Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-dzt6s Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T14:57:14.194Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Patterns of visual sensory and sensorimotor abnormalities in autism vary in relation to history of early language delay

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  27 October 2008

YUKARI TAKARAE*
Affiliation:
Center for Mind and Brain, University of California, Davis, Davis, California
BEATRIZ LUNA
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania Department of Psychology, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania
NANCY J. MINSHEW
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania Department of Neurology, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania
JOHN A. SWEENEY
Affiliation:
Center for Cognitive Medicine, University of Illinois at Chicago, Chicago, Illinois Department of Psychiatry, University of Illinois at Chicago, Chicago, Illinois Department of Neurology, University of Illinois at Chicago, Chicago, Illinois Department of Psychology, University of Illinois at Chicago, Chicago, Illinois
*
Correspondence and reprint requests to: Yukari Takarae, Ph.D., 267 Cousteau Place, Center for Mind and Brain, University of California, Davis, Davis, CA 95616. E-mail: ytakarae@ucdavis.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Visual motion perception and pursuit eye movement deficits have been reported in autism. However, it is unclear whether these impairments are related to each other or to clinical symptoms of the disorder. High-functioning individuals with autism (41 with and 36 without delayed language acquisition) and 46 control subjects participated in the present study. All three subject groups were matched on chronological age and Full-Scale IQ. The autism group with delayed language acquisition had bilateral impairments on visual motion discrimination tasks, whereas the autism group without delay showed marginal impairments only in the left hemifield. Both autism groups showed difficulty tracking visual targets, but only the autism group without delayed language acquisition showed increased pursuit latencies and a failure to show the typical rightward directional advantage in pursuit. We observed correlations between performance on the visual perception and pursuit tasks in both autism groups. However, pursuit performance was correlated with manual motor skills only in the autism group with delayed language, suggesting that general sensorimotor or motor disturbances are a significant additional factor related to pursuit deficits in this subgroup. These findings suggest that there may be distinct neurocognitive phenotypes in autism associated with patterns of early language development. (JINS, 2008, 14, 980–989.)

Type
Symposium
Copyright
Copyright © The International Neuropsychological Society 2008

INTRODUCTION

Autism is a genetic disorder that is associated with impairments in sensorimotor behaviors as well as deficits in language, cognition and social skills. Impairments in postural control (Minshew et al., Reference Minshew, Sung, Jones and Furman2004; Molloy et al., Reference Molloy, Dietrich and Bhattacharya2003), eye movements (Goldberg et al., Reference Goldberg, Lasker, Zee, Garth, Tien and Landa2002; Luna et al., Reference Luna, Doll, Hegedus, Minshew and Sweeney2007; Takarae et al., Reference Takarae, Minshew, Luna, Krisky and Sweeney2004a), and motor learning (Mostofsky et al., Reference Mostofsky, Goldberg, Landa and Denckla2000; Müller et al., Reference Müller, Cauich, Rubio, Mizuno and Courchesne2004; Rinehart et al., Reference Rinehart, Bradshaw, Brereton and Tonge2001); all have been reported in autism. Sensorimotor functions are supported by extensive cortical and subcortical networks. Because the neuroanatomy of sensorimotor systems are well understood, and sensory inputs and motor responses are more easily quantifiable than most higher cognitive processes, an examination of sensorimotor behaviors offers advantages to investigate what neural circuits are selectively affected in autism.

Whereas sensorimotor impairments have been reported in autism, it is not clear whether sensorimotor deficits in autism are primarily consequences of altered sensory input to sensorimotor systems, or result from intrinsic disturbances in sensorimotor systems themselves. For example, smooth pursuit eye movements, which maintain visual focus on moving objects, depend on the analysis of visual motion information and are initiated in response to visual movement (Ilg, Reference Ilg1997; Newsome et al., Reference Newsome, Wurtz, Dursteler and Mikami1985). The emergence of smooth pursuit during development follows maturation of the extrastriate area MT that is dedicated to visual motion processing (Johnson, Reference Johnson1990), suggesting a closely coordinated timing in maturation in the abilities to perceive and respond to visual motion signals.

