Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-8ctnn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T04:56:01.710Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Associations between caregiving status, acculturation, and psychological distress in a diverse sample

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 December 2022

Julia P. Nguyen
Affiliation:
Department of Neurology, University of California, Davis, School of Medicine, Sacramento, CA 95817, USA
Diane Hoang
Affiliation:
Foresight Mental Health, Oakland, CA 94612, USA
Kieran Zhou
Affiliation:
Shanghai Unionlab Co., Ltd. Shanghai, CN, USA
Danielle J. Harvey
Affiliation:
Department of Public Health Sciences, University of California, Davis, CA 95616, USA
QuynhAnh Dam
Affiliation:
San Francisco State University, San Francisco, CA 94132, USA
Oanh L. Meyer*
Affiliation:
Department of Neurology, University of California, Davis, School of Medicine, Sacramento, CA 95817, USA
*
Correspondence should be addressed to: Oanh L. Meyer, Alzheimer’s Disease Research Center, Department of Neurology, UC Davis School of Medicine, 4860 Y Street, Sacramento, CA 95817, USA. Tel: +1 916 734 5218; Fax: +1 916703 5290. E-mail: olmeyer@ucdavis.edu.

Abstract

Objectives:

Increasingly diverse caregiver populations have prompted studies examining culture and caregiver outcomes. Still, little is known about the influence of sociocultural factors and how they interact with caregiving context variables to influence psychological health. We explored the role of caregiving and acculturation factors on psychological distress among a diverse sample of adults.

Design:

Secondary data analysis of the California Health Interview Survey (CHIS).

Participants:

The 2009 CHIS surveyed 47,613 adults representative of the population of California. This study included Latino and Asian American Pacific Islander (AAPI) caregivers and non-caregivers (n = 13,161).

Measurements:

Multivariate weighted regression analyses examined caregiver status and acculturation variables (generational status, language of interview, and English language proficiency) and their associations with psychological distress (Kessler-6 scale). Covariates included caregiving context (e.g., support and neighborhood factors) and demographic variables.

Results:

First generation caregivers had more distress than first-generation non-caregivers (β=0.92, 95% CI: (0.18, 1.65)); the difference in distress between caregivers and non-caregivers was smaller in the third than first generation (β=-1.21, 95% CI: (-2.24, -0.17)). Among those who did not interview in English (β=1.17, 95% CI: (0.13, 2.22)) and with low English proficiency (β=2.60, 95% CI: (1.21, 3.98)), caregivers reported more distress than non-caregivers.

Conclusions:

Non-caregivers exhibited the "healthy immigrant effect," where less acculturated individuals reported less distress. In contrast, caregivers who were less acculturated reported more distress.

Type
Original Research Article
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of International Psychogeriatric Association

Introduction

In 2015, there were an estimated 44 million family caregivers in the nation (National Alliance for Caregiving and AARP Public Policy Institute, 2015). By 2030, the population of older adults residing in the US is estimated to increase by 50% (Mather et al., Reference Mather, Jacobsen and Pollard2015). Many of these older adults will have chronic health conditions and disabilities and may be dependent on a caregiver. Mounting demands for caregiving and an increasingly ethnically diverse population have prompted a number of studies aimed at investigating the role of cultural values in caregiving experiences and health and mental health outcomes. These studies have expanded previous models of caregiver distress by exploring the role of sociocultural and contextual variables. One such model is Knight and Sayegh (Reference Knight and Sayegh2009)’s sociocultural stress and coping model, which elaborates on Pearlin et al. (Reference Pearlin, Menaghan, Lieberman and Mullan1981)’s stress process model and is particularly useful for the current study.

The sociocultural stress and coping model highlights the impact of sociocultural variables on caregiver health outcomes and incorporates not only broad racial/ethnic differences, but nuances in experiences and heterogeneity within racial/ethnic groups, such as acculturation level and immigration experiences. According to the U.S. Census Bureau (2020), over 44 million of the US population were foreign-born with a majority emigrating from Latin America and Asia.

