Hostname: page-component-5f745c7db-rgzdr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-01-06T06:54:46.878Z Has data issue: true hasContentIssue false

Gender differences in giving in the Dictator Game: the role of reluctant altruism

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 January 2025

David Klinowski*
Affiliation:
Santiago Centre for Experimental Social Sciences, Nuffield College, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK Department of Economics, Universidad de Santiago de Chile, Santiago, Chile

Abstract

The Dictator Game has been a helpful tool to study whether men or women are more generous. But recent work suggests that motivations other than generosity also influence behavior in this game. Image concerns and expectations management may cause dictators to “give reluctantly”; that is, to share money with the recipient if asked to, but to renege on their gifts if they can do so without being detected. We provide evidence from two separate experiments that females are more likely than males to give reluctantly in the Dictator Game. After accounting for retraction of gifts, males and females transfer similar amounts to the recipient in expectation. The results suggest that gender differences in non-payoff-related motivations may play a role in producing gender differences in giving in the Dictator Game.

Type
Original Paper
Copyright
Copyright © 2018 Economic Science Association

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

Electronic supplementary material The online version of this article (https://doi.org/10.1007/s40881-018-0058-1) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.

This research was supported in part by a grant from the Science of Philanthropy Initiative. I also would like to thank two anonymous referees and the editor, Nikos Nikiforakis, for comments that greatly improved this paper.

References

Andreoni, J., Bernheim, D. (2009). Social image and the 50–50 norm: A theoretical and experimental analysis of audience effects. Econometrica, 77(5), 16071636. 10.3982/ECTA7384Google Scholar
Andreoni, J., Vesterlund, L. (2001). Which is the fair sex: Gender differences in altruism. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 116(1), 293312. 10.1162/003355301556419CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bardsley, N. (2008). Dictator game giving: Altruism or artefact? Experimental Economics, 11(2), 122133. 10.1007/s10683-007-9172-2CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bertrand, M. (2011). New perspectives on gender. In Handbook of labor economics, Amsterdam: Elsevier 15431590.Google Scholar
Broberg, T., Ellingsen, T., Johannesson, M. (2007). Is generosity involuntary? Economics Letters, 94(1), 3237. 10.1016/j.econlet.2006.07.006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cain, D. M., Dana, J., Newman, G. E. (2014). Giving versus giving in. The Academy of Management Annals, 8(1), 505533. 10.5465/19416520.2014.911576CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Camerer, C. F. (2003). Behavioral game theory: Experiments in strategic interactions, Princeton: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Camerer, C. F., Thaler, R. H. (1995). Anomalies: Ultimatums, dictators, and manners. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 9(2), 209219. 10.1257/jep.9.2.209CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, D. L., Schonger, M., Wickens, C. (2016). oTree: An open-source platform for laboratory, online, and field experiments. Journal of Behavioral and Experimental Finance, 9(1), 8897. 10.1016/j.jbef.2015.12.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Croson, R., Gneezy, U. (2009). Gender differences in preferences. Journal of Economic Literature, 47(2), 448474. 10.1257/jel.47.2.448CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dana, J., Cain, D. M., Dawes, R. M. (2006). What you don’t know won’t hurt me: Costly (but Quiet) exit in dictator games. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 100(2), 193201. 10.1016/j.obhdp.2005.10.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dariel, A., Nikiforakis, N. (2014). Cooperators and reciprocators: A within-subject analysis of prosocial behavior. Economics Letters, 122(2), 163166. 10.1016/j.econlet.2013.10.033CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Wit, A., Bekkers, R. (2016). Exploring gender differences in charitable giving. Nonprofit and Voluntary Sector Quarterly, 45(4), 741761. 10.1177/0899764015601242CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DellaVigna, S., List, J. A., Malmendier, U., Rao, G. (2013). The importance of being marginal: Gender differences in generosity. American Economic Review: Papers and Proceedings, 103(3), 586590. 10.1257/aer.103.3.586CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Exley, C. L. (2018). Incentives for prosocial behavior: The role of reputations. Management Science., 64, 24602471. 10.1287/mnsc.2016.2685 (forthcoming).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fehr, E., Schmidt, K. M. (1999). A theory of fairness, competition, and cooperation. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 114(3), 817868. 10.1162/003355399556151CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fischbacher, U. (2007). Z-Tree: Zurich toolbox for ready-made economic experiments. Experimental Economics, 10(2), 171178. 10.1007/s10683-006-9159-4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Forsythe, R., Horowitz, J. L., Savin, N. E., Sefton, M. (1994). Fairness in simple bargaining experiments. Games and Economic Behavior, 6(3), 347369. 10.1006/game.1994.1021CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gill, D., Prowse, V. (2012). A structural analysis of disappointment aversion in a real effort competition. American Economic Review, 102(1), 469503. 10.1257/aer.102.1.469CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gino, F., Norton, M. I., Weber, R. A. (2016). Motivated Bayesians: Feeling moral while acting egoistically. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 30(3), 189212. 10.1257/jep.30.3.189CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gneezy, U., Imas, A., Madarász, K. (2014). Conscience accounting: Emotion dynamics and social behavior. Management Science, 60(11), 26452658. 10.1287/mnsc.2014.1942CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greiner, B. (2015). Subject pool recruitment procedures: organizing experiments with ORSEE. Journal of the Economic Science Association, 1(1), 114125. 10.1007/s40881-015-0004-4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jones, D., Linardi, S. (2014). Wallflowers: Experimental evidence of an aversion to standing out. Management Science, 60(7), 17571771. 10.1287/mnsc.2013.1837CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klinowski, D. (2016). Reluctant donors and their reactions to social information. Working Paper.Google Scholar
Lazear, E. P., Malmendier, U., Weber, R. A. (2012). Sorting in experiments with application to social preferences. American Economic Journal: Applied Economics, 4(1), 136163.Google Scholar
List, J. A. (2007). On the interpretation of giving in dictator games. Journal of Political Economy, 115(3), 482493. 10.1086/519249CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Niederle, M. (2016). Gender. In Handbook of experimental economics, Princeton: Princeton University Press 481562.Google Scholar
Sachdeva, S., Iliev, R., Medin, D. L. (2009). Sinning saints and saintly sinners: The paradox of moral self-regulation. Psychological Science, 20(4), 523528. 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02326.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Supplementary material: File

Klinowski supplementary material

Klinowski supplementary material
Download Klinowski supplementary material(File)
File 70.3 MB