Several studies have reported impairments in visual pursuit in autism. Scharre & Creedon (Reference Scharre and Creedon1992) and Rosenhall et al. (Reference Rosenhall, Johansson and Gillberg1988) reported difficulty inducing smooth pursuit in subjects with autism. Though the latter reported some normalization of pursuit accuracy once smooth pursuit was successfully induced, lack of pursuit responses in most subjects points to abnormality in the pursuit system. Kemner et al. (Reference Kemner, van der Geest, Verbaten and van Engeland2004) studied smooth pursuit eye movements in 14 children with Pervasive Developmental Disorders (PDD) and 14 age and IQ matched control children. Whereas not statistically significant, they reported about a 10% reduction in pursuit gain (pursuit velocity relative to target velocity) in the PDD group. Takarae and colleagues (Takarae et al., Reference Takarae, Minshew, Luna, Krisky and Sweeney2004a) found pursuit impairments in 60 high-functioning individuals with autism compared to 94 age and IQ matched typically developing control subjects. They reported unidirectional impairments during pursuit to the right during the initiation of smooth pursuit and bidirectional impairments in sustained pursuit driven by predictive signals.

Pursuit impairments in autism might be caused by disturbances in visual processing because past studies have reported impairments in visual motion perception in autism (Bertone et al., Reference Bertone, Mottron, Jelenic and Faubert2003; Milne et al., Reference Milne, Swettenham, Hansen, Campbell, Jeffries and Plaisted2002; Spencer et al., Reference Spencer, O'Brien, Riggs, Braddick, Atkinson and Wattam-Bell2000). The degree to which impairments in visual perception explain visual sensorimotor deficits in autism is, however, not clear. Our past work documented significant correlations between pursuit performance and manual motor skills in autism (Takarae et al., Reference Takarae, Minshew, Luna, Krisky and Sweeney2004a), suggesting that visual pursuit impairments might be part of more general visual sensorimotor or motor impairments. Milne et al. (Milne et al., Reference Milne, White, Campbell, Swettenham, Hansen and Ramus2006) reported abnormally high motion detection thresholds in only 22% of their participants, and whether or not individuals with more severe motion perception impairments have more severe pursuit impairments remains to be established. There is potential heterogeneity with respect to the development of sensorimotor and sensory brain systems in autism, in which each system could be more or less impaired to result in general sensorimotor disturbances.

The current study examined visual motion perception, visual pursuit, manual motor control, and relationships between these behavioral domains in autism. We used two different visual pursuit tasks because some aspects of visual pursuit are more sensitive to the quality of visual sensory input to the oculomotor system than others. Lesions in the area MT most profoundly affect latency of pursuit eye movements and pursuit gain during the initial 100 msec from the onset of smooth pursuit (referred to as the open-loop stage) (Dursteler & Wurtz, Reference Dursteler and Wurtz1988; Newsome et al., Reference Newsome, Wurtz, Dursteler and Mikami1985). Thus, we used two different paradigms to examine pursuit initiation and maintenance separately. We also used two different paradigms to examine visual motion processing, because previous literature has suggested that performance on visual motion tasks can depend on stimulus characteristics (Bertone et al., Reference Bertone, Mottron, Jelenic and Faubert2003). One task was a motion coherence task which requires integration of local motion vectors to compute global motion, and the other was based on detecting moving gratings which could be performed by detection of local motion alone. Because we previously found lateralized impairments during pursuit initiation, we assessed performance in the left and right hemifield separately during visual pursuit and visual motion tasks. To our knowledge, this is the first study that examined visual motion perception in each hemifield separately in autism. We also used neuropsychological measures of manual motor skills to replicate our previous findings of a relationship between manual motor skills and visual pursuit performance and to assess the generality of sensorimotor deficits across visuomotor systems.

Because of previous findings indicating that visual perception may be differentially affected in high-functioning autism and Asperger's Disorder (Spencer & O'Brien, Reference Spencer and O'Brien2006), we divided participants meeting the Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised (ADI-R) and the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS) criteria for autism according to their history of early language development. That is one of critical factors differentiating the two clinical disorders. A diagnosis of Asperger's Disorder requires the absence of delayed language development, whereas DSM-IV criteria for Autistic Disorder do not require delayed language development. Thus, one could have a diagnosis of Autistic Disorder regardless of the presence or absence of delayed language development. In DSM-IV, diagnosis of Autistic Disorder supercedes Asperger's Disorder, and thus the difference between two disorders reflects both qualitative and quantitative differences. In the current study, we compared two autism groups with similar symptom severity on the ADOS rating scale.

