lupien, McEwen, Gunnar and Heim2009). Given these findings, we posited that adverse experiences in childhood contribute to vulnerability for the development of mental health difficulties in adolescence by modifying neural pathways implicated in emotional functioning and stress responsivity and, further, that ELS has different effects on trajectories of brain development and subsequent psychopathology in girls than in boys. Finally, because we wrote the initial grant proposal for this study in response to a call for studies to assess the NIMH’s Research Domain Criteria (RDoC), we focused on the RDoC constructs of Responsiveness to Reward and Responses to Sustained Threat, examining how ELS affects brain circuitry and endocrine functioning implicated in these constructs (Insel et al., Reference Insel, Cuthbert, Garvey, Heinssen, Pine, Quinn, Sanislow and Wang2010).

For this project we recruited a sample of 220 boys and girls in early puberty, between the ages of 9 and 13 years. We matched boys and girls on pubertal stage, which meant that boys were slightly older than girls at entry to the study. We proposed to assess the effects of ELS on brain function, structure, and connectivity, endocrine functioning, information-processing biases, and clinical symptoms relevant to the RDoC Reward and Threat constructs – initially at two timepoints separated by two years, then, in a renewal of our grant, at two additional timepoints, and finally, in a subsequent grant renewal, at a fifth timepoint when the participants reached 20 years of age. An overview of the constructs we proposed to assess in the original study is presented in Figure 1.

Figure 1. Constructs originally assessed in our early life stress study.

We have published several papers describing findings from this project that address the initial goals of our study. For example, in an early paper from this project Humphreys et al. (Reference Humphreys, Kircanski, Colich and Gotlib2016) reported that greater exposure to ELS was associated with attentional biases away from fearful facial expressions, which mediated the relation between ELS and poorer social functioning. In subsequent papers we described the adverse effects of ELS on endocrine functioning, including both stress-related and diurnal patterns of cortisol secretion (e.g., King et al., Reference King, Colich, LeMoult, Humphreys, Ordaz, Price and Gotlib2017; Kircanski et al., Reference Kircanski, Sisk, Ho, Humphreys, King, Colich, Ordaz and Gotlib2019), on neural function (e.g., Colich et al., Reference Colich, Williams, Ho, King, Humphreys, Price, Ordaz and Gotlib2017), structure (e.g., Ho et al., Reference Ho, King, Leong, Colich, Humphreys, Ordaz and Gotlib2017; Humphreys et al., Reference Humphreys, King, Sacchet, Camacho, Colich, Ordaz, Ho and Gotlib2019; King et al., Reference King, Dennis, Humphreys, Thompson and Gotlib2020), and connectivity (e.g., Borchers et al., Reference Borchers, Bruckert, Chahal, Mastrovito, Ho and Gotlib2022; Chahal et al., Reference Chahal, Miller, Yuan, Buthmann and Gotlib2022), and on clinical functioning (e.g., LeMoult et al., Reference LeMoult, Humphreys, King, Colich, Price, Ordaz and Gotlib2019; Lee et al., Reference Lee, Chahal and Gotlib2023). In examining the effects of ELS on adolescents’ functioning, we also documented sex differences in associations of ELS, pubertal status, and brain metrics (e.g., Chahal et al., Reference Chahal, Delevich, Kirshenbaum, Borchers, Ho and Gotlib2021; Colich et al., Reference Colich, Williams, Ho, King, Humphreys, Price, Ordaz and Gotlib2017).

In conducting these analyses, we were guided by, and largely followed, the analytic plan that we proposed in the grant to address and test specific questions and hypotheses. In doing so, we have been able to make what we believe are important contributions to our knowledge of how early adversity increases children’s and adolescents’ risk for developing psychopathology by altering stress-related endocrine function and stress- and reward-related brain function, structure, and connectivity; further, we have documented differential effects of sex and of earlier vs. later puberty on the psychobiological functioning of children and adolescents.

In planning and executing our proposed study, we attempted to address and adhere to contemporary principles and aims of developmental psychopathology. Certainly, it is important that researchers generate theory-driven hypotheses concerning issues in developmental psychopathology and design studies that will allow them to conduct the strongest tests of their formulations. Indeed, we followed these directives in designing and conducting our ELS project. At the same time, however, we believe that investigators must be willing and ready to adapt or extend their study to address societal and environmental issues that arise, as well as scientific advances that are reported, as they are conducting their planned research. In the sections below, we describe how we responded to issues, events, statistical advances, and new scientific foci that emerged as we continued to meet the goals of our project that we had originally proposed. More specifically, as novel data began to be reported concerning the effects of biological aging, inflammation, sleep, environmental toxins, and neighborhood socioeconomic disadvantage on adolescents’ functioning, we revised our experimental protocol and procedures to leverage our existing data to take advantage of these advances. For example, we began to obtain blood spots to assess inflammatory markers, administered ecological momentary assessment prompts and used actigraphy to assess sleep metrics, and collected saliva samples to assess aspects of biological aging (e.g., telomere length, DNA methylation) and microbiome composition and functioning. We also located and obtained access to environmental databases so that we could link our participants’ addresses to levels of air and water contaminants, allowing us to examine associations of exposure to environmental toxins with trajectories of both brain development and psychopathology. And as investigators began to report the development and utilization of increasingly sophisticated statistical methods, we revisited the analytic strategies we had proposed to use with our data and started to incorporate these state-of-the-art approaches into our project. Finally, as was the case for virtually all laboratories, our research was interrupted (and in many ways halted) by the COVID-19 pandemic and its associated lockdowns, requiring us to adapt quickly to constraints on conducting in-person human subjects research and to a changing work environment. In Figure 2 we present an overview of constructs we have added to our ELS study since we initiated the project.

Figure 2. Constructs added to our early life stress study.

The increasing interdisciplinarity of developmental psychopathology

Before we describe in greater detail how we adapted our ELS project in response to the occurrences and advances we noted above, we want to comment more broadly on the growing interdisciplinary focus of research in the field of developmental psychopathology. Over the past two decades, developmental psychopathology has become exponentially more interdisciplinary. Investigators are making significant progress in this field by integrating advances in genetics, neuroimaging, and clinical science into their studies of child and adolescent development. Interestingly, this growing interdisciplinarity in developmental psychopathology was predicted fifty years ago by Cicchetti, Rutter, Sroufe, and Masten, all of whom recognized that different disciplines had an interest in various aspects of this field and in addressing different, but relevant, questions. Summarizing these perspectives, Cicchetti and Rogosch (Reference Cicchetti and Rogosch2002) wrote, “Developmental psychopathology is an integrative discipline that seeks to unify, within a developmental, lifespan framework, contributions from multiple fields of inquiry with the goal of understanding psychopathology and its relation to normative adaptation…” (p. 7). These investigators also acknowledged, however, that there had thus far been little communication or interaction among these fields and disciplines, but all expected that this would increasingly become the norm.

It is important to note here that at the time these researchers were predicting a growing interdisciplinarity in developmental psychopathology, some of the scientific fields and areas of inquiry that have become integrated into research in developmental psychopathology had not yet even emerged. At that point, the important integration focused on reconciling perspectives from academic and clinical psychology; now, of course, the number, breadth, and diversity of scientific areas of study that have begun to (and should) be integrated into the field of developmental psychopathology are much greater. For example, it is now commonplace for studies of developmental psychopathology to include measures of neural structure, function, and connectivity, endocrine function, inflammation, and genetics; less common, but increasing in frequency, are investigations that integrate metrics of epigenetics and aging, the microbiome, and the physical environment, including air and water pollution, access to green spaces, and neighborhood crime and disadvantage. In the sections below, we describe why and how we adapted our research to include many of these emerging constructs of interest.

Biology and developmental psychopathology

One of the most significant advances in developmental psychopathology over the past few decades is the integration of biology into this area of research. With advances in technology, incorporating biological markers into psychological research has become more affordable, feasible, and accessible to researchers in the social sciences. There are now growing literatures addressing how early environment and experiences interact with and shape biological aspects of human functioning that, in turn, affect the onset, severity, and persistence of psychopathology across development. These biological factors include genetics and epigenetics, biological aging (commonly indexed by telomere length, mitochondrial DNA, advanced pubertal stage), hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis functioning (typically indexed by cortisol levels – diurnal cortisol, stress-reactivity cortisol, and hair cortisol), brain function, structure, and connectivity, and immune functioning. Foundational research in these areas has demonstrated that the mind and psychological health are not easily dissociable from the body and physical health, and that to understand, prevent, and treat psychopathology it is critical that we appreciate both mental and physical manifestations of diseases and their comorbidities.

