Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-jkksz Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T22:11:55.171Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Errors on the MoCA's animal-naming: findings from Parkinson's disease patients

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 March 2017

Yassar Alamri*
Affiliation:
New Zealand Brain Research Institute and Canterbury District Health Board, Christchurch, New Zealand
Tim Anderson
Affiliation:
New Zealand Brain Research Institute and University of Otago, Christchurch, New Zealand
John Dalrymple-Alford
Affiliation:
New Zealand Brain Research Institute and University of Canterbury, Christchurch, New Zealand
Michael Macaskill
Affiliation:
New Zealand Brain Research Institute and University of Otago, Christchurch, New Zealand

Extract

We read the findings by Cecato et al. (2016) with great interest. In their study, naming the rhinoceros discriminated between patients with amnestic mild cognitive impairment (aMCI) and Alzheimer's disease (AD) but not healthy controls (HC). Of note, HC participants were significantly younger than aMCI and AD patients. All participants were administered the original version of the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) instrument.

Type
Letter to the Editor
Copyright
Copyright © International Psychogeriatric Association 2017 

We read the findings by Cecato et al. (Reference Cecato, Martinelli, Izbicki, Yassuda and Aprahamian2016) with great interest. In their study, naming the rhinoceros discriminated between patients with amnestic mild cognitive impairment (aMCI) and Alzheimer's disease (AD) but not healthy controls (HC). Of note, HC participants were significantly younger than aMCI and AD patients. All participants were administered the original version of the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) instrument.

We recently recruited a group of Parkinson's disease (PD) patients and matched HC as part of an eye-movement study. A total of 45 participants were included: 15 PD with normal cognition (PD-N), 14 PD with mild cognitive impairment (PD-MCI) and 16 age-, education-, and sex-matched HC. All PD-MCI participants at our institution are diagnosed as such according to the Movement Disorders Society level II criteria, which incorporate the MoCA as one of the utilized instruments (Litvan et al., Reference Litvan2012). All participants were asked to name the animals of the original MoCA as well as those in two alternative MoCA versions (i.e. nine animals in total; Nasreddine, Reference Nasreddine2017). Animals were presented three-at-a-time, as on the paper form.

The percent correct animal-naming was equivalent among the study groups: PD-N 96%, PD-MCI 94%, and HC 98% (F 2,44 = 1.2, p = 0.3). Naming errors included the misidentification of the rhinoceros (MoCA 1), hippopotamus (MoCA 2), giraffe (MoCA 2) and donkey (MoCA 3), but did not differ among the participants (see Table S1, available as supplementary material attached to the electronic version of this paper at https://doi.org/10.1017/S1041610217000345).

Several factors may explain the superior performance of our PD patients compared with aMCI and AD patients in the study by Cecato and colleagues (Reference Cecato, Martinelli, Izbicki, Yassuda and Aprahamian2016). Unlike patients with aMCI and AD, PD patients do not normally exhibit naming deficits until later in the disease process when PD-dementia supervenes (Frank et al., Reference Frank, McDade and Scott1996). Our sample did not include patients with PD-dementia.

Our PD participants were generally younger than aMCI and AD patients reported by Cecato et al. (Reference Cecato, Martinelli, Izbicki, Yassuda and Aprahamian2016), were predominantly male and had many years of formal education – all factors that have been found to significantly influence animal-naming on the MoCA (Del Brutto and Wright, Reference Del Brutto and Wright2015). In addition, the sample for our study is from New Zealand, compared with a Brazilian sample reported by Cecato et al. (Reference Cecato, Martinelli, Izbicki, Yassuda and Aprahamian2016). The influence of sociocultural factors on performance has not been extensively evaluated, but Del Brutto and colleagues found that a common mistake made by elderly participants in rural Ecuador was mistaking the rhinoceros for a cow – a much more familiar animal to the studied farming community (2015).

In conclusion, we did not find MoCA animal-naming to discriminate among PD-N, PD-MCI, and HC participants. It is possible that the small number of our sample led to an underpowered study. Future larger studies – especially those that include PD-dementia patients – ought to provide a fuller picture of the discriminatory value of MoCA animal-naming in this disorder.

Conflict of interest

None.

Supplementary material

To view supplementary material for this article, please visit https://doi.org/10.1017/S1041610217000345

References

Cecato, J. F., Martinelli, J. E., Izbicki, R., Yassuda, M. S. and Aprahamian, I. (2016). A subtest analysis of the montreal cognitive assessment (MoCA): which subtests can best discriminate between healthy controls, mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer's disease? International Psychogeriatrics, 28, 825832. doi: 10.1017/s1041610215001982.Google Scholar
Del Brutto, O. H. and Wright, C. (2015). Animal naming in the Spanish version of the montreal cognitive assessment in rural Latin American communities: a cautionary note. Geriatrics & Gerontology International, 15, 126127.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frank, E. M., McDade, H. L. and Scott, W. K. (1996). Naming in dementia secondary to Parkinson's, Huntington's, and Alzheimer's diseases. Journal of Communication Disorders, 29, 183197.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Litvan, I. et al. (2012). Diagnostic criteria for mild cognitive impairment in Parkinson's disease: movement disorder society task force guidelines. Movement Disorders, 27, 349356.Google Scholar
Nasreddine, Z. (2017). MoCA full tests. Available at http://www.mocatest.org/paper-tests/moca-test-full/; last accessed 05 January 2017.Google Scholar
Supplementary material: File

Alamri supplementary material

Table S1

Download Alamri supplementary material(File)
File 14.5 KB