Caregiving is an integral part of the cultural identities and practices held by many immigrant communities (Angel et al., Reference Angel, Rote, Brown, Angel and Markides2014; Montenegro, Reference Montenegro2014; Rote and Moon, Reference Rote and Moon2016). According to the National Alliance for Caregiving and AARP Public Policy Institute (2015) report, it is found that a higher percentage of Asian and Latino populations are caregivers compared to non-Hispanic White (NHW) populations. In the same report, the number of Latino and Asian surveyed participants 21% and 19.7% were caregivers, respectively, while only 16.9% of NHW participants were caregivers.

Theoretical/conceptual framework

Acculturation was first defined as changes in the “cultural patterns” of individuals that have had “firsthand contact” with each other (Redfield et al., Reference Redfield, Linton and Herskovits1936). Berry (Reference Berry2005)’s model of acculturation posited that factors which are external to the individual, including host culture dynamics, play an essential role in acculturation. Recent studies of acculturation have sought to capture these contextual elements using proxies such as generational status and language use or preference (Abraído-Lanza et al., Reference Abraído-Lanza, Echeverría and Flórez2016; Lara et al., Reference Lara, Gamboa, Kahramanian, Morales and Hayes Bautista2005; Salant and Lauderdale, Reference Salant and Lauderdale2003). In general, acquisition of the host language is thought to be positively correlated with the degree to which an individual is acculturated. Immigrant/generational status is a variable reflecting a time dimension and indicates exposure to host sociocultural norms, with succeeding generations conceptually more acculturated than preceding generations (Portes, Reference Portes1996).

Studies have shown that interview language and self-rated language use proficiency are reliable measures of acculturation (Lee et al., Reference Lee, Nguyen and Tsui2011; Lopez-Class et al., Reference Lopez-Class, Castro and Ramirez2011). Using data from the 2007 California Health Interview Survey (CHIS), Kim et al. Reference Kim(2011) found that Latino and Asian immigrants with low English language proficiency reported more psychological distress than English proficient and English-only speaking groups. In another California sample, Chang and Moon (Reference Chang and Moon2016) showed that immigrants with low English proficiency consistently reported higher psychological distress compared to only English-speaking groups. Whereas these findings fall in line with the acculturative stress model, in which acculturation is associated with less distress, other studies have found the opposite: acculturation being associated with more distress, a phenomenon referred to as the Healthy Immigrant Effect or Immigrant Paradox (Berry, Reference Berry1970).

In a study of US Mexican women, those who spoke English only had higher levels of psychological distress compared to their counterparts who were bilingual or did not speak English at all (Bekteshi et al., Reference Bekteshi, Xu and Van Tran2015). Our own studies revealed that among Chinese- and Vietnamese-American caregivers, those with higher levels of educational attainment and therefore were potentially more acculturated, reported more distress (Meyer et al., Reference Meyer, Liu, Nguyen, Hinton and Tancredi2018). Mancenido et al. Reference Mancenido, Williams and Hajat(2020) found that first-generation immigrants reported higher psychological distress than second-generation non-immigrants in a recent cohort surveyed in the 2015 CHIS.

Most of these studies have sought to define the role of acculturation on psychological distress reported by diverse racial/ethnic, non-caregiving groups. However, little is known about how caregiving interacts with acculturation level and immigration experiences to affect psychological distress. This is important as both acculturation and caregiving may be associated with greater distress, and their interaction may eventually lead to poor mental health. Results from a study on US ethnic minority caregivers found a significant association between acculturation, as measured by generational status, and caregiving practices (Miyawaki, Reference Miyawaki2016). Studies have suggested that various caregiving practices, which are impacted by cultural factors, significantly affect psychological distress among caregivers (Acton and Kang, Reference Acton and Kang2001; Knight and Sayegh, Reference Knight and Sayegh2009; Lawton et al., Reference Lawton, Kleban, Moss, Rovine and Glicksman1989; Novak and Guest, Reference Novak and Guest1989). In addition, though the relationship between acculturation and stress has been widely discussed, it is unknown how caregiving impacts this relationship. Thus, it is important to understand the intertwining relations of caregiving, acculturation, and psychological distress in these diverse populations.