In our study we had already proposed to obtain saliva samples to assay both stress-related and diurnal patterns of cortisol secretion (e.g., King et al., Reference King, Colich, LeMoult, Humphreys, Ordaz, Price and Gotlib2017; Kircanski et al., Reference Kircanski, Sisk, Ho, Humphreys, King, Colich, Ordaz and Gotlib2019) as well as sex hormones like dehydroepiandrosterone and testosterone (King et al., Reference King, Graber, Colich and Gotlib2020). Given advances in the study of biological aging in developmental psychopathology, we leveraged these samples to examine both cross-sectional and longitudinal associations between depressive symptoms and indices of biological aging (operationalized as shorter telomere length and greater mitochondrial DNA copy number [mtDNA-cn]). Specifically, we (Humphreys et al., Reference Humphreys, Sisk, Manczak, Lin and Gotlib2020) showed that depressive symptoms at our baseline assessment when participants were, on average, 11.38 years of age, predicted higher rates of telomere erosion and greater increases in mtDNA-cn over a two-year period, over and above the effects of ELS; in contrast, markers of cellular aging at baseline did not predict subsequent changes in depressive symptoms. Thus, early depressive symptoms might affect the rate of cellular aging in children and adolescents. It will be important in future work to examine factors, such as social support, parenting behaviors, and exercise, that may buffer individuals from experiencing accelerated cellular aging, and to test the formulation that preventing depression will have long-term health benefits through reducing rates of telomere erosion and increases in mtDNA-cn. And as we describe below, we extended our study of biological aging to include the participants’ physical environments.

An emerging focus on the physical environment

Contaminants

Just over a decade ago when we were designing our study of the effects of ELS on trajectories of brain development, endocrinology, and clinical symptomatology over puberty, little attention had been paid to the effects of environmental pollutants and contaminants on symptoms of psychopathology, particularly in youth. Most studies of the effects of air and water pollution had focused on the physical health of adults, including cardiovascular disease (Brook et al., Reference Brook, Rajagopalan, Pope, Brook, Bhatnagar, Diez-Roux, Holguin, Hong, Luepker, Mittleman, Peters, Siscovick, Smith, Whitsel and Kaufman2010; Navas-Acien et al., Reference Navas-Acien, Guallar, Silbergeld and Rothenberg2007), asthma (Clougherty et al., Reference Clougherty, Levy, Kubzansky, Ryan, Suglia, Canner and Wright2007), and Alzheimer’s Disease (Moulton & Yang, Reference Moulton and Yang2012). More recently, investigators have extended this focus to examine the effects of environmental toxins on mental health; in fact, the Environmental Protection Agency (EPA, 2023) and American Psychological Association (Clayton et al., Reference Clayton, Manning, Hill and Speiser2023) have now published reports on the specific threat of climate change-related issues (e.g., extreme temperatures, natural disasters, etc.) to the physical and psychological health of developing children. Further, the WHO (2016) concluded that air pollution in particular is a major environmental threat to public health. Many of the health risks of air pollution have been attributed to fine particulate matter (PM2.5; air particles smaller than 2.5 μm in diameter), which may be especially dangerous given its absorption into the bloodstream to reach the brain, and to ozone, a product of the interaction of pollutants with sunlight. In fact, epidemiological studies of adults are now linking exposure to ozone with depression-relevant phenomena, such as criminality (Lu et al., Reference Lu, Lee, Gino and Galinsky2018) and antidepressant use (Zhao et al., Reference Zhao, Markevych, Romanos, Nowak and Heinrich2018), and with suicides and emergency department visits for mental health (Biermann et al., Reference Biermann, Stilianakis, Bleich, Thürauf, Kornhuber and Reulbach2009; Szyszkowicz et al., Reference Szyszkowicz, Kousha, Kingsbury and Colman2016).

Children and adolescents may be particularly vulnerable to the effects of ambient PM2.5 and ozone on brain development given the time they spend being physically active outdoors and, therefore, breathing in more air in proportion to their body weight than do adults. Indeed, researchers have begun to document negative effects of PM2.5 exposure on cognitive and affective functioning in youth (Jorcano et al., Reference Jorcano, Lubczyńska, Pierotti, Altug, Ballester, Cesaroni, El Marroun, Fernández-Somoano, Freire, Hanke, Hoek, Ibarluzea, Iñiguez, Jansen, Lepeule, Markevych, Polańska, Porta, Schikowski, Slama, Standl, Tardon, Vrijkotte, von Berg, Tiemeier, Sunyer and Guxens2019; Peterson et al., Reference Peterson, Rauh, Bansal, Hao, Toth, Nati, Walsh, Miller, Arias, Semanek and Perera2015; Sunyer et al., Reference Sunyer, Esnaola, Alvarez-Pedrerol, Forns, Rivas, López-Vicente, Suades-González, Foraster, Garcia-Esteban, Basagaña, Viana, Cirach, Moreno, Alastuey, Sebastian-Galles, Nieuwenhuijsen and Querol2015), although it is still unclear how air pollutants affect the brain. In this context, however, there is some evidence of pollution-related alterations in white matter in children (Burnor et al., Reference Burnor, Cserbik, Cotter, Palmer, Ahmadi, Eckel, Berhane, McConnell, Chen, Schwartz, Jackson and Herting2021; Calderón-Garcidueñas et al., Reference Calderón-Garcidueñas, Mora-Tiscareño, Styner, Gómez-Garza, Zhu, Torres-Jardón, Carlos, Solorio-López, Medina-Cortina, Kavanaugh and D’Angiulli2012; Peterson et al., Reference Peterson, Rauh, Bansal, Hao, Toth, Nati, Walsh, Miller, Arias, Semanek and Perera2015), as well as cortical thinning and reduced gray matter volume (Beckwith et al., Reference Beckwith, Cecil, Altaye, Severs, Wolfe, Percy, Maloney, Yolton, LeMasters, Brunst and Ryan2020; Guxens et al., Reference Guxens, Lubczyńska, Muetzel, Dalmau-Bueno, Jaddoe, Hoek, van der Lugt, Verhulst, White, Brunekreef, Tiemeier and El Marroun2018).

When we designed our study, we had not considered examining how environmental toxins might interact with ELS to affect adolescent neurodevelopment. Recently however, given the increasing societal emphasis on climate change and its broad effects, combined with a growing body of research documenting the adverse effects of air and water pollution on psychobiological functioning, we began to explore the possibility of integrating measures of exposure to environmental toxins into our study. Doing so would allow us to examine the possible role of air and water contaminants in affecting relations between early adversity and trajectories of brain development and clinical symptoms in the children and adolescents in our project without adding to participant burden.

Jonas Miller, then a postdoctoral fellow in our lab, located an online resource, CalEnviroScreen, that was created by the Office of Environmental Health Hazard Assessment as a tool to map various environmental indicators for pollution and population characteristics across California neighborhoods at the census-tract level (Faust et al., Reference Faust, August, Bangia, Galaviz, Leichty, Prasad, Schmitz, Slocombe, Welling and Zeise2017). CalEnviroScreen uses environmental, health, and socioeconomic information to produce scores for every census tract in the state. Most relevant to our project, CalEnviroScreen provides census-tract-level data for levels of PM2.5, ozone, and water quality, as well as neighborhood-level socioeconomic disadvantage, indexed by census-tract measures of poverty, educational attainment, unemployment, and housing burden. By mapping our participants’ addresses to CalEnviroScreen census-tract data, we are able to associate potential exposure to environmental toxins with sociodemographic variables and neurobiological functioning across adolescence.

In a series of studies using CalEnviroScreen data, we examined associations of PM2.5 with several of our constructs in our project. For example, Miller et al. (Reference Miller, Gillette, Manczak, Kircanski and Gotlib2019) found that adolescents residing in neighborhoods characterized by higher concentrations of PM2.5 had greater autonomic reactivity in response to social stress during a Trier Social Stress Test (TSST) and, further, that this association was moderated by symptoms of anxiety and depression, with youth who reported more severe anxiety and depression symptoms showing the strongest association between PM2.5 and autonomic reactivity. In a subsequent study using tensor-based morphometry, Miller et al. (Reference Miller, Buthmann and Gotlib2022) found that ELS moderated the effects of PM2.5 on changes in brain volume in distinct regions over a two-year period. More specifically, for adolescents who had experienced less severe ELS, PM2.5 was associated with volumetric changes across several gray and white matter regions; fewer effects of PM2.5 were observed for adolescents who had experienced more severe ELS. Thus, for several brain regions, exposure to ELS constrained the effects of PM2.5 on structural development.

