Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-hc48f Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-25T19:52:48.252Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Trust and Public Health Emergency Events: A Mixed-Methods Systematic Review

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 June 2021

Pradeep Sopory*
Affiliation:
Wayne State University, Detroit, MichiganUSA
Julie M. Novak
Affiliation:
Wayne State University, Detroit, MichiganUSA
Ashleigh M. Day
Affiliation:
University of Texas at Tyler, Tyler, TexasUSA
Stine Eckert
Affiliation:
Wayne State University, Detroit, MichiganUSA
Lee Wilkins
Affiliation:
Wayne State University, Detroit, MichiganUSA
Donyale R. Padgett
Affiliation:
Wayne State University, Detroit, MichiganUSA
Jane P. Noyes
Affiliation:
Bangor University, Bangor, United Kingdom
Tomas Allen
Affiliation:
World Health Organization, Geneva, Switzerland
Nyka Alexander
Affiliation:
World Health Organization, Geneva, Switzerland
Marsha L. Vanderford
Affiliation:
World Health Organization, Geneva, Switzerland
Gaya M. Gamhewage
Affiliation:
World Health Organization, Geneva, Switzerland
*
Corresponding author: Pradeep Sopory, Email: dz3594@wayne.edu.
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

The systematic review examined the phenomenon of trust during public health emergency events. The literature reviewed was field studies done with people directly affected or likely to be affected by such events and included quantitative, qualitative, mixed-method, and case study primary studies in English (N = 38) as well as Arabic, Chinese, French, Russian, and Spanish (all non-English N = 30). Studies were mostly from high- and middle-income countries, and the event most covered was infectious disease. Findings from individual studies were first synthesized within methods and evaluated for certainty/confidence, and then synthesized across methods. The final set of 11 findings synthesized across methods identified a set of activities for enhancing trust and showed that it is a multi-faceted and dynamic concept.

Type
Systematic Review
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
© Society for Disaster Medicine and Public Health, Inc. 2021

The role of trust during the course of public health emergency events, such as emergent infectious disease pandemics, is complex and encompasses multiple stakeholders involved in managing the event. In particular, trust in authorities as well as in the information being conveyed is multi-faceted in the ways it can influence the behavior of the public. Additionally, trust operates in a variety of social-structural contexts when events escalate from local and regional levels to national and global problems. As such, a comprehensive examination of the phenomenon of trust during public health emergency events is needed. To this end, we conducted a systematic review of primary studies on the topic.

Although there were existing reviews related to the topic, Reference Cairns, de Andrade and MacDonald1Reference Wachinger, Renn and Begg7 the present review took a systematic approach to examine the broader phenomenon of trust during public health emergency events. To achieve this, the review looked at studies conducted in the field (contrasted to the laboratory) that measured, observed, or described trust in all affected populations, including publics, communities, and organizations. Thus, the present review not only reviewed evidence from studies that had comparison groups, but also studies that examined factors that may have an association with the concepts/variables contained in the phenomenon of interest, seeing these factors to be potentially associated with trust to find out how trust functions and for whom and in what contexts.

The present review focused on data from multiple methods from field studies of populations that directly experienced a relevant public health emergency event. Also of interest were data from studies of populations who may be likely to be affected by particular public health emergency events, especially studies that examined individual preparedness for such events. Of interest also were data from studies that addressed how organizations, predominantly government organizations or individuals employed by governments, respond to or work to develop public communication messages. As such, the scope of the review was limited to (a) primary studies, (b) done in the field (as opposed to laboratories), (c) with people and organizations directly affected or likely to be affected by public health emergency events.

Methods

The process of evidence synthesis for the present mixed-methods systematic review is similar to and fully described for a related systematic review. Reference Sopory, Day and Novak8 An overview of the steps and any modifications to the process are presented below.

Methodological Streams and Language

After an iterative process of close reading of the literature, 4 methodological streams were adopted for the review: Quantitative-Comparison Groups (QN-CG); Quantitative-Descriptive Survey (QN-DS); Qualitative (QL); and Mixed-Method and Case Study (MM, CS). These constituted the 4 methodological streams for the review.

The primary search was for literature in the English language. Additionally, we conducted searches for studies published in the other United Nations (UN) languages as well, which included Arabic, Chinese, French, Russian, and Spanish. Because we translated only portions of the studies in these languages into English, we treated findings from these studies as a separate “sub-stream” at the time of synthesis of findings within methodological streams.

Search Databases, Terms, and Criteria

In addition to a Google Scholar and a general Google search, we also conducted a search using the academic library Summon function, which searches all holdings in the library as well as several databases including: Academic Search Complete, Communication and Mass Media Complete (CMMC); ArticleFirst, JSTOR; PsychInfo, Science Direct, Scopus, SpringerLink, Taylor & Francis, and Wiley Online. We also searched in Cumulative Index of Nursing and Allied Health Literature (CINAHL); CINAHL Complete; Elsevier; PubMed/Medline-National Library of Medicine (NLM); Web of Science; and WHO databases. Native readers of Arabic, Chinese, French, Russian, and Spanish who were fluent in English conducted the search for non-English language primary studies in databases with holdings in these languages.

The search terms are shown in Table 1. Not all terms worked in all databases; therefore, thesauri were consulted for each database to find synonyms and related terms, if they existed, for each term, or any functionality that allowed the word to be “exploded” or “expanded.” The following inclusion criteria were used:

  • Research related to the practice of risk communication and the process of disaster management with no preference for any specific emergency or health hazards.

  • Research within the viewpoint or scope set by the risk communication field related to: trust, uncertainty, communities, health, misinformation, health protection, media (including social media), messages, and stakeholders.

Table 1. Search terms

The following exclusion criteria were used to keep a focus on trust during public health emergency events:

  • Research in organizational risk communication and disaster management, such as technology failures.

  • Research outside of the specified scope of the study, such as laboratory studies and those related to chronic disease, lifestyle, or personal living/attributes (such as personal health, mental health, etc.).

  • Studies published before 2003. This cutoff was used to focus on current research.

Study Selection and Quality Appraisal

Only data-based primary articles and reports from all methodologies were selected. The selection process broadly conformed to the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Analyses (PRISMA) process. Reference Moher, Liberati and Tetzlaff9 Selected articles and reports were judged for different levels of relevancy to the review objective and phenomena of interest. Reference Lewin, Glenton and Munthe-Kaas10,Reference Noyes, Booth and Lewin11 Studies were judged to have direct relevance (ie, directly mapped onto phenomenon of interest), indirect relevance (ie, corresponded with some aspects of the phenomenon of interest), partial relevance (ie, a part of the issue of interest or population was addressed but not all), or unclear relevance (ie, unclear whether underlying data were relevant) with the review topic. A study judged as directly, indirectly, partially, or unclearly relevant (as opposed to not relevant at all) was selected for extraction of its key findings. Only these relevant (direct, indirect, partial, unclear) primary study articles/reports were used to generate the systematic review for this report.

