Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-p9bg8 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-28T03:39:52.342Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Verbal mediation of cognition in children with specific language impairment

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  17 April 2012

Jane S. M. Lidstone*
Affiliation:
Durham University
Elizabeth Meins
Affiliation:
Durham University
Charles Fernyhough
Affiliation:
Durham University
*
Address correspondence and reprint requests to: Jane Lidstone, School of Psychology, Cardiff University, Tower Building, Park Place, Cardiff, CF10 3AT, UK; E-mail: LidstoneJ@cardiff.ac.uk.

Abstract

Private speech (PS) and inner speech (IS) are thought to be functionally important for children's and adults’ cognition, but they have not been studied systematically in children with specific language impairment (SLI). Participants were 21 children with SLI (7–11 years, expressive or receptive verbal IQ ≤ 75, nonverbal IQ ≥ 84) and 21 age- and nonverbal IQ-matched controls. Participants completed three sets of Tower of London problems: one with no dual task (PS condition), one with articulatory suppression, and one while foot tapping (control condition). Participants also completed a digit span task. There was no group difference in the susceptibility of Tower of London performance to articulatory suppression, but the PS of the SLI group was less internalized than that of the controls on both tasks. The findings suggest that children with SLI experience a significant delay in the development of PS/IS, but that their PS/IS is effective for Tower of London performance in middle childhood. Findings are discussed with reference to the interpretation of the nonlinguistic deficits associated with SLI, and in terms of clinical implications.