In the context of biological aging that we discussed earlier, Miller et al. (Reference Miller, Buthmann and Gotlib2022) examined associations among pollution exposure, telomere length, and hippocampal volume. Miller et al. (Reference Miller, Ho, Kirshenbaum, Chahal, Gifuni and Gotlib2021) found that greater hippocampal volume exacerbated the association of neighborhood pollution burden with telomere length; further, in youth with larger hippocampal volume, pollution burden was indirectly associated with shorter telomere length approximately 2 years later through shorter telomere length at baseline, suggesting that living in high-pollution-burden neighborhoods predisposes children them to develop shorter telomeres later in adolescence.

Similarly, Uy et al. (Reference Uy, Yuan, Colich and Gotlibunder review) examined the association of pollution burden with brain functioning and found that community pollution burden was associated with greater activation and connectivity among brain regions within the default mode network during implicit emotion regulation of negatively-valenced stimuli, which in turn were associated with greater longitudinal increases in depressive symptoms in pollution-burdened adolescents. Finally, Uy et al. (Reference Uy, Shin, Buthmann, Kircanski, LeMoult, Berens and Gotlibin preparation) found that greater exposure to diesel particulate matter was associated with lower/blunted cortisol reactivity during the TSST, which was associated with more severe depressive symptoms in adolescents.

Neighborhood disadvantage

There is also a growing focus in this field on the influence of relative socioeconomic (SES) advantage of an individual’s community on child development. As Hyde et al. (Reference Hyde, Gard, Tomlinson, Burt, Mitchell and Monk2020) highlight, the more immediate home environment (e.g., parenting behaviors, family discord) interacts with more distal neighborhood environment, and because children spend more time in the neighborhood as they mature, the neighborhood context becomes a more salient factor in shaping adolescent brain development. A growing literature has found neighborhood disadvantage is associated with mental health (Rakesh et al., Reference Rakesh, Seguin, Zalesky, Cropley and Whittle2021, Reference Rakesh, Zalesky and Whittle2023), cognition (Hackman et al., Reference Hackman, Cserbik, Chen, Berhane, Minaravesh, McConnell and Herting2021), and brain structure (Miller et al., Reference Miller, López, Buthmann, Garcia and Gotlib2022; Rakesh et al., Reference Rakesh, Zalesky and Whittle2022), and function (Gard et al., Reference Gard, Maxwell, Shaw, Mitchell, Brooks-Gunn, McLanahan, Forbes, Monk and Hyde2021; Rakesh et al., Reference Rakesh, Seguin, Zalesky, Cropley and Whittle2021). In our lab, Dr Miller has integrated the data we collected from our participants with external databases to identify nuanced relations between the neighborhood SES disadvantage, cortical thickness, and depressive symptoms in adolescents (Miller et al., Reference Miller, López, Buthmann, Garcia and Gotlib2022). Specifically, we found that an overall thinner cortex mediated the association of greater neighborhood SES disadvantage with more severe depressive symptoms in adolescents, and that greater neighborhood SES disadvantage, over and above family SES, was uniquely associated with cortical thinning in regions implicated in emotion processing (e.g., superior frontal gyrus, insula, fusiform).

Recognizing that there are likely to be significant associations among neighborhood disadvantage, family SES, environmental contaminants, parenting behaviors, and overall health, we’ve begun to explore the associations among these variables and anxiety symptoms and white matter development in our sample of adolescents. For example, we (Buthmann et al., Reference Buthmann, Uy, Miller, Coury, Yuan, Ho and Gotlib2023) found that living in a community with greater SES disadvantage was associated with lower uncinate fasciculus structural integrity; however, if adolescents also reported higher parental warmth, anxiety symptoms were low. Moreover, when probing the aspects of the community that were associated with risk for poor structural integrity and anxiety symptoms, we identified poverty, contaminated drinking water, poorer health, and traffic density as important predictors. These findings underscore the importance of integrating environmental and neighborhood variables into the study of the neurobiology of adolescence in order to generate more comprehensive theories of the neural and behavioral development of youth.

Inflammation

It is well documented that exposure to ELS is one of the strongest predictors of developing depression in adolescence (Heim & Binder, Reference Heim and Binder2012; LeMoult et al., Reference LeMoult, Humphreys, Tracy, Hoffmeister, Ip and Gotlib2020). Indeed, we designed our project to increase our understanding of how adverse events “get under the skin” to increase risk for depression and other forms of emotional disturbance. Since we developed and began to conduct our project, research began to emerge implicating inflammation as a key causal pathway linking ELS and depression (Danese & J Lewis, Reference Danese and J Lewis2017; Slavich & Irwin, Reference Slavich and Irwin2014). For example, compared to non-maltreated children, maltreated children have been found to have higher peripheral levels of inflammatory biomarkers, such as C-reactive protein (CRP), interleukin-6 (IL-6), and tumor necrosis factor-α (TNF-α) (Copeland et al., Reference Copeland, Wolke, Lereya, Shanahan, Worthman and Costello2014; Danese et al., Reference Danese, Caspi, Williams, Ambler, Sugden, Mika, Werts, Freeman, Pariante, Moffitt and Arseneault2011; Slopen et al., Reference Slopen, Kubzansky, McLaughlin and Koenen2013). Importantly, adversity-related low-grade inflammation appears to persist across the lifespan; researchers have documented elevated concentrations of inflammatory markers in adults who, as children, were exposed to maltreatment and/or adversity (Danese et al., Reference Danese, Pariante, Caspi, Taylor and Poulton2007, Reference Danese, Moffitt, Harrington, Milne, Polanczyk, Pariante, Poulton and Caspi2009; Matthews et al., Reference Matthews, Chang, Thurston and Bromberger2014).

There is also now converging evidence that inflammation plays a causal role in the pathogenesis of depression. A recent meta-analysis of over 100 studies of cytokine levels in depression found that depressed patients had higher concentrations of inflammatory markers than did controls (Osimo et al., Reference Osimo, Pillinger, Rodriguez, Khandaker, Pariante and Howes2020). Other researchers have reported that concentrations of these inflammatory markers predict the subsequent development of depressive symptoms (Khandaker et al., Reference Khandaker, Pearson, Zammit, Lewis and Jones2014; Moriarity et al., Reference Moriarity, Mac Giollabhui, Ellman, Klugman, Coe, Abramson and Alloy2019; Wium-Andersen et al., Reference Wium-Andersen, Ørsted, Nielsen and Nordestgaard2013). Thus, recent data suggest that inflammation is a psychobiological mechanism by which ELS can lead to depression; however, the neural mechanisms by which this process occurs are still unknown. We felt that it is important that we elucidate these mechanisms in light of the significant neurodevelopmental changes that characterize adolescent development. Given findings implicating inflammation in altered stress- and reward-related processing and depression (Pizzagalli et al., Reference Pizzagalli, Holmes, Dillon, Goetz, Birk, Bogdan, Dougherty, Iosifescu, Rauch and Fava2009), we were particularly well positioned to examine inflammation and brain function and structure in our study because of our RDoC focus on stress/affective and reward-related neural circuitry. By leveraging and integrating our existing data on ELS and our planned assessments and analyses of trajectories of neurodevelopment and depression, we were able to examine whether stress/affective and reward-related neurocircuitry is sensitive to the effects of inflammation and mediates the associations between ELS and depression and, further, to test whether the associations among inflammation, stress and reward circuitry, and depression depend on the types of ELS to which participants have been exposed.

At our third time point of data collection (2018–2021, 13–17 years of age), we began implementing a dried blood spot protocol to measure inflammation in our participants. This minimally invasive procedure yields a sample from which several cytokines and C-reactive protein (CRP) can be derived. From these data, Yuan et al. (Reference Yuan, Ho, Coury, Chahal, Colich and Gotlib2022) found that in the context of higher early life stress, higher CRP levels were associated with decreased ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (vlPFC) activity and decreased amygdala-vlPFC functional connectivity during implicit regulation of negative emotional stimuli. Further, CRP was not associated with neural activation or connectivity in youth exposed to lower levels of early life stress, suggesting that ELS exposure disrupts neuroimmune signaling in emotion regulation circuitry. Recently, we also found that nucleus accumbens activity during reward processing was blunted in adolescents with higher levels of CRP and greater exposure to ELS (Yuan et al., Reference Yuan, Coury, Ho and Gotlib2023) and further, that a greater increase in CRP over two years was significantly associated with a smaller increase in amygdala volume, particularly in youth who experienced higher SES disadvantage (Yuan et al., Reference Yuan, Jaeger, Coury, Ho and Gotlibunder review).