The individual data-based primary studies selected for the review were appraised for their quality using available method-specific tools. These tools ascertain quality through a series of questions that identify concerns about methodological limitations that can amplify threats to rigor (qualitative research) or risk of bias (quantitative research). The following tools were used: Quantitative control/comparison groups studies were appraised using the Effective Practice and Organization of Care (EPOC) 9-criteria risk of bias tool 12 (see section 12.2.2 of the Cochrane Handbook for definitions of levels of risk) Reference Higgins and Green13 ; Quantitative descriptive survey studies were appraised using an adapted version of survey quality appraisal criteria that note reporting or nonreporting of sampling, response rate, validity and reliability, sources of data, content and focus of study, and relevancy to the corresponding question information to determine categories of weak, moderate, and strong quality Reference Davids and Roman14 ; Qualitative studies were appraised using Critical Appraisal Skills Programme (CASP) checklist that assesses appropriateness of qualitative methodology, data collection, relationship between research and participants, ethics, rigor of data analysis, clarity of findings, and value of research using “yes.” “no,” and “can’t tell” to determine 4 categories of very low, low, moderate, and high quality 15 ; Mixed method and case study studies were appraised using Mixed Methods Appraisal Tool (MMAT) that assesses areas relevant to each type of methodology (eg, quantitative descriptive, qualitative) using “yes,” “no,” and “can’t tell” to determine an overall 4 categories of very low, low, moderate, and high quality Reference Pluye, Robert and Cargo16 ; and media reports were appraised for their quality using the Authority, Accuracy, Coverage, Objectivity, Date, and Significance (AACODS) tool that assesses the 6 areas noted in the tool title using “yes.” “no,” and “can’t tell” to determine 4 categories of very low, low, moderate, and high quality. Reference Tyndall17

Data Extraction

Given the heterogeneity of methods, as recommended in section 11.7.2 of the Cochrane Handbook dealing with situations where quantitative meta-analyses are not possible to conduct, Reference Higgins and Green13 we followed a narrative summary approach Reference Higgins and Green13,Reference Popay, Roberts and Sowden18 to extract findings from studies in all 4 methodological streams. For qualitative studies the narrative summary approach was an initial step and the final step included reading the entire article to extract the data. Each finding along with supporting information was extracted in the form of short 3- to 5-sentence paragraphs. The findings focused on the phenomena of interest broadly and any outcomes/impacts noted specifically, and the support for each finding was in the form of quantitative and qualitative information. In addition, the following study characteristics were also extracted: method; country focus; disaster/emergency type; disaster/emergency phase; and at-risk/vulnerable population inclusion.

Synthesis of Findings

The synthesis of findings was done in 2 stages. In the first stage, findings from individual studies were synthesized within methodological streams and then these within-method synthesized findings were evaluated for certainty/confidence using appropriate tools. In the second stage, the within-method synthesized findings were synthesized across methodological streams, taking into account the certainty/confidence evaluations by making studies with higher evaluations more salient in the synthesis process. In both the within-method and across-method stages, the synthesis of findings included subgroup analyses. These included examination of type of emergency event, phase of emergency event, country of emergency event, and presence of vulnerable population. The last 2 subgroups allowed considerations of equity in the synthesized findings.

Synthesis of Findings Within Each Methodological Stream

For each methodological stream, the synthesized findings were created by building explanatory and higher level analytical statements supported by quantitative and qualitative evidence from individual studies. For the 2 quantitative methodological streams, we followed a narrative summary approach to synthesis of findings. For the qualitative methodological stream, we broadly followed the framework synthesis method, Reference Barnett-Page and Thomas19,Reference Pope, Ziebland and Mays20 which is a mix of deductive-inductive processes. For the mixed-method and case study methodological stream, the individual studies typically did not differentiate their overall findings based on type of methodology and so we looked at the findings holistically following a broadly narrative summary approach.

The assessment of certainty/confidence of synthesized findings was done separately for each methodological stream. Quantitative-comparison groups within-method synthesized findings were assessed using the Grading of Recommendations Assessment, Development, and Evaluation (GRADE) approach. Reference Higgins and Green13,Reference Working Group21,Reference Guyatt, Oxman and Aki22 Quantitative-descriptive survey within-method synthesized findings, which did not have comparison groups for outcomes of interest, were assessed using a tool developed for the present review that was based on the principles of GRADE as noted above. Qualitative within-method synthesized findings were assessed using GRADE-Confidence in the Evidence from Reviews of Qualitative research (GRADE-CERQual). Reference Lewin, Glenton and Munthe-Kaas10 Mixed method and case study within-method synthesized findings were assessed using principles of GRADE and GRADE-CERQual approaches as appropriate. We want to note here that the adaptation of GRADE principles for application to descriptive quantitative studies and use of GRADE-CERQual principles for application to mixed-method studies has not been approved by the tool originators.

Synthesis of Findings Across Methodological Streams

We synthesized the findings across the 4 methodological streams to develop an overarching synthesis of findings. The synthesized findings within a methodological stream were compared and contrasted with findings from the other methodological streams. Whenever the findings supported and amplified each other, they were combined into higher order findings that represented synthesis across the method streams. The evaluation of certainty in the within-method synthesized findings was kept in mind during this process by making findings with higher evaluations more salient in the synthesis process.

All methodological streams did not yield the same kind or similar number of synthesized findings. We did not consider this a problematic issue as we were seeking to find the points of alignment of the findings across the method streams rather than simply merging them together, which would have given some methodological streams more importance than others. Within-method findings that did not contribute to an across-method higher order finding were analyzed thematically. These thematic analyses were used to uncover a nuance or modification to the across-method findings, which were then either used to create a new higher order across-method finding or incorporated into an existing across-method finding.

A few synthesized findings within a methodological stream provided evidence that countered the synthesized findings from other methodological streams. Whenever this happened, we strived to retain this finding as a separate finding in the final set of across-method findings or used it to modify an existing across-method finding.

Results

Study Selection

For literature in English language, approximately 4300 titles and abstracts were identified and scanned, of which almost 2900 full-texts were quickly read to identify meeting of eligibility criteria. After this, 74 full-texts were downloaded, of which 38 data-based primary field studies were selected for data extraction. These study selection process details and those for Arabic, Chinese, French, Russian, and Spanish languages are provided in Figure 1.

Figure 1. Study selection.

Study Characteristics

Of the 38 English language studies examined for the present review, 18 were directly relevant, 13 were indirectly relevant, 7 were partially relevant, and none were unclearly relevant. Two studies used quantitative-comparison groups method, 21 studies used quantitative descriptive survey methods, 8 studies used qualitative methods, and 7 used mixed methods/case study methods.

Of the 30 other UN languages (ie, not English) data-based primary studies, 3 were in Arabic, 7 in Chinese, 15 in French, 3 in Russian, and 2 in Spanish. A total of 19 studies were directly relevant and 11 were indirectly relevant; the relevancy was judged as only direct and indirect due to lack of sufficient clarity for the partial and unclear categories for the coders. Other study characteristics, including, country, types of disasters/emergencies, disaster/emergency event phase, and populations studied for both English and other UN languages are provided in Table 2.

Table 2. Characteristics of studies

Notes. Some categories are not mutually exclusive and so the frequencies will not sum to the total of 38 (English language) and 30 (other UN languages). Method: Quantitative-Comparison Groups (QN-CG); Quantitative-Descriptive Survey (QN-DS); Qualitative (QL); Mixed-Method/Case Study (MM, CS).

Quality Appraisal of Individual Studies

Of the 38 English language studies used in the present review, 2 were placed in the quantitative-comparison group stream, 21 in the quantitative-descriptive survey stream, 8 in the qualitative stream, and 7 in the mixed methods/case studies stream. Within the quantitative-comparison groups stream, both studies were trials and were rated to be of moderate quality. In the quantitative-descriptive survey stream, 7 studies were rated to be strong quality, 10 were rated to be moderate quality, and 4 were rated to be of weak quality. In the qualitative methods stream, 2 studies were rated to be of high quality, 5 of moderate quality, and 1 of low quality. In the mixed methods/case studies methods stream, 2 studies were rated to be of high quality, 3 of moderate quality, and 2 of low quality. For the other UN languages individual studies, a quality appraisal could not be determined for all the studies.

Synthesis of Findings Within Methodological Stream

Findings from individual studies, both English and other UN languages, were put into 4 method streams, quantitative comparison group, quantitative descriptive survey, qualitative, and mixed method/case study. The findings within each method stream were synthesized using the procedures described above. An individual study could support more than 1 synthesized finding. Most synthesized findings were supported by multiple studies though a few were supported by only 1 study. There were total 41 synthesized findings within method streams. The findings are detailed in Table 3 along with the evaluations of certainty/confidence for each finding.

Table 3. Synthesis of findings within methodological stream and evaluation of certainty/confidence

Notes. Quantitative-Comparison Groups (QN-CG); Quantitative-Descriptive Survey (QN-DS); Qualitative (QL); Mixed-Method/Case Study (MM, CS). Citations: English has no suffix; Arabic (AR); Chinese (CH); French (FR); Russian (RU); Spanish (SP). Certainty/confidence evaluation categories are high, moderate, low, and very low.