Type
Regular Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2012

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Akshoomoff, N., Stiles, J., & Wulfeck, B. (2006). Perceptual organization and visual immediate memory in children with specific language impairment. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 12, 465474.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baker, S. C., Rogers, R. D., Owen, A. M., Frith, C. D., Dolan, R. J., Frackowiak, R. S. J., et al. (1996). Neural systems engaged by planning: A PET study of the Tower of London task. Neuropsychologia, 34, 515526.Google Scholar
Baldo, J. V., Dronkers, N. F., Wilkins, D., Lucy, C., Raskin, P., & Kim, J. Y. (2005). Is problem solving dependent on language? Brain and Language, 92, 240250.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bavin, E. L., Wilson, P. H., Maruff, P., & Sleeman, F. (2005). Spatio–visual memory of children with specific language impairment: Evidence for generalized processing problems. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 40, 319332.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Behrend, D. A., Rosengren, K., & Perlmutter, M. (1989). A new look at children's private speech: The effects of age, task difficulty, and parent presence. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 12, 305320.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Behrend, D. A., Rosengren, K. S., & Perlmutter, M. (1992). The relation between private speech and parental interactive style. In Diaz, R. M. & Berk, L. E. (Eds.), Private speech: From social interaction to self-regulation (pp. 85100). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Berk, L. E. (1986). Relationship of elementary school children's private speech to behavioral accompaniment to task, attention, and task performance. Developmental Psychology, 22, 671680.Google Scholar
Berk, L. E., & Spuhl, S. T. (1995). Maternal interaction, private speech, and task-performance in preschool children. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 10, 145169.Google Scholar
Bishop, D. V. M. (2003). Test for reception of grammar (version 2). London: Harcourt Assessment.Google Scholar
Bishop, D. V. M., & Norbury, C. F. (2005a). Executive functions in children with communication impairments, in relation to autistic symptomatology—2: Response inhibition. Autism, 9, 2943.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bishop, D. V. M., & Norbury, C. F. (2005b). Executive functions in children with communication impairments, in relation to autistic symptomatology—1: Generativity. Autism, 9, 727.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bivens, J. A., & Berk, L. E. (1990). A longitudinal study of the development of elementary school children's private speech. Merrill–Palmer Quarterly, 36, 443463.Google Scholar
Boghi, A., Rasetti, R., Avidano, F., Manzone, C., Orsi, L., D'Agata, F., et al. (2006). The effect of gender on planning: An fMRI study using the Tower of London task. NeuroImage, 33, 9991010.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carlin, D., Bonerba, J., Phipps, M., Alexander, G., Shapiro, M., & Grafman, J. (2000). Planning impairments in frontal lobe dementia and frontal lobe lesion patients. Neuropsychologia, 38, 655665.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chincotta, M., & Chincotta, D. (1996). Digit span, articulatory suppression, and the deaf: A study of the Hong Kong Chinese. American Annals of the Deaf, 141, 252257.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Diaz, R. M., & Berk, L. E. (1995). A Vygotskian critique of self-instructional training. Development and Psychopathology, 7, 369392.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Diaz, R. M., Winsler, A., Atencio, D. J., & Harbers, K. (1992). Mediation of self-regulation through the use of private speech. International Journal of Cognitive Education and Mediated Learning, 2, 155167.Google Scholar
Donlan, C., Cowan, R., Newton, E. J., & Lloyd, D. (2007). The role of language in mathematical development: Evidence from children with specific language impairments. Cognition, 103, 2333.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duncan, R., & Pratt, M. (1997). Microgenetic change in the quality and quantity of preschoolers’ private speech. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 20, 367383.Google Scholar
Duncan, R., & Tarulli, D. (2009). On the persistence of private speech: Empirical and theoretical considerations. In Winsler, A., Fernyhough, C., & Montero, I. (Eds.), Private speech, executive functioning, and the development of verbal self-regulation (pp. 176187). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Elliott, C. D., Smith, P., & McCullough, K. (1996). British Ability Scales (2nd ed.). London: NFER Nelson.Google Scholar
Emerson, M. J., & Miyake, A. (2003). The role of inner speech in task switching: A dual-task investigation. Journal of Memory and Language, 48, 148168.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Farrant, B. M., Fletcher, J., & Maybery, M. T. (2006). Specific language impairment, theory of mind, and visual perspective taking: Evidence for simulation theory and the developmental role of language. Child Development, 77, 18421853.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fernyhough, C. (1996). The dialogic mind: A dialogic approach to the higher mental functions. New Ideas in Psychology, 14, 4762.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fernyhough, C. (2010). Vygotsky, Luria, and the social brain. In Sokol, B. W., Müller, U., Carpendale, J. I. M., Young, A. R., & Iarocci, G. (Eds.), Self- and social-regulation; Social interaction and the development of social understanding and executive functions (pp. 5679). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Fernyhough, C., & Fradley, E. (2005). Private speech on an executive task: Relations with task difficulty and task performance. Cognitive Development, 20, 103120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fernyhough, C., & Meins, E. (2009). Private speech and theory of mind: Evidence for developing interfunctional relations. In Winsler, A., Fernyhough, C., & Montero, I. (Eds.), Private speech, executive functioning, and the development of verbal self-regulation (pp. 95104). New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Finneran, D. A., Francis, A. L., & Leonard, L. B. (2009). Sustained attention in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 52, 915929.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fujiki, M., Brinton, B., & Clarke, D. (2002). Emotion regulation in children with specific language impairment. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 33, 102111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fujiki, M., Spackman, M. P., Brinton, B., & Hall, A. (2004). The relationship of language and emotion regulation skills to reticence in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 47, 637646.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gillam, R. B., Montgomery, J. W., & Gillam, S. L. (2009). Attention and memory in child language disorders. In Schwartz, R. G. (Ed.), Hand-book of child language disorders (pp. 201215). Hove: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Holland, L., & Low, J. (2010). Do children with autism use inner speech and visuospatial resources for the service of executive control? Evidence from suppression in dual tasks. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 28, 369391.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Im-Bolter, N., Johnson, J., & Pascual-Leone, J. (2006). Processing limitations in children with specific language impairment: The role of executive function. Child Development, 77, 18221841.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnston, J. R. (1994). Cognitive abilities of language impaired children. In Watkins, R. & Rice, M. (Eds.), Specific language impairments in children (pp. 107122). Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes.Google Scholar