Sleep

An emerging literature has been documenting that sleep behaviors and quality undergo significant changes during the transition to adolescence, increasing the prevalence of sleep disturbances during adolescence (Crowley et al., Reference Crowley, Wolfson, Tarokh and Carskadon2018). These sleep disturbances have been linked to neural, endocrine, immune, and mental health outcomes during development, and are a prevalent feature of many physical and mental disorders. These sleep-related alterations in biology and in outcomes are similar to those that have been attributed to ELS; however, empirical investigations in these areas have been conducted in relative isolation (Fuligni et al., Reference Fuligni, Chiang and Tottenham2021). To increase our understanding of the role of sleep and its effects on the neural and biological mechanisms that link ELS to mental health outcomes during adolescence, we added several measures of sleep to our third assessment, including subjective reports of sleep time/quality, actigraphy recordings of sleep, and sleep diaries. In analyzing data obtained with these measures, we found that, consistent with formulations that pubertal processes underlie changes in sleep during adolescence, advanced pubertal stage, but not chronological age, was associated with poorer subjective sleep satisfaction, which in turn was associated with higher levels of negative affect (Kirshenbaum et al., Reference Kirshenbaum, Coury, Colich, Manber and Gotlib2023). Integrating these data with other measures of sleep that we had included earlier in the study, Uy and Gotlib (Reference Uy and Gotlibin press) also found that female adolescents who were exposed to more severe adverse events during childhood reported more sleep problems during early adolescence (9–13 years, our first timepoint) as well as greater longitudinal increases (across the first 3 time points) in sleep problems across adolescence, which in turn were each independently associated with longitudinal increases in depressive symptoms. Interestingly, for male adolescents, while more severe adverse events during childhood and sleep problems during adolescence were also related to increases in depressive symptoms, early adversity was not related to sleep problems, suggesting that there are sex differences in the mechanisms by which early adversity relates to depressive symptoms during adolescence. Uy et al. (Reference Uy, Ho, Buthmann, Coury and Gotlib2023) found, further, that more sleep problems were associated with lower uncinate fasciculus microstructural integrity in adolescents exposed to high levels of early adversity, and with higher cingulum cingulate integrity in adolescents exposed to low levels of early adversity, which in turn was associated with greater increases in depressive symptoms.

Finally, integrating measurements of environmental toxins, inflammation, and sleep, we found that greater pollution burden was associated with higher levels of CRP among adolescents who spent less time asleep (based on actigraphy). In contrast, pollution burden was not related to levels of CRP among adolescents who slept longer on average and also among those who engaged in greater catch-up sleep during the weekends, suggesting that extending sleep time (and/or catching up on lost sleep) may buffer the inflammatory effects of pollution (Uy et al., Reference Uy, Yuan, Kirshenbaum, Coury, Buthmann, Ho and Gotlibin preparation).

COVID-19

We had designed our project initially to address the alarmingly high prevalence of ELS (Child Welfare Information Gateway, 2019; Kim & Drake, Reference Kim and Drake2019) and its significant impact on mental health in youth. In analyzing the data we collected at the first two timepoints of our study, we were obtaining findings consistent with the formulation that ELS increases adolescents’ vulnerability or reactivity to the effects of stress, placing them at higher risk for developing symptoms of depression and other forms of emotional disturbance. Midway through the third timepoint assessment in March 2020, the global COVID-19 pandemic halted in-person research for what would prove to be almost a year. At the time, of course, we had no idea that the university would be closed for that long (and longer in other respects). We pivoted toward assessing in depth how the adolescents in our study were coping not with early life stress, but with a concurrent, multifaceted stressor. To adapt and ensure we could understand the toll of the global pandemic, we remotely administered several additional measures of loneliness, screen time usage, resilience, perceived stress, and other psychopathology symptoms (e.g., anxiety and obsessive-compulsive symptoms).

Amid a global backdrop of fear and uncertainty, the shelter-in-place orders in spring 2020 that led to school closures, academic disruptions, social restrictions, and reduced access to school-based mental health services (Golberstein et al., Reference Golberstein, Wen and Miller2020) made the pandemic particularly difficult for children and adolescents (Jiao et al., Reference Jiao, Wang, Liu, Fang, Jiao, Pettoello-Mantovani and Somekh2020; Xie et al., Reference Xie, Xue, Zhou, Zhu, Liu, Zhang and Song2020; Zhou et al., Reference Zhou, Zhang, Wang, Guo, Wang, Chen, Liu, Chen and Chen2020). Indeed, a meta-analysis found that the prevalence of internalizing symptoms in children and adolescents doubled during the COVID-19 pandemic (Racine et al., Reference Racine, McArthur, Cooke, Eirich, Zhu and Madigan2021). We now know that, in addition to an enormous death toll, the pandemic led to increased personal and familial distress, economic hardship, and enforced social isolation (Galea et al., Reference Galea, Merchant and Lurie2020; Holmes et al., Reference Holmes, O'Connor, Perry, Tracey, Wessely, Arseneault, Ballard, Christensen, Cohen Silver, Everall, Ford, John, Kabir, King, Madan, Michie, Przybylski, Shafran, Sweeney, Worthman, Yardley, Cowan, Cope, Hotopf and Bullmore2020; Racine et al., Reference Racine, McArthur, Cooke, Eirich, Zhu and Madigan2021). We were well-situated to assess the extent of the psychological toll of the pandemic in our richly characterized sample.

Of course, we had not anticipated the pandemic when we designed our study, but we were nonetheless able to test the formulation that ELS would increase adolescents’ vulnerability to the effects of COVID-19-related stress, placing them at higher risk for developing symptoms of depression. As we expected, severity of ELS predicted levels of depressive symptoms during the pandemic, which continued to be higher in females than in males (Gotlib et al., Reference Gotlib, Borchers, Chahal, Gifuni, Teresi and Ho2021). Importantly, the association between ELS and depression was mediated by adolescents’ reported levels of perceived stress. In other studies, we used data we had obtained before the pandemic to predict our participants’ functioning during the lockdowns. For example, Chahal et al. (Reference Chahal, Delevich, Kirshenbaum, Borchers, Ho and Gotlib2021) found that early-maturing youth had greater increases in internalizing symptoms during the pandemic if the connectivity/coherence of their brains’ executive control network (ECN) at T1 was low; in contrast, relative pubertal stage was not associated with changes in internalizing symptoms in adolescents with higher ECN coherence, highlighting the role of neural functional architecture in protecting against risk factors that may exacerbate symptoms of internalizing psychopathology during periods of stress and uncertainty. Miller et al. (Reference Miller, Chahal, Kirshenbaum, Ho, Gifuni and Gotlib2021) reported similar predictive effects of pre-pandemic quality of parenting and connectivity between the amygdala and subgenual anterior cingulate cortex on subsequent depressive symptoms, and of pre-pandemic levels of family adversity and heart rate variability (Chahal et al., Reference Chahal, Kirshenbaum, Miller, Ho and Gotlib2021).

When we resumed in-person research (and scanning), we also began to examine how adolescents who were assessed prior to the pandemic differed from adolescents who were assessed after the COVID-19 lockdowns ended. Prior to the pandemic, researchers found that exposure to ELS (i.e., violence, neglect, and family dysfunction) is associated with both poorer mental health and with maladaptive neurodevelopment indicative of accelerated brain maturation (Colich et al., Reference Colich, Rosen, Williams and McLaughlin2020). Consistent with our hypotheses, we found that participants assessed after the pandemic began had higher levels of internalizing symptoms, reduced cortical thickness, larger limbic structures, and accelerated brain ages (Gotlib et al., Reference Gotlib, Miller, Borchers, Coury, Costello, Garcia and Ho2023), underscoring the need for longitudinal studies that began before the pandemic to consider the effects of lockdowns on trajectories of neurodevelopment. Understanding factors that mitigate or exacerbate these impacts, as well as the continued developmental trajectories in a post-pandemic world, will continue to be an important part of our work.