There were 2 synthesized findings in the quantitative comparison group stream. Each was supported by a single study. The countries covered included Japan and the United States. Infectious disease and radiological events were covered; the phases covered were onset, containment, and recovery. No vulnerable populations were studied. The evaluation of certainty in the findings ranged from low to moderate.

There were 20 synthesized findings in the quantitative descriptive survey stream. Four findings were supported by only a single study, whereas the rest were supported by multiple studies. The countries covered included Australia, Belgium, Canada, China, France, India, Japan, Netherlands, New Zealand, Norway, Oman, Saudi Arabia, Slovenia, Spain, Sweden, Switzerland, Thailand, the United Kingdom, several European Union countries, the United States, and Vietnam. Bioterrorism, climate change-related severe weather, cyclone, earthquake, flood, foodborne illness, infectious disease, general natural disaster, industrial accident, radiological, tsunami, volcanic, water contamination, and wildfire events were covered. All 4 phases of a disaster event were covered along with evaluation. Vulnerable populations were covered in 3 findings. The evaluation of certainty in the findings ranged from low to high, with the majority being moderate.

There were 10 synthesized findings in the qualitative stream. One finding was supported by only a single study whereas the rest were supported by multiple studies. The countries covered included Canada, China, France, Iran, Russia, the United Kingdom, and the United States. Bioterrorism, earthquake, floods, foodborne illnesses, infectious disease, and radiological events were covered. All 4 phases of an event were covered along with evaluation. Vulnerable populations were covered in 3 findings. The evaluation of confidence in the findings ranged from low to high, with the majority being moderate.

There were 9 synthesized findings in the mixed methods/case study stream. Three findings were supported by only a single study, whereas the rest were supported by multiple studies. The countries covered included Canada, the Caribbean nations, France, Germany, Indonesia, Japan, Philippines, Russia, Singapore, Spain, several European Union countries, and the United States. Bioterrorism, earthquake, floods, foodborne illnesses, infectious disease, and radiological events were covered. All 4 phases of an event were covered along with evaluation. Vulnerable populations were covered in 1 of the findings. The evaluation of certainty/confidence in the findings ranged from low to high, with the majority being moderate.

Synthesis of Findings Across Methodological Streams

The 41 within method synthesized findings were further synthesized yielding a final set of 11 synthesized findings across the 4 method streams. Of these, 1 synthesized finding was based on all 4 method streams, 4 synthesized findings were based on 3 method streams, 4 synthesized findings were based on 2 method streams, and 2 synthesized findings were based on just 1 method stream. We wish to note here that the across-method synthesis sought to identify commonalities in themes across the method streams but at the same time it allowed for findings that were unique to not get subsumed under more general themes; this resulted in 2 synthesized findings that drew only from 1 method stream.

The quantitative comparison group within-method synthesized findings appeared in 2 across-method findings, quantitative descriptive survey within-method synthesized findings appeared in 9 across-method findings, qualitative within-method synthesized findings appeared in 10 across-method synthesized findings, and mixed method/case study within-method synthesized findings appeared in 5 across-method findings.

There was coverage of a large number of countries, but countries in Africa and South America were not represented at all. The coverage of different types of events was adequate and all 4 phases of an event (preparation, onset, containment, and recovery) along with evaluation were covered. Vulnerable populations appeared in all the findings.

Core Aspects of Across-Method Synthesized Findings

The review examined the phenomenon of trust in the context of public health emergency events. The final 11 across-method synthesized findings covered various features of the phenomenon, with a focus on trust in authorities, and are discussed in full in Table 4. The core aspects of each finding are presented next.

Table 4. Synthesis of findings across methodological streams

Notes. Citations-language: English has no suffix; Arabic (AR); Chinese (CH); French (FR); Russian (RU); Spanish (SP). Certainty/confidence evaluation categories are high, moderate, low, and very low. Only English language studies considered for certainty/confidence evaluation.

  1. 1. Trust in authorities is a multi-component construct and not a singular concept. It is important to distinguish among and account for these components, and not treat trust as a unidimensional concept, to fully explicate the processes through which trust may be enhanced. Some examples of components are: reliability, competence, openness, and integrity; fiduciary responsibility, honesty, competency, consistency, and faith; and confidence in government preparedness; allocation of resources; expectations of government; honesty; disclosure; dedication/commitment; and caring/empathy. Reference Haynes, Barclay and Pidgeon23Reference Wray, Rivers and Jupka30 Trust can also be conceptualized as critical trust, which is that people can trust a person or institution for information and action but combine this with a healthy skepticism. Reference Petts and Niemeyer26

  2. 2. High trust in authorities can lead to both positive and negative psychological and behavioral outcomes. The positive outcomes of high trust include higher: investment in event warning and control; health protection behaviors; vaccination behaviors; preparation, but only if benefits are clear; evacuation; attention to news; message acceptance; and willingness to attend public meetings. Reference van der Weerd, Timmermans and Beaujean29,Reference Besley, McComas and Trumbo31Reference Su, Sun and Zhao39 The negative outcomes of high trust may include higher: fearfulness; uncertainty; and discounting of probability estimates of event occurrence, if event control mechanisms are effective. Reference Vaughan, Tinker and Truman28,Reference Janmaimool and Watanabe40,Reference Flood41 Along the same lines, low trust can also lead to negative outcomes, such as anxiety and lack of preparation. Reference Wray, Rivers and Jupka30,Reference Bass, Greener and Ruggieri42Reference Xie, Yang and Ou44

  3. 3. Trust in authorities is a strong predictor of risk perceptions. Generally, there is a linear negative relationship between trust in authorities and perceived risk of a hazard (higher trust, lower perceived risk), although the strength of the relationship may change based on the component of trust, type of organization, event type, demographics, and personal or global risk. The trust-risk perception relationship can be a positive one (higher trust, higher perceived risk) for citizen groups and climate change induced severe weather. However, the relationship between trust and risk perceptions may be more complex. For example, perceiving high credibility for industry and state health departments, and perceiving low credibility for citizen groups, may promote heuristic processing, which in turn may lead to perception of lower risk; in contrast, perceiving low credibility for industry and state health departments may promote greater systematic processing, which in turn may lead to perception of greater risk. Reference Siegrist, Gutscher and Earle27,Reference Wray, Rivers and Jupka30,Reference Su, Sun and Zhao39,Reference Janmaimool and Watanabe40,Reference Bass, Greener and Ruggieri42Reference Viklund48 It is also important to note that, although trust in authorities can be a significant source of variation in perceived risk, the amount of variation in perceived risk explained by trust is small and most of the variation remains unexplained or can be explained by other factors. Reference Janmaimool and Watanabe40,Reference Viklund48

  4. 4. Trust varies greatly across different message sources, with people usually assessing differently the credibility of 3 information sources: industry, citizen groups, and health-related departments. In general, local health-care workers and agencies, personal health professionals, friends, neighbors, and relatives, local self-help and community groups, and scientists are the more trusted sources; also in general, local elected authorities and politicians, government officials, industry, and media are relatively less trusted sources. Reference Meredith, Eisenman and Rhodes24,Reference Petts and Niemeyer26,Reference Wray, Rivers and Jupka30,Reference Freimuth, Musa and Hilyard33,Reference Bass, Greener and Ruggieri42,Reference Boon45,Reference Trumbo and McComas47,Reference Frewer and Miles49Reference Zhong and Ye53 It is important to note that there may be different levels of trust in different agencies of the government, with higher trust in those arms of the government that are perceived as nonbiased. Reference Wray, Rivers and Jupka30,Reference Kjaernes50 Within different media sources, trust varies between traditional and digital/social media sources, with trust in traditional media (eg, television news) relatively higher than social media (eg, Twitter). Reference Bitsch, Koković and Rombach32,Reference Fernandez Souto54Reference Xie, Wang and Ren61 For social media, Facebook (compared with Twitter and print newspaper) can result in a more positive perception of organizational reputation. Reference Utz, Schultz and Glocka62