This study aims to investigate the role of acculturation and caregiving status on psychological distress among Latinos and Asian Americans and Pacific Islanders (AAPIs) in a population-based sample. Moreover, we build on the social determinants of health literature and ecological theories of caregiver distress by examining how education, income, and neighborhood safety influence distress (Dilworth-Anderson et al., Reference Dilworth-Anderson, Williams and Gibson2002). Multiple studies examining non-caregiving populations match the healthy immigrant perspective (Booth et al., Reference Booth, Ayers and Marsiglia2014; Dey and Lucas, Reference Dey and Lucas2006; Frisbie et al., Reference Frisbie, Cho and Hummer2001; Gomez et al., Reference Gomez, Kelsey, Glaser, Lee and Sidney2004; Kennedy et al., Reference Kennedy, Kidd, McDonald and Biddle2015; Markides and Eschbach, Reference Markides and Eschbach2005). Thus, based on prior literature, we hypothesize that Latinos and AAPIs who are more acculturated will have more psychological distress, and that being a caregiver will further exacerbate this relationship (Booth et al., Reference Booth, Ayers and Marsiglia2014; Meyer et al., Reference Meyer, Liu, Nguyen, Hinton and Tancredi2018).

Methods

Sample

Data from the 2009 California Health Interview Survey (CHIS) were available as a public dataset (California Health Interview Survey, 2012). The CHIS has been conducted every other year since 2001 and is one of the largest population-based telephone health surveys in the nation. Surveys were conducted in English, Spanish, Mandarin, Cantonese, Vietnamese, and Korean. In California Health Interview Survey (2009), CHIS surveyed 47,614 adults that were representative of California’s non-institutionalized population. From the full sample, we limited our study sample to only those who reported their ethnicity as Latino/Hispanic or AAPI, bringing our sample to N = 13,161.

Measures

Outcomes. Psychological distress was measured using the Kessler-6 (K6) scale (Kessler et al., Reference Kessler2002). Participants were asked to recall the worst month in the past year when they had experienced serious psychological distress and were asked to report, during that time, how often they felt nervous, hopeless, restless, depressed, worthless, or that everything was an effort. Values ranged from 0 to 24, with higher values representing more distress.

Race/ethnicity and acculturation. Participants were coded into self-reported monoracial categories based on the US Office of Management and Budget’s federal race/ethnicity classification standards: (1) Hispanic/Latino, (2) Asian American, and (3) Native Hawaiian or Pacific Islander; we combined the latter two categories (AAPIs). We assessed acculturation in three different ways, via generational status, language of interview, and English language proficiency. Examining these acculturation proxies separately allowed us to compare and validate the acculturation proxies against one another. A categorical measure was created to indicate whether participants were third generation or higher (US born with both parents born in the US), second generation (US born with at least one non-US born parent), or first generation (non-US born). Language of interview was dichotomized as 0 = English and 1 = Spanish/Asian language. Self-reported English language proficiency was dichotomized as 0 = not well or 1 = well or only speak English.

Caregiving variables. Caregiver status was determined if participants endorsed providing care to a spouse, parent/parent-in-law, or grandparent in the last year. Co-residence with the care recipient and use of respite care were dichotomous variables (0 = no, 1 = yes). The support variable was created from two separate CHIS variables: caregivers were asked if there was someone else who could help them if they were unable to do it; those who said yes were noted as having informal support. Caregivers were also asked if they had paid for caregivers; those who said yes were noted as having formal support. Those who said no to both items received a 0 on the support variable, indicating no support.

Covariates. Education level was coded as 0 = less than high school diploma, 1 = high school diploma or equivalent, 2 = some college or Bachelor’s degree, and 3 = some graduate school or graduate degree. Marital status was coded by widowed/separated/divorced/never married or married/living with partner. Participants self-reported their gender (male versus female) and health status (on a scale from 1—Poor to 5—Excellent). Age was a continuous variable and centered at 50 in regression analyses. Income was computed by dividing total annual household income (in dollars) by the number of adults residing in the household. Neighborhood safety fears were assessed with the question, “How often do you feel safe in your neighborhood (1 = All of the time to 4 = None of the time)?” Responses were reversed coded so that higher values represented greater perceptions of neighborhood safety.