Implications of interdisciplinarity for training in developmental psychopathology

While it is easy to simply write here that interdisciplinarity is important (and, we think, necessary) for the advancement of developmental psychopathology and to encourage researchers to embrace this, we must recognize that a greater focus on interdisciplinarity has significant implications for training in this field. Fifty years ago, interdisciplinarity may have meant that investigators receive training that would allow them to integrate clinical psychology into the “academic” study of development and psychopathology. Today, the range and diversity of domains of research in developmental psychopathology seem limitless, arguably broader than a single investigator can master. As other investigators have described (e.g., Cicchetti & Toth, Reference Cicchetti and Toth2009; Susman et al., Reference Susman, Marceau, Dockray and Ram2019), some of the challenges in conducting interdisciplinary research include (1) establishing communication and collaborations among researchers across disciplines, which may be especially challenging in institutions with traditional discipline-based academic departments; (2) bridging gaps in technical skills and knowledge between or among disciplines; (3) obtaining funding to conduct large-scale, interdisciplinary, longitudinal studies; and (4) finding and selecting suitable journals in which to publish interdisciplinary findings. Meeting these challenges will require us to ensure that the next generations of scientists have the opportunity to receive interdisciplinary training in order to conduct the most impactful research possible in developmental psychopathology.

In considering the changes that such training would entail, it is clear that there are not only advantages over current training models, but also drawbacks and obstacles to receiving training aimed at increasing the interdisciplinarity of trainees. For example, as trainees are taking courses with experts across disciplines to broaden their understanding of various aspects of developmental psychopathology, they may at the same time be sacrificing depth of their knowledge and skills. And with this training, graduate degrees and postdoctoral fellowships may require more mentors and additional time to complete, which can place a burden on and exacerbate disparities for those trainees who are experiencing economic hardship. Certainly, funding opportunities are available – for example, the NIH’s K-Award mechanism – but they remain extremely competitive. And compounding this situation, the more interdisciplinary a training proposal becomes, the more difficult it will be to find expert reviewers (also an issue, of course, with interdisciplinary scientific papers!).

We should note that there is already an increasing demand and expectation that trainees in developmental psychopathology develop skills in advanced methodologies and statistical techniques. The emergence both of sophisticated statistical approaches to data analysis, such as structural equation modeling, k-means clustering, and machine learning, and of larger longitudinal, multivariate, publicly available datasets can both help trainees to begin to engage in interdisciplinary research and hinder the depth of theoretical knowledge that they are able to develop. And as a related point, larger sample sizes and the emerging need for reproducibility of findings extract a considerable toll in time, funding, and resources.

At the institutional level, interdisciplinary collaborations will require financial support, space, supervision, and other resources that may be limited. Expectations of what constitutes a “focus” of a research program for tenure decisions will have to change, as will valuations of single- versus multiple-authored scientific papers. As a related point, current standards of “productivity” for investigators may have to be revisited in light of the time and effort it takes to develop and foster interdisciplinary collaborations, collect multi-domain data, and prepare specialized grant applications and manuscripts that bridge more than one discipline. By making explicit efforts to address these hurdles and difficulties, institutions and funding bodies can increase both the number of interdisciplinary trainees and investigators, and the quality of boundary-breaking research. As Susman et al. (Reference Susman, Marceau, Dockray and Ram2019) note, we can begin to address challenges in this area by encouraging communication, terminology, and research practices across disciplines, facilitating cross-discipline data sharing, and beginning interdisciplinary training at the undergraduate level.

Concluding comments

As our understanding of the brain, the body, and the environment has advanced, so too has the field of developmental psychopathology. It has become increasingly apparent over the past few decades that the emergence of psychopathology, often in the face of adversity, cannot be studied in isolation because it does not develop independent of many other personal and environmental factors. To gain expertise in developmental psychopathology now requires researchers to be open and flexible, and to be knowledgeable about a range of domains, from dynamic and interacting biological systems to equally dynamic and interacting environments in which individuals live. Research that focuses on developmental psychopathology without taking the perspective of a different (even if related) discipline is not likely to move the field forward in a significant way. Indeed, working towards the goals that Masten (Reference Masten2006) formulated – to prevent and alleviate the suffering of psychopathology – we have made great strides in identifying mechanisms through which a range of adverse experiences can affect children’s and adolescents’ psychobiological and emotional development. These discoveries, and those still to come as our field continues to evolve, are contributing to the generation of prevention and intervention strategies that can reduce the burden of psychopathology on individuals, families, and society. Supporting and promoting strong interdisciplinary research is paramount to seeing these goals to fruition.

Funding statement

Preparation of this article was supported by National Institute of Mental Health Grants MHR37101495 to IHG and F32MH135657 to JPU and by a Brain and Behavior Research Foundation Young Investigator Award to JLB.

Competing interests

None.