  5. 5. Trust in authorities varies across the course of an emergency event, type of hazard, and demographics. Trust in different information sources may be dependent upon the phase of an event. Generally, trust is usually high at the start of an event but can get eroded as the event progresses. Reference Meredith, Eisenman and Rhodes24,Reference van der Weerd, Timmermans and Beaujean29,Reference Freimuth, Musa and Hilyard33,Reference Frewer and Miles49,Reference Liu and Zeng57 Trust in different information sources may also be dependent upon the nature of a specific hazard itself and the extent to which the particular hazard is perceived to be threatening at different points in time during a crisis. The fluctuation is influenced by several factors, such as history with authorities’ response to events and associated politics, inefficient response especially for recovery, poor communication, and changing nature of the event. Reference Haynes, Barclay and Pidgeon23,Reference Fernandez Souto54,Reference Maeno58,Reference Rousseau, Moreau and Dumas59,Reference Kutovaya63 Trust in the government and individual spokespersons also varies considerably across demographic groups. For example, generally a highly trusted source is one’s own physician, but at-risk/vulnerable groups such as low socioeconomic status (SES) racial and ethnic minorities may trust their own physicians less than majority groups. Similarly, trust in the early stages of an infectious disease event predicts vaccine acceptance later in the event, but only for White, non-Hispanic individuals. Reference Paek, Hilyard and Freimuth25,Reference Freimuth, Musa and Hilyard33,Reference Rundblad, Knapton and Hunter52

  6. 6. People use credibility of information sources as a primary means of resolving the conflict among multiple voices typical in a public health emergency situation. People engage in a thoughtful process of considering the credibility of multiple sources offering information and recommendations in an emergency event, at least in the preparation phase. People may avoid rushing to judgment when considering the multiple arguments surrounding crises; people remain in a “wait and watch” mode for what they consider the most accurate account of the crisis and of the best actions to take to protect themselves. Reference Meredith, Eisenman and Rhodes24,Reference Anthony, Sellnow and Millner64

  7. 7. Trust in authorities occurs in a life context and should not be seen in isolation for just a specific hazard. For example, people generally tend to perceive higher risk levels than is warranted by the scientific evidence; thus, for all hazards, there will always be a gap between the public perception of risk and the scientific estimation of risk, even when the trust in government is high. Reference Malet and Korbitz51 Similarly, the whole living environment may be risky and uncertain due to economic poverty; thus, a particular risk may be just one among many other risks. In such living circumstances, it is unrealistic to interpret a behavior just as a direct response to a single, acute hazard. Along the same lines, life circumstances include people’s local knowledge and cultural traditions. Reference Affletranger and de Richemond65Reference van Voorst68

  8. 8. Trust in authorities can depend on the extent of coordination among different agencies, institutions, and the media. Integration of local and national agencies in emergency response preparedness and communication, with an emphasis on full disclosure, action steps, and leadership, enhances trust. When health professionals, experts, and politicians have clear coordination among themselves and with the traditional and social/digital media, and all relay a uniform communication strategy, there can be higher trust in authorities. When there is a gap between information conveyed by health authorities and the media, this can lead to reduced trust. Reference Fernandez Souto54,Reference Karan, Aileen and Elaine56,Reference Rousseau, Moreau and Dumas59,Reference Jakubowski and Charpak69 Collaboration with mass and digital media is important while dealing with crisis because media can take on the spokesperson role and put attention on political responsibilities and shortcomings rather than talking about the event itself. In times of great uncertainty and with highly diverse audiences, having multiple voices is useful; however, professionals or agencies in disagreement should jointly discuss in public the rationale and processes by which they come to their conclusions to build trust. If a coordinated effort is not undertaken, media can take the spokesperson role of presenting the doubts and disagreements about definitive recommendations expressed by various organizations and public health experts, which can lead to distrust. Reference Meredith, Eisenman and Rhodes24,Reference Wray, Rivers and Jupka30,Reference Bitsch, Koković and Rombach32,Reference Malet and Korbitz51,Reference Fernandez Souto54Reference Karan, Aileen and Elaine56,Reference Maeno58,Reference Rousseau, Moreau and Dumas59,Reference Anthony, Sellnow and Millner64,Reference Quinn, Thomas and Kumar70,Reference Wilkinson71

  9. 9. Past experience with authorities contributes to perceptions of trust. Distrust of the government and nongovernment aid groups is related to problems (eg, mismanagement, inefficiency, incompetence) with recovery efforts in previous events. Distrust can also stem from questioning the intentions of authorities based on past experiences. Distrust in authorities is also shaped by past, disappointing experiences regarding minimization of health hazards, that turned out to be incorrect, in official communications during the early phases of previous events. Along the same lines, credibility of messages in a current hazard event can affect credibility of and the response to warnings in the next future event if sufficient uncertainty about the predictions is not included in the messages. Reference Wray, Rivers and Jupka30,Reference Rousseau, Moreau and Dumas59,Reference van Voorst68,Reference Alipour, Khankeh and Fekrazad72,Reference Sharma and Patt73

  10. 10. Trust in authorities as an outcome is predicted by several person-level factors. Some important factors are: exposure and attention to news about the event; self-reported knowledge of event; self-reporting of local impacts of event; previous experience of discrimination; ability to articulate problems and empowerment to achieve goals; involvement, engagement, and participation with issue; political ideology; concern with risk of hazard; and perception of consensual values with and sympathy for organization. Reference Freimuth, Musa and Hilyard33,Reference Paton36,Reference Johnson46,Reference Figuié and Fournier7478

  11. 11. Trust in authorities as an outcome is predicted by several organizational message and action factors. Health and related authorities can explicitly acknowledge uncertainty in their messages, including forecasts and warnings, as this will enhance trust during the event as well as for future events. Reference Janmaimool and Watanabe40,Reference Sharma and Patt73,Reference Glatron79 Sometimes trust in authorities may show a slight decrease as a result of openly acknowledging uncertainties; however, this decrease is only for a small proportion of the total number of message recipients, and for the vast majority, there is no change in their level of trust. Reference Johnson and Slovic80 Trust in authorities can be enhanced by several actions by organizations, some of which are: quickly informing the public and rapidly intervening; developing new information systems to respond quickly and efficiently; creating scientific communication in an easy to understand manner; seeking input from the public and encouraging a dialog; ensuring coordination between different health authorities and the media along with a uniform message; avoiding rapid changes in information and preventing conflicting information; disseminating information through multiple platforms; providing specific and clear information; communicating competence, openness, honesty, concern, care, and commitment; being impartial and relying on methodologies that minimize bias; and proactively cooperating with media outlets to disseminate information and efficiently managing conflicting official statements to the media by multiple organizations. Reference Rundblad, Knapton and Hunter52,Reference Zhong and Ye53,Reference Rousseau, Moreau and Dumas59,Reference Anthony, Sellnow and Millner64,Reference Jakubowski and Charpak69,Reference Quinn, Thomas and Kumar70,Reference Maeda and Miyahara76,Reference Glatron79,Reference Al-Douwihi81Reference Sun, Jin and Cao91

Media Reports

One English-language data-based media report examined a multi-platform health campaign in Sierra Leone during the Ebola virus outbreak. Reference Wilkinson71 Lack of trust in governmental and health systems was the largest barrier to stopping the spread of the disease. Radio services, especially local radio stations, were a highly trusted communication channel. Specific radio programs that had a large following and were trusted were useful in conveying behavior change information. The editorial independence of the radio services also helped build the public’s trust by questioning elements of the response when necessary. The main lesson learnt was that local media have a strong role in building community trust. The media report findings contributed to the across-method synthesized finding that trust in authorities can depend on the extent of coordination among different agencies, institutions, and the media.