Data analysis

To account for the complex sampling design of CHIS, analyses were conducted via survey data analysis procedures (PROC SURVEYFREQ, PROC SURVEYMEANS, and PROC SURVEYREG) using SAS software, version 9.4 (SAS version 9.4, 2022). Weighted chi-square and linear regression analyses with the jackknife method for variance estimation and replicate weights (as recommended by CHIS) were conducted to examine Latino and AAPI group differences on the main variables of interest and demographic covariates. To assess associations between caregiver status, acculturation variables, and possible interactions, four separate linear regression models were fit to the data: Model 1 included indicator variables for race/ethnicity, caregiver status, generational status, and a generational status by caregiver status interaction, adjusting for covariates. Model 2 was the same as Model 1 except that the other acculturation proxy – language of interview – and its interaction with caregiver status was examined. Model 3 used English language proficiency as the acculturation proxy. Lastly, we examined whether acculturation by caregiver status interactions were further moderated by race/ethnicity. Unstandardized regression coefficient estimates and their standard errors are reported in the tables.

Results

Descriptive analyses

Table 1 presents the characteristics of participants in the study by generational status. Among non-caregivers, the mean age was 43.8 (SD = 0.2), 33.3 (SD = 0.4), and 39.9 years (SD = 0.8) in first-, second-, and third-generation groups, respectively. These means were comparable to those among caregivers, which were 43.3 (SD = 0.8), 36.5 (SD = 1.2), and 39.6 years (SD = 1.3) in first-, second-, and third-generation groups, respectively. Among both caregivers and non-caregivers, a higher percentage of the third generation was Latino compared to the second or first generation; in contrast, a higher percentage of the first generation was AAPI compared to the second or third generations. First-generation caregivers and non-caregivers had the highest proportion of individuals who did not achieve a 12th grade education, while second- and third-generation caregivers and non-caregivers had the highest proportion of individuals who graduated from college compared to their first generation counterparts. Among both caregivers and non-caregivers, first generation individuals had the lowest incomes.

Table 1. Weighted demographic characteristics of the sample (n = 13,161)

a Unweighted sample size.

b Mean and standard error for continuous variables and percentages for categorical variables.

c Asian American Pacific Islander.

Factors associated with psychological distress

Table 2 shows the results of the separate regression models. Model 1 showed that first-generation caregivers had more distress than first-generation individuals who were not caregivers (β = 0.92, 95% CI: (0.18, 1.65)); moreover, the difference in distress level between caregivers and non-caregivers was smaller in the third generation than in the first (β = −1.21, 95% CI: (−2.24, −0.17)). Among non-caregivers, those in the third generation had more distress than those in the first generation (β = 0.82, 95% CI: (0.22, 1.42)). Figure 1 illustrates the significant interactions. In Model 2, caregivers had higher levels of distress than non-caregivers among those interviewed in a language other than English relative to those assessed in English (β = 1.17, 95% CI: (0.13, 2.22)). The difference in distress was not significant between caregivers and non-caregivers who were interviewed in English (p = 0.15). In Model 3, results were consistent in that among those with low English proficiency, caregivers had higher psychological distress than non-caregivers (β = 2.60, 95% CI: (1.21, 3.98)); this difference was not as large among those proficient in English (β = −2.28, 95% CI: (−3.65, −0.91)). There was no evidence to suggest that the association between acculturation and caregiving status differed by race/ethnicity or gender for each of the three-way interactions (results not shown).

Figure 1. Generation status, interview language, and English proficiency by caregiver status interaction on psychological distress. Model adjusts for continuous covariates centered at the mean. Caregiving status is a dichotomous variable.

Table 2. Multivariate regression of variables associated with psychological distress a

a Unstandardized regression coefficient estimates and their standard errors from the regression models are reported.

b Standard error.

c Confidence interval.

d Reference categories are Latino, first generation, non-caregivers, English interview, and non-English proficient speaker.

Each model used a different acculturation proxy: Model 1 – generational status; Model 2 – language of interview; Model 3 – English language proficiency. All models adjusted for education, gender, marital status, age, income, neighborhood safety and self-reported health. Bolded values have p < 0.05.

Discussion

We hypothesized that individuals who were more acculturated would experience more psychological distress, and that the additional role of being a caregiver to an older adult would further exacerbate this distress. That is, acculturated caregivers would have the highest levels of distress. Our results only partially confirmed our hypothesis. The healthy immigrant hypothesis was supported in our findings with non-caregivers, while findings among caregivers were indicative of models of acculturative stress. Among caregivers, those who were less acculturated experienced more psychological distress than those who were more acculturated. These acculturation–distress associations were supported in all acculturation proxy models – generational status, language of interview, and English language proficiency. Furthermore, race/ethnicity did not moderate these associations: the association between acculturation proxies and psychological distress was the same in both Latinos and AAPIs.