References

Achenbach, T. M. (1974), Developmental psychopathology (pp. 726). Ronald Press.Google Scholar
Beckwith, T., Cecil, K., Altaye, M., Severs, R., Wolfe, C., Percy, Z., Maloney, T., Yolton, K., LeMasters, G., Brunst, K., & Ryan, P. (2020). Reduced gray matter volume and cortical thickness associated with traffic-related air pollution in a longitudinally studied pediatric cohort. PLOS ONE, 15(1), e0228092. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0228092 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Biermann, T., Stilianakis, N., Bleich, S., Thürauf, N., Kornhuber, J., & Reulbach, U. (2009). The hypothesis of an impact of ozone on the occurrence of completed and attempted suicides. Medical Hypotheses, 72(3), 338341. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.mehy.2008.09.042 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Borchers, L. R., Bruckert, L., Chahal, R., Mastrovito, D., Ho, T. C., & Gotlib, I. H. (2022). White matter microstructural properties of the cerebellar peduncles predict change in symptoms of psychopathology in adolescent girls. The Cerebellum, 21(3), 380390. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12311-021-01307-x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Breslau, J., Gilman, S. E., Stein, B. D., Ruder, T., Gmelin, T., & Miller, E. (2017). Sex differences in recent first-onset depression in an epidemiological sample of adolescents. Translational Psychiatry, 7(5), e1139. https://doi.org/10.1038/tp.2017.105 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brook, R. D., Rajagopalan, S., Pope, C. A. III, Brook, J. R., Bhatnagar, A., Diez-Roux, A. V., Holguin, F., Hong, Y., Luepker, R. V., Mittleman, M. A., Peters, A., Siscovick, D., Smith, S. C., Whitsel, L., & Kaufman, J. D. (2010). Particulate matter air pollution and cardiovascular disease: An update to the scientific statement from the American heart association. Circulation, 121(21), 23312378. https://doi.org/10.1161/CIR.0b013e3181dbece1 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burnor, E., Cserbik, D., Cotter, D. L., Palmer, C. E., Ahmadi, H., Eckel, S. P., Berhane, K., McConnell, R., Chen, J.-C., Schwartz, J., Jackson, R., & Herting, M. M. (2021). Association of outdoor ambient fine particulate matter with intracellular white matter microstructural properties among children. JAMA Network Open, 4(12), e2138300. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2021.38300 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buthmann, J. L., Uy, J. P., Miller, J. G., Coury, S. M., Yuan, J. P., Ho, T. C., & Gotlib, I. H. (2023). Neighborhood disadvantage and parenting predict longitudinal clustering of brain structure and clinical symptomatology in adolescents. OSF Storage.Google Scholar
Calderón-Garcidueñas, L., Mora-Tiscareño, A., Styner, M., Gómez-Garza, G., Zhu, H., Torres-Jardón, R., Carlos, E., Solorio-López, E., Medina-Cortina, H., Kavanaugh, M., & D’Angiulli, A. (2012). White matter hyperintensities, systemic inflammation, brain growth, and cognitive functions in children exposed to air pollution. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease: JAD, 31(1), 183191. https://doi.org/10.3233/JAD-2012-120610 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chahal, R., Delevich, K., Kirshenbaum, J. S., Borchers, L. R., Ho, T. C., & Gotlib, I. H. (2021). Sex differences in pubertal associations with fronto-accumbal white matter morphometry: Implications for understanding sensitivity to reward and punishment. NeuroImage, 226, 117598. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117598 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chahal, R., Kirshenbaum, J. S., Miller, J. G., Ho, T. C., & Gotlib, I. H. (2021). Higher executive control network coherence buffers against puberty-related increases in internalizing symptoms during the COVID-19 pandemic. Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging, 6(1), 7988. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bpsc.2020.08.010 Google ScholarPubMed
Chahal, R., Miller, J. G., Yuan, J. P., Buthmann, J. L., & Gotlib, I. H. (2022). An exploration of dimensions of early adversity and the development of functional brain network connectivity during adolescence: Implications for trajectories of internalizing symptoms. Development and Psychopathology, 34(2), 557571. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579421001814 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cicchetti, D., & Rogosch, F. A. (2002). A developmental psychopathology perspective on adolescence. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 70(1), 620. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.70.1.6 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cicchetti, D., & Toth, S. L. (2009). The past achievements and future promises of developmental psychopathology: The coming of age of a discipline. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 50(1-2), 1625. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.01979.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clayton, S., Manning, C. M., Hill, A. N., & Speiser, M. (2023). Mental health and our changing climate: Children and youth report 2023. American Psychological Association and ecoAmerica.Google Scholar
Clougherty, J. E., Levy, J. I., Kubzansky, L. D., Ryan, P. B., Suglia, S. F., Canner, M. J., & Wright, R. J. (2007). Synergistic effects of traffic-related air pollution and exposure to violence on urban asthma etiology. Environmental Health Perspectives, 115(8), 11401146. https://doi.org/10.1289/ehp.9863 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Colich, N. L., Rosen, M. L., Williams, E. S., & McLaughlin, K. A. (2020). Biological aging in childhood and adolescence following experiences of threat and deprivation: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 146(9), 721764. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000270 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Colich, N. L., Williams, E. S., Ho, T. C., King, L. S., Humphreys, K. L., Price, A. N., Ordaz, S. J., & Gotlib, I. H. (2017). The association between early life stress and prefrontal cortex activation during implicit emotion regulation is moderated by sex in early adolescence. Development and Psychopathology, 29(5), 18511864. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579417001444 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Copeland, W. E., Wolke, D., Lereya, S. T., Shanahan, L., Worthman, C., & Costello, E. J. (2014). Childhood bullying involvement predicts low-grade systemic inflammation into adulthood. Proceedings of The National Academy of Sciences of The United States of America, 111(21), 75707575. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1323641111 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crowley, S. J., Wolfson, A. R., Tarokh, L., & Carskadon, M. A. (2018). An update on adolescent sleep: New evidence informing the perfect storm model☆. Journal of Adolescence, 67(1), 5565. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2018.06.001 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Danese, A., Caspi, A., Williams, B., Ambler, A., Sugden, K., Mika, J., Werts, H., Freeman, J., Pariante, C. M., Moffitt, T. E., & Arseneault, L. (2011). Biological embedding of stress through inflammation processes in childhood. Molecular Psychiatry, 16(3), 244246. https://doi.org/10.1038/mp.2010.5 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Danese, A., & J Lewis, S. (2017). Psychoneuroimmunology of early-life stress: The hidden wounds of childhood trauma? Neuropsychopharmacology, 42(1), 99114. https://doi.org/10.1038/npp.2016.198 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Danese, A., Moffitt, T. E., Harrington, H., Milne, B. J., Polanczyk, G., Pariante, C. M., Poulton, R., & Caspi, A. (2009). Adverse childhood experiences and adult risk factors for age-related disease: Depression, inflammation, and clustering of metabolic risk markers. Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine, 163(12), 11351143. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpediatrics.2009.214 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Danese, A., Pariante, C. M., Caspi, A., Taylor, A., & Poulton, R. (2007). Childhood maltreatment predicts adult inflammation in a life-course study. Proceedings of The National Academy of Sciences of The United States of America, 104(4), 13191324. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0610362104 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
EPA (2023). Climate change and children’s health and well-being in the United States (EPA 430-R-23-001). U.S. Environmental Protection Agency.Google Scholar
Faust, J., August, L., Bangia, K., Galaviz, V., Leichty, J., Prasad, S., Schmitz, R., Slocombe, A., Welling, R., & Zeise, L. (2017). Update to the California communities environmental health screening tool. CalEnviroScreen 3.0. https://oehha.ca.gov/calenviroscreen/report/calenviroscreen-30 Google Scholar
Fuligni, A. J., Chiang, J. J., & Tottenham, N. (2021). Sleep disturbance and the long-term impact of early adversity. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 126, 304313. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.03.021 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Galea, S., Merchant, R. M., & Lurie, N. (2020). The mental health consequences of COVID-19 and physical distancing: The need for prevention and early intervention. JAMA Internal Medicine, 180(6), 817818. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamainternmed.2020.1562 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gard, A. M., Maxwell, A. M., Shaw, D. S., Mitchell, C., Brooks-Gunn, J., McLanahan, S. S., Forbes, E. E., Monk, C. S., & Hyde, L. W. (2021). Beyond family-level adversities: Exploring the developmental timing of neighborhood disadvantage effects on the brain. Developmental Science, 24(1), e12985. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12985 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Child Welfare Information Gateway, 2019). Long-term consequences of child abuse and neglect. U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, Administration for Children and Families, Children’s Bureau. https://www.childwelfare.gov/pubPDFs/long_term_consequences.pdf Google Scholar