Discussion

Overall Summary

The synthesis of evidence on the phenomenon of trust during public health emergency events was based on findings from 68 studies (38 English language, 30 other UN languages). The findings were limited to (a) primary studies, (b) done in the field (as opposed to laboratories), and (c) with people and organizations directly affected or likely to be affected by public health emergency events. The final set of 11 across-method synthesized findings provide an understanding of trust in health and related authorities during public health emergency events and the message and activities that can be undertaken to maintain and enhance the trust in this situation. Overall, the synthesized findings illuminate multiple aspects of the phenomenon of trust in health-related authorities during public health emergency events. The findings in various ways cover the following: structure/components of trust (in the context of emergency health events); the life circumstances in which trust as a phenomenon is experienced; the role of trust in the common situation of multiple information sources; the variability in trust across contexts; trust as an outcome of different factors; and trust as a predictor of different outcomes. Although of most interest for the present review might be the findings related to factors that can lead to trust as an outcome, such as extent of coordination among agencies and the media, past experience with authorities, and organization action and messaging, it is important to note that all of the findings directly contribute to maintaining and enhancing trust in authorities.

At-risk/vulnerable populations (eg, children, pregnant women, people with chronic disease, older people, low-SES urban and rural communities) whose life circumstances may exhibit inequalities/inequities relative to the general population were only marginally covered in the studies under review. Thus, it is not possible to fully address how the pattern of trust in these populations may specifically differ from that in the general populations. However, the findings show that vulnerable communities may often rely on personal networks to make their decisions as they trust information from these sources more than from media sources. Similarly, a life circumstance of economic poverty along with a less-than-desirable response to events by authorities may lead to lowered trust among such communities. Importantly, the findings also point to not assuming that the pattern of trust in authorities in all vulnerable groups is similar. The findings show, for example, that there are differences between urban African American and Hispanic communities in the United States with regard to judgements of trustworthiness of health and other related agencies.

Results Vis-a-Vis Findings From Other Reviews

There were 7 existing reviews related to trust during public health emergency events Reference Cairns, de Andrade and MacDonald1Reference Wachinger, Renn and Begg7 that were assessed as high and moderate quality using a modified Assessment of Multiple Systematic Reviews (AMSTAR) quality appraisal checklist. Reference Shea, Grimshaw and Wells92 (Four existing reviews Reference Kraut, Acquisti and Kleinberg93Reference Vaughan and Tinker96 were rated as low quality and these were “unpacked” for their data-based primary studies, which were added to the literature for the present review.) The results from the present review generally overlap with and extend the findings from these published reviews, and provide newer findings as well; in 1 case, the present results do not include a previous finding.

The present findings broadly replicate and extend the previous findings about organizational actions and messages that can enhance trust. These include: trust is influenced by organizational reputation; quality of stakeholder relationships; understanding and managing media relations; risk information provision strategies; accuracy, timeliness, and comprehensive information; transparency about available information; fairness in treatment of populations; building trust and trustworthiness through participatory dialogue and involvement in pre-event planning, exercises, and the design and testing of communication plans; and trust in public officials and the governments’ ability to respond to a public health emergency are related to greater likelihood of adoption of recommended actions. In particular, the present review more comprehensively details the complex relationship between trust and risk perceptions. The present review also more clearly identifies that trust in authorities can depend on the extent of coordination among different agencies, institutions, and the media.

Some findings not emphasized in the previous reviews are highlighted in the present review. These include that trust in authorities is a multi-component construct and not a singular concept, which needs to be kept in mind when developing any message strategies; people engage in a thoughtful process of considering multiple sources offering information and recommendations, and use source credibility for resolving the conflict among the many pieces of information; and trust in authorities occurs in a general life context and should not be seen narrowly in isolation for just a specific hazard or emergency event. The present review extends previous results and offers new findings regarding variation in trust across different message sources, the course of an emergency event, demographics, and type of hazard.

One finding from the existing reviews not covered in the present review was that lack of trust between employees and supervisor within authority organizations minimizes employees’ responses that could undermine operations. Studies that examined within-organization communication were not included in the review; only studies that dealt with communication with the general public in some way were included.

Suggestions for Practice

The final set of findings provides an understanding of the phenomenon of trust in the unique situation of public health emergency events and the activities that can be undertaken by authorities to communicate and increase trust in this context. Overall, to develop communication strategies for enhancing trust, there are several organizational message and action factors that can predict higher trust when developing communication strategies. Among these especially are: acknowledging uncertainty in messages, including forecasts and warnings; being transparent and not concealing negative information, such as rates of casualties; creating groups with specialized skills and knowledge; speedily disseminating information and intervening; creating scientific communication in an easy to understand manner; seeking input from the public and encouraging a dialog; ensuring coordination between different health authorities and the media along with a uniform message; avoiding rapid changes in information and preventing conflicting information dissemination from different agencies; and disseminating information through multiple platforms. It is again important to note that these actions occur in a larger context that includes factors such as different components of trust, history with authorities’ response to events, life circumstances of the public, and person/individual differences, all of which can both strengthen or weaken the message-trust relationship. It should also be kept in mind that trust develops over time in a relational manner particularly through participatory dialogue and involvement, which often is through community engagement.

Some of these findings may not be entirely new to practitioners; nevertheless, they may help strengthen current practices and inform their adaptation to novel and unanticipated circumstances. In contrast, some of the present findings may not accord with work from other domains of trust research (eg, laboratory studies, politics). As such, we wish to alert practitioners that translating such present findings into practical implications should be done in consultation with that work.

Research Gaps in the Reviewed Literature

The present review identified 5 main gaps in the literature on trust during public health emergency events. First, there is insufficient coverage of low-income countries. It could be that the characteristics of low-income countries, especially in terms of infrastructure and national histories, influence trust processes differently enough for the practices of health authorities to be different. To address this, comparative research between countries needs to be undertaken. The review did not identify even a single study that compared countries, even those using a case study methodology.

Second, there is a lack of a comprehensive examination of the various components of trust along with concepts that substantially overlap with trust but may behave somewhat differently, such as confidence. There are studies that investigated different sets of components, but the review did not identify any study that comprehensively examined all relevant components and concepts, and tested their relationships with variables of interest, both as outcome, such as communication strategies that influence trust as an outcome, and as predictor, such as health protection behaviors that are influenced by trust as a predictor.

Third, also completely absent in the literature are longitudinal studies. It is not always necessary to have randomized comparison group research design, which may be precluded due to the nature of public health emergency events, to draw out causal relationships. Such linkages between variables of interest, such as trust as an outcome of certain communication strategies, can also be examined using a longitudinal research design where data of interest are measured at multiple time points. Such a research design can better reveal how trust dynamically varies during the phases of an event; even if, say, preparation and recovery phases are only used for data collection, this will still provide insight into how trust in authorities varies across the phases. Such a design can also provide knowledge about how trust operates simultaneously as both an outcome and predictor.

Fourth, a research gap exists in how mass media and personal networks interact during events. Several studies talk about the importance of integration of traditional mass media (eg, television news, newspapers) with personal networks that include both face-to-face and digital/social media (such as Facebook, Twitter). Communities, especially those that may identify themselves as marginalized or be considered vulnerable, often rely on personal networks for guidance to inform their decisions as they consider information from these sources more trustworthy than from media sources. Although there are studies that recommend authorities aim to integrate information disseminated through mass media and personal networks, the review did not identify any study that investigated how this integration may actually take place.

Fifth, there is an absence of integrative model building and theory construction. Trust in health authorities and other risk communication sources and trust in information from these sources varies across populations, especially that may be considered vulnerable, and hazards/events, among other contexts. The present review identified very few models or theories that sought to provide insightful theoretical explanations of these variations. To develop effective communication strategies that enhance trust, effective theory development needs to take place as otherwise a set of empirical facts of relationships between trust and other variables will not add up to accurate predictions about these relationships that can assist with planning and management.