Acculturative stress is induced when there are pressures to adapt to a lifestyle and environment that is different from those in the native country and is exacerbated when an individual cannot reconcile and adapt to these changes (Berry, Reference Berry1970). Acculturative stress can affect multiple generations, and can be intensified for first-generation immigrants who are also caregivers (Cervantes et al., Reference Cervantes, Padilla, Napper and Goldbach2013). These individuals may face significant limitations to accessing psychological and social resources including support networks and educational and financial resources compared to their counterparts in later generations (Cervantes et al., Reference Cervantes, Padilla, Napper and Goldbach2013; Sörensen and Pinquart, Reference Sörensen and Pinquart2005). Caregiver burden studies have shown worse psychological distress outcomes among those with low-resource accessibility (Sörensen and Pinquart, Reference Sörensen and Pinquart2005). Furthermore, we found that caregivers who did not speak English well or completed the interview in a language other than English also experienced higher distress. This suggests that linguistic barriers may underlie this lack of resource accessibility and acculturative stress experienced by first-generation caregivers.

In alignment with the healthy immigrant effect, our study found that among non-caregivers, first-generation immigrants reported lower psychological distress than third generation. Both mechanisms proposed by the healthy immigrant effect, self-selection, and the salmon bias theory support our findings among less acculturated individuals (who were not caregivers). Self-selection is a premigration process and suggests that only the “healthiest” individuals leave their country and emigrate to the new host country. Salmon bias occurs when acculturative stress drives those who are unable to thrive in the host country to return to their country of origin (Abraído-Lanza et al., Reference Abraído-Lanza, Dohrenwend, Ng-Mak and Turner1999). Our results suggest that less acculturated non-caregivers, or rather first-generation immigrants, who experienced more acculturative stress, possibly caused by language barriers associated with having low-English proficiency, may return to their country of origin, while their counterparts who are caregivers might stay due to their caregiving responsibilities (Abraído-Lanza et al., Reference Abraído-Lanza, Echeverría and Flórez2016; Palloni and Arias, Reference Palloni and Arias2004). However, this speculation requires more in-depth study in future research.

In our study, two language variables served as the acculturation proxies: English proficiency and language of interview. In alignment with models of host language use as a proxy for acculturation, we found that both caregivers who were interviewed in a language other than English and those who were not proficient in English (i.e., less acculturated) had significantly higher psychological distress. These associations using the language variables mirrored results we obtained for generational status. Differences in psychological distress by acculturation status were much larger when English language proficiency was the proxy rather than language of interview. This difference was especially apparent among caregivers, in which the difference in psychological distress between less acculturated and more acculturated individuals was almost twice as large when using English proficiency rather than using language of interview. Thus, although both English proficiency and language of interview seem to mirror one another in their associations with psychological distress, they varied in strength as predictors, suggesting the importance of using varied language-based acculturation proxies.

Our results can also be understood in the context of a structural model of acculturation which acknowledges the role of structural barriers to acculturation (California Health Interview Survey, 2012 Castañeda et al., Reference Castañeda, Holmes, Madrigal, Young, Beyeler and Quesada2015; Viruell-Fuentes, Reference Viruell-Fuentes2007). For example, our findings indicated that third-generation individuals reported higher neighborhood safety scores than first- and second-generation non-caregivers. Furthermore, first-generation caregivers experienced more distress than second- and third-generation caregivers. Analysis of socioeconomic variables within the caregiver group revealed that first-generation caregivers had the lowest proportion of college graduates and lowest annual incomes compared with first-generation non-caregivers and caregivers in later generations. These results highlight that structural and contextual factors, such as education and financial resources, may contribute to health disparities among first-generation caregivers in both Asian and Latino immigrants.