Gee, D. G., Gabard-Durnam, L. J., Flannery, J., Goff, B., Humphreys, K. L., Telzer, E. H., Hare, T. A., Bookheimer, S. Y., & Tottenham, N. (2013). Early developmental emergence of human amygdala-prefrontal connectivity after maternal deprivation. Proceedings of The National Academy of Sciences of The United States of America, 110(39), 1563815643. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1307893110 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goff, B., & Tottenham, N. (2015). Early-life adversity and adolescent depression: Mechanisms involving the ventral striatum. CNS Spectrums, 20(4), 337345. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1092852914000674 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Golberstein, E., Wen, H., & Miller, B. F. (2020). Coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19) and mental health for children and adolescents. JAMA Pediatrics, 174(9), 819820. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapediatrics.2020.1456 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gotlib, I. H., Borchers, L. R., Chahal, R., Gifuni, A. J., Teresi, G. I., & Ho, T. C. (2021). Early life stress predicts depressive symptoms in adolescents during the COVID-19 pandemic: The mediating role of perceived stress. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 3864. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.603748 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gotlib, I. H., Miller, J. G., Borchers, L. R., Coury, S. M., Costello, L. A., Garcia, J. M., & Ho, T. C. (2023). Effects of the COVID-19 pandemic on mental health and brain maturation in adolescents: Implications for analyzing longitudinal data. Biological Psychiatry Global Open Science, 3(4), 912918. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bpsgos.2022.11.002 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Guxens, M., Lubczyńska, M. J., Muetzel, R. L., Dalmau-Bueno, A., Jaddoe, V. W. V., Hoek, G., van der Lugt, A., Verhulst, F. C., White, T., Brunekreef, B., Tiemeier, H., & El Marroun, H. (2018). Air pollution exposure during fetal life, brain morphology, and cognitive function in school-age children. Biological Psychiatry, 84(4), 295303. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2018.01.016 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hackman, D. A., Cserbik, D., Chen, J. C., Berhane, K., Minaravesh, B., McConnell, R., & Herting, M. M. (2021). Association of local variation in neighborhood disadvantage in metropolitan areas with youth neurocognition and brain structure. JAMA Pediatrics, 175(8), e210426. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapediatrics.2021.0426 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hanson, J. L., Albert, D., Iselin, A.-M. R., Carré, J. M., Dodge, K. A., & Hariri, A. R. (2016). Cumulative stress in childhood is associated with blunted reward-related brain activity in adulthood. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 11(3), 405412. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsv124 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heim, C., & Binder, E. B. (2012). Current research trends in early life stress and depression: Review of human studies on sensitive periods, gene-environment interactions, and epigenetics. Experimental Neurology, 233(1), 102111. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.expneurol.2011.10.032 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ho, T. C., King, L. S., Leong, J. K., Colich, N. L., Humphreys, K. L., Ordaz, S. J., & Gotlib, I. H. (2017). Effects of sensitivity to life stress on uncinate fasciculus segments in early adolescence. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 12(9), 14601469. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsx065 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holmes, E. A., O'Connor, R. C., Perry, V. H., Tracey, I., Wessely, S., Arseneault, L., Ballard, C., Christensen, H., Cohen Silver, R., Everall, I., Ford, T., John, A., Kabir, T., King, K., Madan, I., Michie, S., Przybylski, A. K., Shafran, R., Sweeney, A., Worthman, C. M., Yardley, L., Cowan, K., Cope, C., Hotopf, M., & Bullmore, E. (2020). Multidisciplinary research priorities for the COVID-19 pandemic: A call for action for mental health science. The Lancet Psychiatry, 7(6), 547560. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2215-0366(20)30168-1 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Humphreys, K. L., King, L. S., Sacchet, M. D., Camacho, M. C., Colich, N. L., Ordaz, S. J., Ho, T. C., & Gotlib, I. H. (2019). Evidence for a sensitive period in the effects of early life stress on hippocampal volume. Developmental Science, 22(3), e12775. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12775 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Humphreys, K. L., Kircanski, K., Colich, N. L., & Gotlib, I. H. (2016). Attentional avoidance of fearful facial expressions following early life stress is associated with impaired social functioning. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry, 57(10), 11741182. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12607 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Humphreys, K. L., Sisk, L. M., Manczak, E. M., Lin, J., & Gotlib, I. H. (2020). Depressive symptoms predict change in telomere length and mitochondrial DNA copy number across adolescence. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 59(12), 13641370.e2. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2019.09.031 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hyde, L. W., Gard, A. M., Tomlinson, R. C., Burt, S. A., Mitchell, C., & Monk, C. S. (2020). An ecological approach to understanding the developing brain: Examples linking poverty, parenting, neighborhoods, and the brain. American Psychologist, 75(9), 12451259. https://doi.org/10.1037/amp0000741 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Insel, T., Cuthbert, B., Garvey, M., Heinssen, R., Pine, D. S., Quinn, K., Sanislow, C., & Wang, P. (2010). Research domain criteria (RDoC): Toward a new classification framework for research on mental disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry, 167(7), 748751. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.09091379 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jiao, W. Y., Wang, L. N., Liu, J., Fang, S. F., Jiao, F. Y., Pettoello-Mantovani, M., & Somekh, E. (2020). Behavioral and emotional disorders in children during the COVID-19 epidemic. The Journal of Pediatrics, 221, 264266.e1. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpeds.2020.03.013 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jorcano, A., Lubczyńska, M. J., Pierotti, L., Altug, H., Ballester, F., Cesaroni, G., El Marroun, H., Fernández-Somoano, A., Freire, C., Hanke, W., Hoek, G., Ibarluzea, J. A.;s, Iñiguez, C., Jansen, P. W., Lepeule, J., Markevych, I., Polańska, K., Porta, D., Schikowski, T., Slama, R., Standl, M., Tardon, A., Vrijkotte, T. G. M., von Berg, A., Tiemeier, H., Sunyer, J., & Guxens, M. (2019). Prenatal and postnatal exposure to air pollution and emotional and aggressive symptoms in children from 8 European birth cohorts. Environment International, 131, 104927. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.envint.2019.104927 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Khandaker, G. M., Pearson, R. M., Zammit, S., Lewis, G., & Jones, P. B. (2014). Association of serum Interleukin 6 and C-reactive protein in childhood with depression and psychosis in young adult life: A population-based longitudinal study. JAMA Psychiatry, 71(10), 1121. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2014.1332 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, H., & Drake, B. (2019). Cumulative prevalence of onset and recurrence of child maltreatment reports. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 58(12), 11751183. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2019.02.015 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
King, L. S., Colich, N. L., LeMoult, J., Humphreys, K. L., Ordaz, S. J., Price, A. N., & Gotlib, I. H. (2017). The impact of the severity of early life stress on diurnal cortisol: The role of puberty. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 77, 6874. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2016.11.024 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
King, L. S., Dennis, E. L., Humphreys, K. L., Thompson, P. M., & Gotlib, I. H. (2020). Cross-sectional and longitudinal associations of family income-to-needs ratio with cortical and subcortical brain volume in adolescent boys and girls. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 44, 100796. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2020.100796 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
King, L. S., Graber, M. G., Colich, N. L., & Gotlib, I. H. (2020). Associations of waking cortisol with DHEA and testosterone across the pubertal transition: Effects of threat-related early life stress. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 115, 104651. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2020.104651 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kircanski, K., Sisk, L. M., Ho, T. C., Humphreys, K. L., King, L. S., Colich, N. L., Ordaz, S. J., & Gotlib, I. H. (2019). Early life stress, cortisol, frontolimbic connectivity, and depressive symptoms during puberty. Development and Psychopathology, 31(3), 10111022. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579419000555 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kirshenbaum, J. S., Coury, S. M., Colich, N. L., Manber, R., & Gotlib, I. H. (2023). Objective and subjective sleep health in adolescence: Associations with puberty and affect. Journal of Sleep Research, 32(3), e13805. https://doi.org/10.1111/jsr.13805 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, Y., Chahal, R., & Gotlib, I. H. (2023). The default mode network is associated with changes in internalizing and externalizing problems differently in adolescent boys and girls. Development and Psychopathology, 110. https://www.cambridge.org/core/journals/development-and-psychopathology/article/default-mode-network-is-associated-with-changes-in-internalizing-and-externalizing-problems-differently-in-adolescent-boys-and-girls/30501799073E28361A31F656AFF28A8E Google ScholarPubMed
LeMoult, J., Humphreys, K. L., King, L. S., Colich, N. L., Price, A. N., Ordaz, S. J., & Gotlib, I. H. (2019). Associations among early life stress, rumination, symptoms of psychopathology, and sex in youth in the early stages of puberty: A moderated mediation analysis. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 47(2), 199207. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-018-0430-x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
LeMoult, J., Humphreys, K. L., Tracy, A., Hoffmeister, J.-A., Ip, E., & Gotlib, I. H. (2020). Meta-analysis: Exposure to early life stress and risk for depression in childhood and adolescence. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 59(7), 842855. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2019.10.011 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lu, J. G., Lee, J. J., Gino, F., & Galinsky, A. D. (2018). Polluted morality: Air pollution predicts criminal activity and unethical behavior. Psychological Science, 29(3), 340355. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797617735807 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lupien, S. J., McEwen, B. S., Gunnar, M. R., & Heim, C. (2009). Effects of stress throughout the lifespan on the brain, behaviour and cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10(6), Article6. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2639 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Masten, A. S. (2006). Developmental psychopathology: Pathways to the future. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 30(1), 4754. https://doi.org/10.1177/0165025406059974 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Matthews, K. A., Chang, Y.-F., Thurston, R. C., & Bromberger, J. T. (2014). Child abuse is related to inflammation in mid-life women: Role of obesity. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity, 36, 2934. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbi.2013.09.013 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McLaughlin, K. A., Greif Green, J., Gruber, M. J., Sampson, N. A., Zaslavsky, A. M., & Kessler, R. C. (2012). Childhood adversities and first onset of psychiatric disorders in a national sample of us adolescents. Archives of General Psychiatry, 69(11), 11511160. https://doi.org/10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.2277 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, J. G., Buthmann, J. L., & Gotlib, I. H. (2022). Hippocampal volume indexes neurobiological sensitivity to the effect of pollution burden on telomere length in adolescents. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 2022(181-182), 155172. https://doi.org/10.1002/cad.20471 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, J. G., Chahal, R., Kirshenbaum, J. S., Ho, T. C., Gifuni, A. J., & Gotlib, I. H. (2021). Heart rate variability moderates the effects of COVID-19-related stress and family adversity on emotional problems in adolescents: Testing models of differential susceptibility and diathesis stress. Development and Psychopathology, 1-12(5), 19741985. https://doi.org/10.1017/S095457942100033X Google Scholar