Future research should address these research gaps and also undertake 2 additional lines of investigation. First, future research should take findings from field studies as synthesized in the present systematic review and explore their integration with findings regarding trust obtained from laboratory studies as well as findings from domains other than public health emergency events, especially when there is divergence in the findings. This has the potential to inform theoretical frameworks for future primary studies. Second, future studies should investigate the conditions under which people use trust in message sources to decide which message to attend when faced with multiple sources of information. People may effort fully or rapidly engage in this process, and future research can disentangle the factors, such as the phase of a public health emergency event, that influence the 2 processes.

Limitations of the Present Review

There are 3 main limitations of the present systematic review. First, some information from the articles and reports that were in non-English UN languages may have been missed as these were not translated into English in their entirety. Only selected paragraphs from the different sections, with an emphasis on the findings, were translated, which may have inadvertently led to overlooking of relevant information.

Second, the extraction of data from individual studies was conducted primarily by 1 person (for English language by the review lead author; other UN languages by a reader with native proficiency) as was done the same for the synthesis of findings across studies (by the review lead author), with the results scrutinized by another research team member. However, this cross-checking process was not done formally, which did not allow the computation of inter-coder ratings statistics to assess the degree of consistency of the results.

Third, in an attempt to search a diversity of resources to obtain references representing multidisciplinary viewpoints, we were not able to include every potential keyword for all the concepts related to trust. In particular, search terms “distrust” and “mistrust” (concepts on the opposite side of the question) were not included in the searches due to limits of time, search interfaces, number of characters allowed, and other such reasons. As a result, although the broader search provided more comprehensive set of references from a range of disciplines, a potentially small number of references focusing exclusively on distrust or mistrust (rather than “trust”) may have been missed.

Conclusions

The public’s trust in health-related authorities during times of emergency public health events is a complex phenomenon. Trust is a multi-faceted concept with multiple components and closely related concepts, all of which may be affected differently by the same message designed to enhance it. Trust is also dynamic. It changes across different message sources, the public’s demographics, type of hazard/event, and the course of the event. Thus, a message designed to enhance trust in a message source for a particular event affecting a particular population may be quite effective at 1 point in time but may fail to work at a different point in time. Thus, the specificity of each message situation needs to be carefully analyzed to create messages that work.

During an emergency event, people use source credibility for resolving the conflict among multiple information sources and may engage in a thoughtful process of considering different sources offering information and recommendations, especially in the preparation phase. The careful sorting of information and its sources occurs in life circumstances that may include economic poverty and associated multitude daily hazards and risks, entrenched cultural beliefs and behaviors, and history with authorities’ response to events. Messages that disregard this broader social context outside of basic demographics will fail to work.

Irrespective of the difficulties for message and activities design posed by the above for health and related organizations, some cautious generalizations about what works to enhance trust are possible. Some of these include: coordination with other agencies, institutions, and the media; swift and uniform message dissemination and intervention; communicating uncertainties; being transparent and not concealing negative data; avoiding rapid changes in information and preventing conflicting information dissemination from different agencies; disseminating information through multiple platforms; and sustaining public involvement and dialog. However, it should be noted that, although high trust in authorities can lead to positive outcomes, such as higher vaccination behaviors, it can also lead to negative outcomes, such as lowered perceived risk for hazards.

Conflict of interest

The views expressed in the present paper are those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect the views of the World Health Organization.

Funding statement

This project was funded by the World Health Organization, Department of Communications (Contract PO 201393190 WHO Registration 2015/586494-0 and Contract PO 201428650 WHO Registration 2016/601521-0). The present systematic review is part of a larger project sponsored by the World Health Organization. The review framework is identical across all the papers stemming from this project. A previous version of the present manuscript has been published as a White Paper for the sponsor and is available online at the sponsor’s website.