Kaller, C. P., Unterrainer, J. M., Rahm, B., & Halsband, U. (2004). The impact of problem structure on planning: Insights from the Tower of London task. Cognitive Brain Research, 20, 462472.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, H. S. (2002). We talk, therefore we think? A cultural analysis of the effect of talking on thinking. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 828842.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kochanska, G., Murray, K. T., & Harlan, E. T. (2000). Effortful control in early childhood: Continuity and change, antecedents, and implications for social development. Developmental Psychology, 36, 220232.Google Scholar
Landry, S. H., Miller-Loncar, C. L., Smith, K. E., & Swank, P. R. (2002). The role of early parenting in children's development of executive processes. Developmental Neuropsychology, 21, 1541.Google Scholar
Lengua, L. J., Honorado, E., & Bush, N. R. (2007). Contextual risk and parenting as predictors of effortful control and social competence in preschool children. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 28, 4055.Google Scholar
Leonard, L. B. (2000). Children with specific language impairment. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google ScholarPubMed
Lidstone, J. (2010). Private speech and inner speech in typical and atypical development. Unpublished doctoral thesis, Durham University.Google Scholar
Lidstone, J., Meins, E., & Fernyhough, C. (2010). The roles of private speech and inner speech in planning in middle childhood: Evidence from a dual task paradigm. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 107, 438451.Google Scholar
Lidstone, J. S. M., Fernyhough, C., Meins, E., & Whitehouse, A. J. O. (2009). Inner speech impairment in children with autism is associated with greater nonverbal than verbal skills. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39, 12221225.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mainela-Arnold, E., Evans, J. L., & Alibali, M. W. (2006). Understanding conservation delays in children with specific language impairment: Task representations revealed in speech and gesture. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 49, 12671279.Google Scholar
Müller, U., Zelazo, P. D., Hood, S., Leone, T., & Rohrer, L. (2004). Interference control in a new rule use task: Age-related changes, labeling, and attention. Child Development, 75, 15941609.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Owen, A. M., Sahakian, B. J., Hodges, J. R., Summers, B. A., Polkey, C. E., & Robbins, T. W. (1995). Dopamine-dependent fronto–striatal planning deficits in early Parkinson's disease. Neuropsychology, 9, 126140.Google Scholar
Semel, E., Wiig, E. H., & Secord, W. A. (2006). Clinical evaluation of language fundamentals (4th UK ed.). London: Harcourt Assessment.Google Scholar
Shallice, T. (1982). Specific impairments of planning. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, Biology, 298, 199209.Google ScholarPubMed
Sturn, A., & Johnston, J. (1999). Thinking out loud: An exploration of problem-solving language in preschoolers with and without language impairment. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 34, 115.Google Scholar
Towse, J. N., Hitch, G. J., & Hutton, U. (1998). A reevaluation of working memory capacity in children. Journal of Memory and Language, 39, 195217.Google Scholar
Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). In Cole, M., John-Steiner, V., Scribner, S., & Souberman, E. (Eds.), Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. (Original work published 1930, 1933, and 1935)Google Scholar
Vygotsky, L. S. (1987). Thinking and speech. In Rieber, R. W. & Carton, A. S. (Eds.), The collected works of L. S. Vygotsky (Vol. 1). New York: Plenum Press. (Original work published 1934)Google Scholar
Wallace, G. L., Silvers, J. A., Martin, A., & Kenworthy, L. E. (2009). Brief report: Further evidence for inner speech deficits in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39, 17351739.Google Scholar
Weckerly, J., Wulfeck, B., & Reilly, J. (2001). Verbal fluency deficits in children with specific language impairment: Slow rapid naming or slow to name? Child Neuropsychology, 7, 142152.Google Scholar
Whitehouse, A. J. O., Maybery, M. T., & Durkin, K. (2006). Inner speech impairments in autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47, 857865.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Williams, D., Bowler, D. M., & Jarrold, C. (in press). Inner speech is used to mediate short-term memory, but not planning, among intellectually high-functioning adults with autism spectrum disorder. Development and Psychopathology.Google Scholar
Windsor, J., & Kohnert, K. (2009). Processing speed, attention, and perception in child language disorders. In Schwartz, R. G. (Ed.), Handbook of child language disorders (pp. 445462). Hove: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Windsor, J., Kohnert, K., Loxtercamp, A. L., & Kan, P. F. (2008). Performance on nonlinguistic visual tasks by children with language impairment. Applied Psycholinguistics, 29, 237268.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Winsler, A. (2009). Still talking to ourselves after all these years: A review of current research on private speech. In Winsler, A., Fernyhough, C., & Montero, I. (Eds.), Private speech, executive functioning, and the development of verbal self-regulation (pp. 341). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Winsler, A., Abar, B., Feder, M. A., Schunn, C. D., & Rubio, D. A. (2007). Private speech and executive functioning among high-functioning children with autistic spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 16171635.Google Scholar
Winsler, A., De León, J.R., Wallace, B., Carlton, M. P., & Willson-Quayle, A. (2003). Private speech in preschool children: Developmental stability and change, across-task consistency, and relations with classroom behavior. Journal of Child Language, 30, 583608.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Winsler, A., Diaz, R. M., McCarthy, E. M., Atencio, D., & Adams Chabay, L. (1999). Mother–child interaction, private speech, and task performance in preschool children with behavior problems. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 40, 891904.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Winsler, A., Diaz, R. M., & Montero, I. (1997). The role of private speech in the transition from collaborative to independent task performance in young children. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 12, 5979.Google Scholar
Winsler, A., Fernyhough, C., McClaren, E. M., & Way, E. (2005). Private speech coding manual. Unpublished manuscript, George Mason University, Department of Psychology. Retrieved from http://classweb.gmu.edu/awinsler/PSCodingManual.pdfGoogle Scholar