Miller, J. G., Dennis, E. L., Heft-Neal, S., Jo, B., & Gotlib, I. H. (2022). Fine particulate air pollution, early life stress, and their interactive effects on adolescent structural brain development: A longitudinal tensor-based morphometry study. Cerebral Cortex, 32(10), 21562169. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhab346 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, J. G., Gillette, J. S., Manczak, E. M., Kircanski, K., & Gotlib, I. H. (2019). Fine particle air pollution and physiological reactivity to social stress in adolescence: The moderating role of anxiety and depression. Psychosomatic Medicine, 81(7), 641648. https://doi.org/10.1097/PSY.0000000000000714 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, J. G., Ho, T. C., Kirshenbaum, J. S., Chahal, R., Gifuni, A. J., & Gotlib, I. H. (2021). Testing a developmental model of positive parenting, amygdala-subgenual anterior cingulate cortex connectivity, and depressive symptoms in adolescents before and during the COVID-19 pandemic. Biological Psychiatry Global Open Science, 1(4), 291299. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bpsgos.2021.07.005 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, J. G., López, V., Buthmann, J. L., Garcia, J. M., & Gotlib, I. H. (2022). A social gradient of cortical thickness in adolescence: Relationships with neighborhood socioeconomic disadvantage, family socioeconomic status, and depressive symptoms. Biological Psychiatry Global Open Science, 2(3), 253262. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bpsgos.2022.03.005 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moriarity, D. P., Mac Giollabhui, N., Ellman, L. M., Klugman, J., Coe, C. L., Abramson, L. Y., & Alloy, L. B. (2019). Inflammatory proteins predict change in depressive symptoms in male and female adolescents. Clinical Psychological Science, 7(4), 754767. https://doi.org/10.1177/2167702619826586 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moulton, P. V., & Yang, W. (2012). Air pollution, oxidative stress, and alzheimer’s disease. Journal of Environmental and Public Health, 2012, e4727519. https://doi.org/10.1155/2012/472751 2012.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Navas-Acien, A., Guallar, E., Silbergeld, E. K., & Rothenberg, S. J. (2007). Lead exposure and cardiovascular disease—A systematic review. Environmental Health Perspectives, 115(3), 472482. https://doi.org/10.1289/ehp.9785 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Osimo, E. F., Pillinger, T., Rodriguez, I. M., Khandaker, G. M., Pariante, C. M., & Howes, O. D. (2020). Inflammatory markers in depression: A meta-analysis of mean differences and variability in 5,166 patients and 5,083 controls. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity, 87, 901909. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbi.2020.02.010 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peterson, B. S., Rauh, V. A., Bansal, R., Hao, X., Toth, Z., Nati, G., Walsh, K., Miller, R. L., Arias, F., Semanek, D., & Perera, F. (2015). Effects of prenatal exposure to air pollutants (polycyclic aromatic hydrocarbons) on the development of brain white matter, cognition, and behavior in later childhood. JAMA Psychiatry, 72(6), 531540. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2015.57 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pizzagalli, D. A., Holmes, A. J., Dillon, D. G., Goetz, E. L., Birk, J. L., Bogdan, R., Dougherty, D. D., Iosifescu, D. V., Rauch, S. L., & Fava, M. (2009). Reduced caudate and nucleus accumbens response to rewards in unmedicated individuals with major depressive disorder. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 166(6), 702710. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.08081201 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Racine, N., McArthur, B. A., Cooke, J. E., Eirich, R., Zhu, J., & Madigan, S. (2021). Global prevalence of depressive and anxiety symptoms in children and adolescents during COVID-19. JAMA Pediatrics, 175(11), 110. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapediatrics.2021.2482 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rakesh, D., Seguin, C., Zalesky, A., Cropley, V., & Whittle, S. (2021). Associations between neighborhood disadvantage, resting-state functional connectivity, and behavior in the adolescent brain cognitive development study: The moderating role of positive family and school environments. Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging, 6(9), 877886. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bpsc.2021.03.008 Google Scholar
Rakesh, D., Zalesky, A., & Whittle, S. (2022). Assessment of parent income and education, neighborhood disadvantage, and child brain structure. JAMA Network Open, 5(8), e2226208. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2022.26208 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rakesh, D., Zalesky, A., & Whittle, S. (2023). The role of school environment in brain structure, connectivity, and mental health in children: A multimodal investigation. Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging, 8(1), 3241. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bpsc.2022.01.006 Google ScholarPubMed
Slavich, G. M., & Irwin, M. R. (2014). From stress to inflammation and major depressive disorder: A social signal transduction theory of depression. Psychological Bulletin, 140(3), 774815. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0035302 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Slopen, N., Kubzansky, L. D., McLaughlin, K. A., & Koenen, K. C. (2013). Childhood adversity and inflammatory processes in youth: A prospective study. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 38(2), 188200. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2012.05.013 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sroufe, L. A., & Rutter, M. (1984). The domain of developmental psychopathology. Child Development, 55(1), 1729. https://doi.org/10.2307/1129832 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sunyer, J., Esnaola, M., Alvarez-Pedrerol, M., Forns, J., Rivas, I., López-Vicente, M., Suades-González, E., Foraster, M., Garcia-Esteban, R., Basagaña, X., Viana, M., Cirach, M., Moreno, T., Alastuey, A., Sebastian-Galles, N., Nieuwenhuijsen, M., & Querol, X. (2015). Association between traffic-related air pollution in schools and cognitive development in primary school children: A prospective cohort study. PLOS Medicine, 12(3), e1001792. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmed.1001792 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Susman, E. J., Marceau, K., Dockray, S., & Ram, N. (2019). Interdisciplinary work is essential for research on puberty: Complexity and dynamism in action. Journal of Research On Adolescence, 29(1), 115132. https://doi.org/10.1111/jora.12420 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Szyszkowicz, M., Kousha, T., Kingsbury, M., & Colman, I. (2016). Air pollution and emergency department visits for depression: A multicity case-crossover study. Environmental Health Insights, 10, EHI.S40493. https://doi.org/10.4137/EHI.S40493 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Uy, J. P., & Gotlib, I. H. (in press). Associations among early life adversity, sleep disturbances, and depressive symptoms in adolescent females and males: A longitudinal investigation. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry.Google Scholar
Uy, J. P., Ho, T. C., Buthmann, J. L., Coury, S. M., & Gotlib, I. H. (2023). Early life stress, sleep disturbances, and depressive symptoms during adolescence: The role of the cingulum bundle. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 63, 101303. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2023.101303 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Uy, J. P., Shin, K., Buthmann, J., Kircanski, K., LeMoult, J., Berens, A., & Gotlib, I. H. (in preparation). Greater diesel-related particulate matter exposure relates to higher levels of depressive symptoms through blunted cortisol stress reactivity in adolescents.Google Scholar
Uy, J. P., Yuan, J. P., Colich, N. L., & Gotlib, I. H. (under review). Effects of pollution burden on neural function during implicit emotion regulation and longitudinal changes in depressive symptoms in adolescents.Google Scholar
Uy, J. P., Yuan, J. P., Kirshenbaum, J. S., Coury, S. M., Buthmann, J. L., Ho, T. C., & Gotlib, I. H. (in preparation). Neighborhood disadvantage, pollution burden, and inflammation during adolescence: The buffering effect of sleep.Google Scholar
WHO (2016). Ambient air pollution: A global assessment of exposure and burden of disease. World Health Organization. http://apps.who.int/iris/bitstream/10665/250141/1/9789241511353-eng.pdf Google Scholar
Wium-Andersen, M. K., Ørsted, D. D., Nielsen, S. F., & Nordestgaard, B. G. (2013). Elevated C-reactive protein levels, psychological distress, and depression in 73 131 individuals. JAMA Psychiatry, 70(2), 176. https://doi.org/10.1001/2013.jamapsychiatry.102 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Xie, X., Xue, Q., Zhou, Y., Zhu, K., Liu, Q., Zhang, J., & Song, R. (2020). Mental health status among children in home confinement during the coronavirus disease 2019 outbreak in Hubei Province. JAMA Pediatrics, 174(9), 898. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapediatrics.2020.1619 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yuan, J. P., Coury, S. M., Ho, T. C., & Gotlib, I. H. (2023). Early life stress moderates the relation between systemic inflammation and neural activation to reward in adolescents both cross-sectionally and longitudinally. Neuropsychopharmacology, 49(3), 19. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41386-023-01708-y Google ScholarPubMed
Yuan, J. P., Ho, T. C., Coury, S. M., Chahal, R., Colich, N. L., & Gotlib, I. H. (2022). Early life stress, systemic inflammation, and neural correlates of implicit emotion regulation in adolescents. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity, 105, 169179. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbi.2022.07.007 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yuan, J. P., Jaeger, E. L., Coury, S. M., Ho, T. C., & Gotlib, I. H. (under review). Socioeconomic disadvantage moderates the association of systemic inflammation with amygdala gray matter volume in adolescents over a two-year period.Google Scholar
Zhao, T., Markevych, I., Romanos, M., Nowak, D., & Heinrich, J. (2018). Ambient ozone exposure and mental health: A systematic review of epidemiological studies. Environmental Research, 165, 459472. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.envres.2018.04.015 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhou, S. J., Zhang, L. G., Wang, L. L., Guo, Z. C., Wang, J. Q., Chen, J. C., Liu, M., Chen, X., & Chen, J. X. (2020). Prevalence and socio-demographic correlates of psychological health problems in chinese adolescents during the outbreak of COVID-19. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 29(6), 749758. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-020-01541-4 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Figure 1. Constructs originally assessed in our early life stress study.

Figure 1

Figure 2. Constructs added to our early life stress study.