Our study was not without limitations. We did not disaggregate findings based on AAPI or Latino ethnicity, and we know that within-group heterogeneity can be quite large in these populations. However, these groupings allowed us to observe psychological distress across broader populations and to test for interactions. Our findings may not be generalizable to individuals living outside of California, who may have different resources for caregiving. The 2009 CHIS had a response rate of 36.1% which raises concerns about selection bias (California Health Interview Survey, 2012). Although we know that caregivers were caring for an adult, because CHIS did not collect data on the care recipient’s characteristics (e.g., health problems and presence of dementia), we were unable to control for these important characteristics. Perceptions of neighborhood safety may be indicative of anxiety or negative affect rather than objective features of the neighborhood. The latter should be combined with subjective measures of neighborhood safety in future research (Robinette et al., Reference Robinette, Piazza and Stawski2021). Finally, although we are confident in the robustness of our results given consistent findings across acculturation proxies, future research should include a validated acculturation measure in addition to using proxies such as generational status and language use/proficiency.

To our knowledge, this study is the first to examine the relationship between multiple acculturation proxies and their impacts on psychological distress in caregivers and non-caregivers in a large, diverse, and population-based sample. Using three related but different measures allows for checking the robustness of the relation between acculturation and psychological distress. Our results showed that that among caregivers, differences in psychological distress were about 1–1.8 points between those who were least acculturated and those who were most acculturated. While these are relatively small effects for a scale that ranges from 0 to 24, identifying factors associated with any change in distress are theoretically important and can inform targets for intervention. In addition, our study highlights the need to build on discussion of within-group differences for understanding caregiver distress. Psychological distress and demographic variables of first-generation immigrants and subsequent generations differed markedly from each other (e.g., income level). Acculturation and generational differences need to be further dissected to create interventions and resources for caregivers that are context relevant. As US immigration policies continue to change rapidly, it is important to expand our understanding of how health disparities among first-generation and limited English proficient immigrants manifest, especially among those who may experience additional resource constraints due to their dual roles as immigrants and caregivers. Our study expands the sociocultural caregiving literature by demonstrating that nuances within AAPI and Latino racial/ethnic groups, such as level of acculturation, may have profound impacts on caregiver outcomes.

Conflict of interest

The authors have no conflict of interest. This research was partially supported by the National Institute on Aging [P30AG072972, K01AG052646, R01AG067541, and R24AG063718] and the Alzheimer’s Association [AARGD-19-619832]. The sponsors had no role in the conduct of the research.

Description of authors’ roles

JN formulated the research question, led the literature review, and wrote the manuscript. KZ and DH were responsible for the statistical analysis and interpretation and writing of the results. DH assisted with conceptualizing the study and assisted in writing the literature review. QD assisted in putting the results together. OM assisted in formulating the research question and guiding the conceptualization of the study. All authors assisted in writing the manuscript.