References

Cairns, G, de Andrade, M, MacDonald, L. Reputation, relationships, risk communication, and the role of trust in the prevention and control of communicable disease: a review. J Health Commun. 2013;18(12):1550-1565. doi: 10.1080/10810730.2013.840696 Google ScholarPubMed
Gesser-Edelsburg, A, Stolero, N, Mordini, E, et al. Emerging infectious disease communication during the 2009 H1N1 influenza outbreak: literature review (2009-2013) of the methodology used for EID communication analysis. Disaster Med Public Health Prep. 2015;9(2):199-206. doi: 10.1017/dmp.2014.126 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lettieri, E, Masella, C, Radaelli, G. Disaster management: findings from a systematic review. Disaster Prev Manage. 2009. doi: 10.1108/09653560910953207 Google Scholar
Lin, L, Savoia, E, Agboola, F, et al. What have we learned about communication inequalities during the H1N1 pandemic: a systematic review of the literature. BMC Public Health. 2014;14:484. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-14-484 Google ScholarPubMed
McCaffrey, S. Community wildfire preparedness: a global state-of-the-knowledge summary of social science research. Curr Forest Rep. 2015;1:81-90. doi: 10.1007/s40725-015-0015-7 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ruggiero, A, Vos, M. Terrorism communication: characteristics and emerging perspectives in the scientific literature 2002-2011. J Conting Crisis Manage. 2013;21:153-166. doi: 10.1111/1468-5973.12022 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wachinger, G, Renn, O, Begg, C, et al. The risk perception paradox: implications for governance and communication of natural hazards. Risk Anal. 2013;33:1049-1065. doi: 10.1111/j.1539-6924.2012.01942.x Google ScholarPubMed
Sopory, P, Day, AM, Novak, JM, et al. Communicating uncertainty during public health emergency events: a systematic review. Rev Commun Res. 2019;7. doi: 10.12840/ISSN.2255-4165.019 Google Scholar
Moher, D, Liberati, A, Tetzlaff, J, et al. Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and analyses: the PRISMA statement. PLoS Med. 2009;6:e1000097. doi: 1371/journal.pmed. 1000097Google ScholarPubMed
Lewin, S, Glenton, C, Munthe-Kaas, H, et al. Using qualitative evidence in decision making for health and social interventions: an approach to assess confidence in findings from qualitative evidence syntheses (GRADE-CERQual). PLoS Med. 2015;12(10):e1001895. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1001895 Google Scholar
Noyes, J, Booth, A, Lewin, S, et al. Applying GRADE-CERQual approach to qualitative evidence synthesis findings-paper 6: how to assess relevance of the data. Implementation Sci. 2018;13(Suppl 1):51-61. doi: 10.1186/s13012-017-0693-6 Google ScholarPubMed
Effective Practice and Organisation of Care (EPOC). Suggested risk of bias criteria for EPOC reviews. EPOC resources for review authors. Norwegian Knowledge Centre for the Health Services. Cochrane website. http://epoc.cochrane.org/epoc-specific-resources-review-authors. Published 2015. Accessed September 24, 2016.Google Scholar
Higgins, JPT, Green, S. Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions Version 5.1.0 [updated March 2011]. The Cochrane Collaboration website. www.handbook.cochrane.org. Published 2011. Accessed September 24, 2016.Google Scholar
Davids, EL, Roman, NV. A systematic review of the relationship between parenting styles and children’s physical activity. Afr J Physical Health Educ Recreation Dance. 2014;2(1):228-246.Google Scholar
Critical Appraisal Skills Programme (CASP). CASP qualitative checklist. CASP website. http://www.casp-uk.net/casp-tools-checklists. Published 2013. Accessed September 24, 2016.Google Scholar
Pluye, P, Robert, E, Cargo, M, et al. Proposal: a mixed methods appraisal tool for systematic mixed studies reviews. Mixed Methods Appraisal Tools website. http://mixedmethodsappraisaltoolpublic.pbworks.com. Published 2011. Accessed September 24, 2016. Archived by WebCite at http://www.webcitation.org/5tTRTc9yJ Google Scholar
Tyndall, J. How low can you go? Towards a hierarchy of grey literature. Flinders University website. https://dspace.flinders.edu.au/xmlui/handle/2328/3326. Published 2008-09. Accessed July 27, 2020.Google Scholar
Popay, J, Roberts, H, Sowden, A, et al. Guidance on the Conduct of Narrative Synthesis in Systematic Reviews: Final Report. Swindon: ESRC Methods Programme; 2006.Google Scholar
Barnett-Page, E, Thomas, J. Methods for the synthesis of qualitative research: a critical review. BMC Medical Res Methodol. 2009;9:59. doi: 10.1186/1471-2288-9-59 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pope, C, Ziebland, S, Mays, N. Analysing qualitative data. BMJ. 2000;320:114-116.Google ScholarPubMed
Working Group, GRADE. Grading quality of evidence and strength of recommendations. BMJ. 2004;328(7454):1490-1494.Google Scholar
Guyatt, G, Oxman, AD, Aki, EA, et al. GRADE guidelines: 1. Introduction-GRADE evidence profiles and summary of findings table. J Clin Epidemiol. 2011;64(4):383-394. doi: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2010.04.026 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haynes, K, Barclay, J, Pidgeon, N. The issue of trust and its influence on risk communication during a volcanic crisis. Bull Volcanol. 2008;70(5):605-621.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meredith, LS, Eisenman, DP, Rhodes, H, et al. Trust influences response to public health messages during a bioterrorist event. J Health Commun. 2007;12(3):217-232.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paek, HJ, Hilyard, K, Freimuth, VS, et al. Public support for government actions during a flu pandemic: lessons learned from a statewide survey. Health Promot Pract. 2008;9(4 Suppl):60S-72S.Google ScholarPubMed
Petts, J, Niemeyer, S. Health risk communication and amplification: learning from the MMR vaccination controversy. Health Risk Soc. 2004;6(1):7-23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Siegrist, M, Gutscher, H, Earle, TC. Perception of risk: the influence of general trust, and general confidence. J Risk Res. 2005;8(2):145-156.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vaughan, E, Tinker, TL, Truman, BI, et al. Predicting responses to reassurances and uncertainties in bioterrorism communications for urban populations in New York and California. Biosecur Bioterror. 2012;10:188-202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van der Weerd, W, Timmermans, DRM, Beaujean, DJMA, et al. Monitoring the level of government trust, risk perception and intention of the general public to adopt protective measures during the influenza A (H1N1) pandemic in the Netherlands. BMC Public Health. 2011;11:575.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wray, R, Rivers, J, Jupka, K, et al. Public perceptions about trust in emergency risk communication: qualitative research findings. Int J Mass Emerg Disasters. 2006;24(1):45.Google Scholar
Besley, JC, McComas, KA, Trumbo, CW. Citizen views about public meetings. J Risk Res. 2012;15(4):355-371.Google Scholar
Bitsch, V, Koković, N, Rombach, M. Risk communication and market effects during foodborne illnesses: a comparative case study of bacterial outbreaks in the US and in Germany. Int Food Agribus Manage Rev. 2014;17(3):97-114.Google Scholar
Freimuth, VS, Musa, D, Hilyard, K, et al. Trust during the early stages of the 2009 H1N1 pandemic. J Health Commun. 2014;19:321-339.Google ScholarPubMed
Gilles, I, Bangerter, A, Clemence, A, et al. Trust in medical organizations predicts pandemic (H1N1) 2009 vaccination behavior and perceived efficacy of protection measures in the Swiss public. Eur J Epidemiol. 2011;26:203-210.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mei, ETW, Lavigne, F, Picquout, A, et al. Lessons learned from the 2010 evacuations at Merapi volcano. J Volcanol Geothermal Res. 2013;261:348-365.Google Scholar
Paton, D. Risk communication and natural hazard mitigation: how trust influences its effectiveness. Int J Glob Environ Issues. 2008;8(1-2):2-16.Google Scholar
Perko, T, Železnik, N, Turcanu, C, et al. Is knowledge important? Empirical research on nuclear risk communication in two countries. Health Phys. 2012;102(6):614-625.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rød, SK, Botan, C, Holen, A. Risk communication and the willingness to follow evacuation instructions in a natural disaster. Health Risk Soc. 2012;14(1):87-99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Su, Y, Sun, X, Zhao, F. Trust and its effects on the public’s perception of flood risk: a social science investigation of the middle and lower reaches of the Yangtze River. J Flood Risk Manage. 2014. doi: 10.1111/jfr3.12138 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Janmaimool, P, Watanabe, T. Environmental concerns and uncertainty communication for building public trust in environmental risk management: a case study of Maptaphut municipality, Thailand. Int J Dev Sustain. 2014;3:1152-1173.Google Scholar
Flood, Kutovaya SV. 2013: social well-being of the evacuated residents. In: 4th Regional Reddevkiy Readings Conference; 2014. [Russian].Google Scholar
Bass, SB, Greener, JR, Ruggieri, D, et al. Attitudes and perceptions of urban African Americans of a “dirty bomb” radiological terror event: results of a qualitative study and implications for effective risk communication. Disaster Med Public Health Prep. 2015;9(01):9-18.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Xie, R, Kan, J. Analysis of traits of the demand for information during public health. Chin Health Educ. 2010;26(1):43-45. [Chinese].Google Scholar
Xie, R, Yang, C, Ou, JM, et al. Qualitative study on health demands of community residents for human infection with H7N9 virus of two Cities of Fujian. Chin J Health Educ. 2013;29(7):582-589. [Chinese].Google Scholar
Boon, HJ. Perceptions of climate change risk in four disaster-impacted rural Australian towns. Reg Environ Change. 2016;16:137-149.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, BB. Explaining Americans’ responses to dread epidemics: an illustration with Ebola in late 2014. J Risk Res. 2016;20:1338-1357.Google Scholar
Trumbo, CW, McComas, KA. The function of credibility in information processing for risk perception. Risk Anal. 2003;23(2):343-353.Google ScholarPubMed
Viklund, MJ. Trust and risk perception in western Europe: a cross-national study. Risk Anal. 2003:23(4):727-738.Google ScholarPubMed
