Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-g4j75 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-01-07T18:07:02.673Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Pronoun drop and prosocial behavior: experimental evidence from Japan

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 January 2025

Tai-Sen He*
Affiliation:
Economics Programme, School of Social Sciences, Nanyang Technological University, 48 Nanyang Avenue, Singapore 639818, Singapore
Yohanes E. Riyanto*
Affiliation:
Economics Programme, School of Social Sciences, Nanyang Technological University, 48 Nanyang Avenue, Singapore 639818, Singapore
Saori C. Tanaka*
Affiliation:
Advanced Telecommunication Research Institute International, Keihanna Science City, Kyoto 619-0288, Japan
Katsunori Yamada*
Affiliation:
Faculty of Economics, Kindai University, 228-3, Shin-Kami-Kosaka, Higashiōsaka 577-0813, Japan

Abstract

We join a growing body of literature suggesting that the languages people speak influence their decision-making. We tested whether dropping the first-person pronoun “I” affects pro-social behavior in a dictator game-like setting. To this end, we conducted an online randomized, incentivized experiment with a socially representative sample of 2000 Japanese respondents. We provide compelling causal evidence that pronoun dropping reduces pro-sociality. Given that our results provide little empirical support for previous research findings linking first-person pronoun use and lower pro-sociality, we prescribe caution in using languages as a proxy for culture in several cross-country empirical studies in economics.

Type
Original Paper
Copyright
Copyright © Economic Science Association 2020

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

Electronic supplementary material The online version of this article (https://doi.org/10.1007/s40881-020-00083-4) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.

References

Ackermann, K. A., & Murphy, R. O. (2012). Tutorial on how to evaluate the SVO slider measure’s secondary items. https://ryanomurphy.com/resources/SVO_second_item_tutorial.pdf.Google Scholar
Alesina, A., Giuliano, P. (2015). Culture and institutions. Journal of Economic Literature, 53(4), 898944. 10.1257/jel.53.4.898CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Amir, O., Rand, D. G., Gal, Y. K. (2012). Economic games on the Internet: The effect of $1 stakes. PLoS ONE, 7(2),e31461. 10.1371/journal.pone.0031461CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Breuer, W., Riesener, M., & Salzmann, A. J. (2014). Risk aversion vs. individualism: What drives risk taking in household finance? The European Journal of Finance, 20(5), 446462.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, M. K. (2013). The effect of language on economic behavior: Evidence from savings rates, health behaviors, and retirement assets. American Economic Review, 10.2139/ssrn.1914379CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, J. I., He, T.-S., Riyanto, Y. E. (2019). The effect of language on economic behavior: Examining the causal link between future tense and time preference in the lab. European Economic Review, 120, 103307. 10.1016/j.euroecorev.2019.103307CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davis, L. S., Abdurazokzoda, F. (2016). Language, culture and institutions: Evidence from a new linguistic dataset. Journal of Comparative Economics, 44, 541561. 10.1016/j.jce.2015.10.015CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Falk, A., Becker, A., Dohmen, T., Enke, B., Huffman, D., Sunde, U. (2018). Global evidence on economic preferences. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 133(4), 16451692. 10.1093/qje/qjy013CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feldmann, H. (2019). Do linguistic structures affect human capital? The case of pronoun drop. Kyklos, 72, 2954. 10.1111/kykl.12190CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gorodnichenko, Y., Roland, G. (2011). Individualism, innovation, and long-run growth. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 108, 2131621319. 10.1073/pnas.1101933108CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gorodnichenko, Y., Roland, G. (2011). Which dimensions of culture matter for long-run growth? American Economic Review, 101, 492498. 10.1257/aer.101.3.492CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gorodnichenko, Y., Roland, G. (2017). Culture, institutions, and the wealth of nations. The Review of Economics and Statistics, 99(3), 402416. 10.1162/REST_a_00599CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hofstede, G. (2001). Culture’s consequences: Comparing values, behaviors, institutions and organizations across nations, 2Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Kammas, P., Kazakis, P., Sarantides, V. (2017). The effect of culture on fiscal redistribution: Evidence based on genetic, epidemiological and linguistic data. Economics Letters, 160, 9599. 10.1016/j.econlet.2017.08.029CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kashima, E. S., Kashima, Y. (1998). Culture and language: The case of cultural dimensions and personal pronoun use. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 29(3), 461486. 10.1177/0022022198293005CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kashima, Y., Kashima, E. S. (2003). Individualism, GNP, climate, and pronoun drop. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 34(1), 125134. 10.1177/0022022102239159CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kay, P., Kempton, W. (1984). What is the Sapir-Whorf hypothesis? American Anthropologist, 86(1), 6579. 10.1525/aa.1984.86.1.02a00050CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klasing, M. J. (2013). Cultural dimensions, collective values and their importance for institutions. Journal of Comparative Economics, 41(2), 447467. 10.1016/j.jce.2012.09.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Licht, A. N., Goldschmidt, C., Schwartz, S. H. (2007). Culture rules: The foundations of the rule of law and other norms of governance. Journal of Comparative Economics, 35(4), 659688. 10.1016/j.jce.2007.09.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Murphy, R. O., Ackermann, K. A., Handgraaf, M. J. J. (2011). Measuring social value orientation. Judgment and Decision Making, 6(8), 771781.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pérez, E. O., Tavits, M. (2017). Language shapes people’s time perspective and support for future-oriented policies. American Journal of Political Science, 61(3), 715727. 10.1111/ajps.12290CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roberts, S. G., Winters, J., Chen, K. (2015). Future tense and economic decisions: Controlling for cultural evolution. PLoS ONE, 10(7),e0132145. 10.1371/journal.pone.0132145CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sutter, M., Angerer, S., Glätzle-Rützler, D., Lergetporer, P. (2018). Language group differences in time preferences: Evidence from primary school children in a bilingual city. European Economic Review, 106(July), 2134. 10.1016/j.euroecorev.2018.04.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tabellini, G. (2010). Culture and institutions: Economic development in the regions of Europe. Journal of the European Economic Association, 8(4), 677716. 10.1111/j.1542-4774.2010.tb00537.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Utz, S. (2004). Self-activation is a two-edged sword: The effects of I primes on cooperation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 40(6), 769776. 10.1016/j.jesp.2004.03.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Supplementary material: File

He et al. supplementary material 1

He et al. supplementary material 1
Download He et al. supplementary material 1(File)
File 1.7 MB
Supplementary material: File

He et al. supplementary material 2

He et al. supplementary material 2
Download He et al. supplementary material 2(File)
File 1.5 MB
Supplementary material: File

He et al. supplementary material 3

He et al. supplementary material 3
Download He et al. supplementary material 3(File)
File 405.8 KB
Supplementary material: File

He et al. supplementary material 4

He et al. supplementary material 4
Download He et al. supplementary material 4(File)
File 141.6 KB