References

Abraído-Lanza, A. F., Dohrenwend, B. P., Ng-Mak, D. S. and Turner, J. B. (1999). The Latino mortality paradox: a test of the “salmon bias” and healthy migrant hypotheses. American Journal of Public Health, 89, 15431548. DOI 10.2105/ajph.89.10.1543.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Abraído-Lanza, A. F., Echeverría, S. E. and Flórez, K. R. (2016). Latino immigrants, acculturation, and health: promising new directions in research. Annual Review of Public Health, 37, 219236. DOI 10.1146/annurev-publhealth-032315-021545.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Acton, G. J. and Kang, J. (2001). Interventions to reduce the burden of caregiving for an adult with dementia: a meta-analysis. Research in Nursing & Health, 24, 349360. DOI https://doi.org/10.1002/nur.1036.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Angel, J. L., Rote, S. M., Brown, D. C., Angel, R. J. and Markides, K. S. (2014). Nativity status and sources of care assistance among elderly Mexican-origin adults. Journal of Cross-Cultural Gerontology, 29, 243258. DOI 10.1007/s10823-014-9234-9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bekteshi, V., Xu, Q. and Van Tran, T. (2015). Psychological distress among low-income U.S.- and foreign-born women of Mexican descent: impact of acculturation. Women’s Health Issues, 25, 185192. DOI 10.1016/j.whi.2014.11.001.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berry, J. W. (1970). Marginality, stress and ethnic identification in an acculturated aboriginal community. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 1, 239252. DOI 10.1177/135910457000100303.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berry, J. W. (2005). Acculturation: living successfully in two cultures. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 29, 697712. DOI https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijintrel.2005.07.013.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Booth, J., Ayers, S. and Marsiglia, F. (2014). Perceived neighborhood safety and psychological distress: exploring protective factors. Journal of Sociology and Social Welfare, 39. https://scholarworks.wmich.edu/cgi/viewcontent.cgi?article=3703&context=jssw Google Scholar
California Health Interview Survey (2012). CHIS 2009 Adult Survey. Los Angeles, CA: UCLA Center for Health Policy Research.Google Scholar
Castañeda, H., Holmes, S. M., Madrigal, D. S., Young, M.-E. D., Beyeler, N. and Quesada, J. (2015). Immigration as a social determinant of health. Annual Review of Public Health, 36, 375392. DOI 10.1146/annurev-publhealth-032013-182419.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cervantes, R. C., Padilla, A. M., Napper, L. E. and Goldbach, J. T. (2013). Acculturation-related stress and mental health outcomes among three generations of Hispanic adolescents. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 35, 451468. DOI 10.1177/0739986313500924.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chang, M. and Moon, A. (2016). Correlates and predictors of psychological distress among older Asian immigrants in California. Journal of Gerontological Social Work, 59, 7797. DOI 10.1080/01634372.2016.1140694.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dey, A. N. and Lucas, J. W. (2006). Physical and mental health characteristics of U.S.- and foreign-born adults: United States, 1998-2003. Advance Data, 369, 119.Google Scholar
Dilworth-Anderson, P., Williams, I. C. and Gibson, B. E. (2002). Issues of race, ethnicity, and culture in caregiving research: a 20-year review (1980-2000. The Gerontologist, 42, 237272. DOI 10.1093/geront/42.2.237.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frisbie, W. P., Cho, Y. and Hummer, R. A. (2001). Immigration and the health of Asian and Pacific Islander adults in the United States. American Journal of Epidemiology, 153, 372380. DOI 10.1093/aje/153.4.372.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gomez, S. L., Kelsey, J. L., Glaser, S. L., Lee, M. M. and Sidney, S. (2004). Immigration and acculturation in relation to health and health-related risk factors among specific Asian subgroups in a health maintenance organization. American Journal of Public Health, 94, 19771984. DOI 10.2105/ajph.94.11.1977.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kennedy, S., Kidd, M. P., McDonald, J. T. and Biddle, N. (2015). The healthy immigrant effect: patterns and evidence from four countries. Journal of International Migration and Integration, 16, 317332. DOI 10.1007/s12134-014-0340-x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kessler, R. C. et al. (2002). Short screening scales to monitor population prevalences and trends in non-specific psychological distress. Psychological Medicine, 32, 959976. DOI 10.1017/S0033291702006074.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, G. et al. (2011). Vulnerability of older Latino and Asian immigrants with limited english proficiency. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 59, 12461252. DOI 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2011.03483.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Knight, B. G. and Sayegh, P. (2009). Cultural values and caregiving: the updated sociocultural stress and coping model. The Journals of Gerontology: Series B, 65B, 513. DOI 10.1093/geronb/gbp096.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lara, M., Gamboa, C., Kahramanian, M. I., Morales, L. S. and Hayes Bautista, D. E. (2005). Acculturation and Latino health in the United States: a review of the literature and its sociopolitical context. Annual Review of Public Health, 26, 367397. DOI 10.1146/annurev.publhealth.26.021304.144615.