Frewer, LJ, Miles, S. Temporal stability of the psychological determinants of trust: Implications for communication about food risks. Health Risk Soc. 2003;5(3):259-271.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kjaernes, U. Trust and distrust: cognitive decisions or social relations. J Risk Res. 2006;9:911-932.Google Scholar
Malet, D, Korbitz, M. Accountability between experts and the public in times of risk. Aust J Public Admin. 2014;73(4):491-500.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rundblad, G, Knapton, O, Hunter, PR. Communication, perception and behaviour during a natural disaster involving a ‘Do Not Drink’ and a subsequent ‘Boil Water’ notice: a postal questionnaire study. BMC Public Health. 2010;10(1):1.Google ScholarPubMed
Zhong, J, Ye, L. Examining disaster risk communication based on regional flood perception of the public. J Instit Disaster-Prev Sci Technol. 2009;11(4):16-20. [Chinese].Google Scholar
Fernandez Souto, AB. Communication and crisis management. Comparative notes between Prestige case and Gulf of Mexico’s tragedy. Anagrams. 2012;10(21):143-154. [Spanish].Google Scholar
Francescutti, P. From cow disease to SARS: Risk communication in Spain. In: Gomez JR, ed. Risk within information society. http://ruc.udc.es/dspace/bitstream/handle/2183/12807/CC-91_art_3.pdf?sequence=1. 2007:41-59. [Spanish]. Accessed April 22, 2021.Google Scholar
Karan, K, Aileen, L, Elaine, PYL. Emerging victorious against an outbreak: Integrated communication management of SARS in Singapore - media coverage and impact of the SARS campaign in moving a nation to be socially responsible. J Creat Commun. 2007;2(3):383-403.Google Scholar
Liu, Y, Zeng, Y. Risk communication, trust in government and consumers’ recovery behavior: case study of the melamine incident. Beijing Soc Sci. 2014;3:52-62. [Chinese].Google Scholar
Maeno, R. The Fukushima nuclear power plant disaster and perceptions of health risk communication: a case study. J Health Saf Environ. 2014;30(1):113-133.Google Scholar
Rousseau, C, Moreau, N, Dumas, MP, et al. Public media communications about H1N1, risk perceptions and immunization behaviours: a Quebec-France comparison. Public Underst Sci. 2008;24:225-240.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Su, G, Ma, Z, Wang, R, et al. Examining the characteristics of people’s response to and perception of Wenchuan earthquake: case study of Deyang city in Sichuang province. Seismol Geol. 2008;30(4):877-891. [Chinese].Google Scholar
Xie, X, Wang, H, Ren, J, et al. Analyzing the risk communication in the outbreak of SARS. Chin J Appl Psychol. 2005;11(2):104-109 [Chinese].Google Scholar
Utz, S, Schultz, F, Glocka, S. Crisis communication online: how medium, crisis type and emotions affected public reactions in the Fukushima Daiichi nuclear disaster. Public Relat Rev. 2013;39(1):40-46.Google Scholar
Kutovaya, SV. Flood in the southern far east of Russia: social well-being of the affected population. Fundamental Issled. 2015;8-3:572-575. [Russian].Google Scholar
Anthony, KE, Sellnow, TL, Millner, AG. Message convergence as a message-centered approach to analyzing and improving risk communication. J Appl Commun Res. 2013;41:346-364.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Affletranger, B, de Richemond, NM. Management of warning and information during floods: conditions and limitations of a local participatory approach. The example of Deux-Sèvres. Flux. 2003;1:6-27. [French].Google Scholar
D’Ercole, R. Forces and weaknesses of the risk management in Japan: a reflexion after the crisis provoked by the Usu, Hokkaidô eruption in 2000. Ann Geography. 2002:524-548. [French].Google Scholar
Gaillard, JC, Liamzon, CC, Maceda, EA. Catastrophes considered “natural” and development: reflections on the origin of disasters in the Philippines. Third World Rev. 2008;2:371-390. [French].Google Scholar
van Voorst, R. Risk-handling styles in a context of flooding and uncertainty in Jakarta, Indonesia: an analytical framework to analyse heterogeneous risk-behavior. Dis Prev Manage. 2015;24:484-505.Google Scholar
Jakubowski, E, Charpak, Y. Sixth forum on the future of crisis communication. World Health Organization website. 2004. https://www.euro.who.int/__data/assets/pdf_file/0005/90536/e85056f.pdf?ua=1. Published May 2004. [French]. Accessed April 22, 2021.Google Scholar
Quinn, SC, Thomas, T, Kumar, S. The anthrax vaccine and research: reactions from postal workers and public health professionals. Biosecur Bioterr. 2008;6:321-333.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilkinson, S. Practice briefing: using media and communication to respond to public health emergencies: lessons learned from Ebola. BBC Action Media website. http://downloads.bbc.co.uk/mediaaction/pdf/practicebriefings/ebola-lessons-learned.pdf. Published June 2016. Accessed July 27, 2020.Google Scholar
Alipour, F, Khankeh, H, Fekrazad, H, et al. Social issues and post-disaster recovery: a qualitative study in an Iranian context. Int Soc Work. 2015;58:689-703.Google Scholar
Sharma, U, Patt, A. Disaster warning response: the effects of different types of personal experience. Nat Hazards. 2012;60:409-423.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figuié, M, Fournier, T. Global health risks and national politics: the management of avian flu in Vietnam. J Agri Environ Stud. 2010;91(3):327-343. [French].Google Scholar
Government of Canada Office of the Protection of Critical Infrastructure and Emergency Preparedness. The road to a national strategy of disaster mitigation. 2002. [French] https://www.publicsafety.gc.ca/cnt/mrgnc-mngmnt/dsstr-prvntn-mtgtn/ntnl-dsstr-mtgtn-strtg-dscssn-ppr-fr.aspx. Accessed May 7, 2021.Google Scholar
Maeda, Y, Miyahara, M. Determinants of trust in industry, government, and citizen’s groups in Japan. Risk Anal. 2003;23:303-310.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ruin, I. Driving against the currents of flash floods, every day and exceptional conflict. Ann Geography. 2010; 4:419-443. [French].Google Scholar
United Nations Food and Agriculture Organization and World Health Organization. FAO/WHO guide for the implementation of the principles and procedures of risk analysis in food safety emergencies. FAO website. http://www.fao.org/3/ba0092e/ba0092e00.pdf. Published 2011. Accessed July 27, 2020.Google Scholar
Glatron, S. Cognitive and spatial representations of risks and nuisances for city dwellers. Doctoral dissertation, University of Strasbourg. 2009. [French].Google Scholar
Johnson, BB, Slovic, P. Fearing or fearsome Ebola communication? Keeping the public in the dark about possible post-21-day symptoms and infectiousness could backfire. Health Risk Soc. 2015;17(5-6):458-471.Google Scholar
Al-Douwihi, AA. Role of informative planning in facing disasters and crisis. Survey study of staff members of general directorate of civil defense and ministry of information in Riyadh. Master’s thesis. 2004:1-198. [Arabic].Google Scholar
Al-Khayli, A. Relationship between media and the process of decision-making during disasters. Master’s thesis. 2007:1-265. [Arabic].Google Scholar
Al-Shaqsi, O. Public attitudes towards Omani media’s management of the Cyclone Gonu crisis. Al-Manhal. 2013:1-17. [Arabic].Google Scholar
Deshaies, D, Pilon, P, Valiquette, L, et al. Public health response during a case of human rabies in Quebec. Can J Public Health. 2004;95(2):March/April. [French].CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duchêne, F, Journel, CM. Experience of the flood. Ann Urban Res. 2004:71-77. [French].Google Scholar
Gryzunova, EA. Models of governmental crisis communications and information management. Vestnik MGIMO. 2012;6:225-229. [Russian].Google Scholar
Hechmati, G. Influenza epidemics: an information system for decision-making in public health. Doctoral dissertation, University of Geneva. 2004. [French].Google Scholar
Heitz, C, Glatron, S. Informational public tools on major risks: are the maps vector acculturation? Exploratory study of the perception of flood maps by individuals at risk. Eurometropole of Strasbourg. 2013. [French].Google Scholar
Lord, S. Study of the impact of avian flu risk communication on social interpretation and creation of public opinion. Presented as partial requirement for Master’s degree in communications, University of Quebec at Montreal. 2009. [French].Google Scholar
Massé, R, Weinstock, D, Désy, M, Moisan, C. Popular perceptions of risk and expert knowledge in the context of pandemic: the case of A (H1N1) in Québec. Anthropology & Health: Francophone International Journal of the Anthropology of Health. 201;3. [French]. doi: 10.4000/anthropologiesante.739CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sun, J, Jin, S, Cao, S. A qualitative psychological analysis of the rumors appeared after a disaster: the evidence from the rumors appeared after 5.12 Wenchuan earthquake. Adv Psychol Sci. 2009;17(3):602-609. [Chinese].Google Scholar
Shea, BJ, Grimshaw, JM, Wells, GA, et al. Development of AMSTAR: a measurement tool to assess the methodological quality of systematic reviews. BMC Medical Research Methodology website. http://www.biomedcentral.com/1471-2288/7/10. Published February 15, 2007. Accessed July 27, 2020.Google Scholar
Kraut, RE, Acquisti, A, Kleinberg, JM, et al. Public Response to Alerts and Warnings Using Social Media: Report of a Workshop on Current Knowledge and Research Gaps. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press; 2013.Google Scholar
McComas, KA. Defining moments in risk communication research: 1996-2005. J Health Commun. 2006;11:75-91. doi: 10.1080/10810730500461091 Google ScholarPubMed
Siegrist, M, Zingg, A. The role of public trust during pandemics implications for crisis communication. Eur Psychol. 2014;19:23-32. doi: 10.1027/1016-9040/a000169 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vaughan, E, Tinker, T. Effective health risk communication about pandemic influenza for vulnerable populations. Am J Public Health. 2008;99:S324-S332. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2009.162537 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Search terms

Figure 1

Figure 1. Study selection.

Figure 2

Table 2. Characteristics of studies

Figure 3

Table 3. Synthesis of findings within methodological stream and evaluation of certainty/confidence

Figure 4

Table 4. Synthesis of findings across methodological streams