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lawton, M. P., Kleban, M. H., Moss, M., Rovine, M. and Glicksman, A. (1989). Measuring caregiving appraisal. Journal of Gerontology, 44, P61P71. DOI 10.1093/geronj/44.3.P61.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, S., Nguyen, H. A. and Tsui, J. (2011). Interview language: a proxy measure for acculturation among Asian Americans in a population-based survey. Journal of Immigrant and Minority Health, 13, 244252. DOI 10.1007/s10903-009-9278-z.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lopez-Class, M., Castro, F. G. and Ramirez, A. G. (2011). Conceptions of acculturation: a review and statement of critical issues. Social Science & Medicine, 72, 15551562. DOI 10.1016/j.socscimed.2011.03.011.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mancenido, A., Williams, E. C. and Hajat, A. (2020). Examining psychological distress across intersections of immigrant generational status, race, poverty, and gender. Community Mental Health Journal, 56, 12691274. DOI 10.1007/s10597-020-00584-w.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Markides, K. S. and Eschbach, K. (2005). Aging, migration, and mortality: current status of research on the Hispanic paradox. The Journals of Gerontology: Series B, 60, S68S75. DOI 10.1093/geronb/60.Special_Issue_2.S68.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mather, M., Jacobsen, L. A. and Pollard, K. M. (2015). Population bulletin: Aging in the United States. Population Reference Bureau, 70, https://www.prb.org/wp-content/uploads/2019/07/population-bulletin-2015-70-2-aging-us.pdf.Google Scholar
Meyer, O. L., Liu, X., Nguyen, T.-N., Hinton, L. and Tancredi, D. (2018). Psychological distress of ethnically diverse adult caregivers in the California Health Interview Survey. Journal of Immigrant and Minority Health, 20, 784791. DOI 10.1007/s10903-017-0634-0.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miyawaki, C. E. (2016). Caregiving practice patterns of Asian, Hispanic, and Non-Hispanic White American family caregivers of older adults across generations. Journal of Cross-Cultural Gerontology, 31, 3555. DOI 10.1007/s10823-016-9281-5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Montenegro, X. (2014). Caregiving among Asian Americans and Pacific Islanders Age 50+. Available at: https://www.aarp.org/research/topics/care/info-2014/caregiving-asian-americans-pacific-islanders.html.Google Scholar
National Alliance for Caregiving, & AARP Public Policy Institute. (2015). Caregivers of older adults: a focused look at those caring for someone age 50+. Reserach Report: Caregiving in the United States 2015. Available at: https://www.aarp.org/content/dam/aarp/ppi/2015/caregivers-of-older-adults-focused-look.pdf.Google Scholar
Novak, M. and Guest, C. (1989). Application of a multidimensional caregiver burden inventory. The Gerontologist, 29, 798803. DOI 10.1093/geront/29.6.798.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Palloni, A. and Arias, E. (2004). Paradox lost: explaining the hispanic adult mortality advantage. Demography, 41, 385415. DOI 10.1353/dem.2004.0024.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pearlin, L. I., Menaghan, E. G., Lieberman, M. A. and Mullan, J. T. (1981). The stress process. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 22, 337356. DOI 10.2307/2136676.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Portes, A. (1996). New Second Generation. The Russell Sage Foundation. Available at: http://www.jstor.org/stable/10.7758/9781610444538.Google Scholar
Redfield, R., Linton, R. and Herskovits, M. J. (1936). Memorandum for the study of acculturation. American Anthropologist, 38, 149152. DOI 10.1525/aa.1936.38.1.02a00330.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robinette, J. W., Piazza, J. R. and Stawski, R. S. (2021). Neighborhood safety concerns and daily well-being: a national diary study. Wellbeing, Space, and Society, 2, 100047.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rote, S. M. and Moon, H. (2016). Racial/ethnic differences in caregiving frequency: does immigrant status matter? The Journals of Gerontology: Series B, 73, 10881098. DOI 10.1093/geronb/gbw106.Google Scholar
Salant, T. and Lauderdale, D. S. (2003). Measuring culture: a critical review of acculturation and health in Asian immigrant populations. Social Science & Medicine, 57, 7190. DOI 10.1016/S0277-9536(02)00300-3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
SAS version 9.4 (2022, SAS® 9.4 — Today and Tomorrow. Cary, NC: SAS Institute, Inc. Available at: https://support.sas.com/software/94/.Google Scholar
Sörensen, S. and Pinquart, M. (2005). Racial and ethnic differences in the relationship of caregiving stressors, resources, and sociodemographic variables to caregiver depression and perceived physical health. Aging & Mental Health, 9, 482495. DOI 10.1080/13607860500142796.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
U.S. Census Bureau. (2020). Selected characteristics of the foreign-born population by period of entry into the United States. Available at: https://data.census.gov/cedsci/table?q=foreign%20born&tid=ACSST5Y2020.S0502&moe=false.Google Scholar
Viruell-Fuentes, E. A. (2007). Beyond acculturation: immigration, discrimination, and health research among Mexicans in the United States. Social Science & Medicine, 65, 15241535. DOI 10.1016/j.socscimed.2007.05.010.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Weighted demographic characteristics of the sample (n = 13,161)

Figure 1

Figure 1. Generation status, interview language, and English proficiency by caregiver status interaction on psychological distress. Model adjusts for continuous covariates centered at the mean. Caregiving status is a dichotomous variable.

Figure 2

Table 2. Multivariate regression of variables associated with psychological distressa