Defining subgroups of individuals with autism based on language development has been proposed as a viable phenotype in autism by genetic and brain imaging studies as well as behavioral analysis (Tager-Flusberg & Joseph, Reference Tager-Flusberg and Joseph2003), and those with language delay might have specific genetic alterations (Alarcon et al., Reference Alarcon, Cantor, Liu, Gilliam and Geschwind2002; Bradford et al., Reference Bradford, Haines, Hutcheson, Gardiner, Braun, Sheffield, Cassavant, Huang, Wang, Vieland, Folstein, Santangelo and Piven2001). Our previous work on saccadic eye movements documented that autism groups that were defined by the presence or absence of early language delay were associated with a distinct pattern of neurophysiological impairments (Takarae et al., Reference Takarae, Minshew, Luna and Sweeney2004b). Delay in language acquisition likely represents an observable clinical marker for changes in complex brain maturation at the age that early language skills are acquired; these have diverse implications for many neuropsychological processes. Effects involving language development and visual processing might occur via alteration in the superior temporal sulcus or its white matter connections that are implicated in autism and are associated with both verbal abilities and high-level visual processing (Redcay, Reference Redcay2008; Zilbovicius et al., Reference Zilbovicius, Meresse, Chabane, Brunelle, Samson and Boddaert2006). In the present study, we examined whether problems in early language acquisition are related to impairments in visual pursuit and visual motion perception.

METHODS

Participants

Participants were 77 individuals with autism and 46 typically developing individuals matched for age and Full-Scale IQ from age-appropriate versions of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale (Table 1). One control subject and 4 subjects with autism that were included in our previous work on smooth pursuit eye movements (Takarae et al., Reference Takarae, Minshew, Luna, Krisky and Sweeney2004a) were retested for this study, and the remaining 118 subjects were newly recruited. The diagnosis of autism was established by expert clinical opinion verified by results from the ADI-R and ADOS (Lord et al., Reference Lord, Rutter, Goode, Heemsbergen, Jordan, Mawhood and Schopler1989; Lord et al., Reference Lord, Rutter and Le Couteur1994; Lord et al., Reference Lord, Risi, Lambrecht, Cook, Leventhal, DiLavore, Pickles and Rutter2000). Subjects were excluded if they had an associated infectious, genetic, or metabolic disorder known to cause autistic features such as fragile X syndrome or tuberous sclerosis. Individuals with autism were divided into two groups depending on the presence of delay in language acquisition in their developmental history. Language delay was defined as the absence of single word utterances by 24 months and/or spoken phrases by three years of age. This resulted in a group of 41 individuals with delay in language acquisition and 36 individuals without delay. Average ADOS scores for these groups were similar (Table 1).

Table 1. Demographic and neuropsychological characteristics of participants

Typically developing control participants had no personal history of psychiatric or neurological disorder, no family history of autism, and no first-degree relatives with any neuropsychiatric disorder considered to have a genetic component. They had no personal history of developmental delay, significant problems in school performance, or signs of learning disability in psychoeducational testing that included language tests as described in our previous reports (Minshew et al., Reference Minshew, Goldstein and Siegel1997; Williams et al., Reference Williams, Goldstein and Minshew2006). No participants were taking stimulant, lithium, antipsychotic, or anticonvulsant medications at the time of testing. Twelve subjects with autism were taking antidepressants. No participant had a history of head injury, birth injury, or seizure disorder. Far acuity of all participants was normal or corrected to at least 20/40. After providing the complete description of the study, informed consent (or assent) was obtained from all participants and consent was obtained for parents/guardians for those under 18 years of age. The study was approved by the Institutional Review Boards of the University of Pittsburgh and University of Illinois at Chicago.

Procedures

Participants were tested in a darkened flat black room, with task instructions provided via intercom. Targets for pursuit paradigms were presented in the horizontal plane at eye level on an acrylic hemi-arc with 1-meter radius. Participants were seated at the center of the arc. A laser diode module was mounted immediately over participants' heads to produce a 3-mm point source of light. The light source was reflected onto the arc by the use of a mirror attached to a rotary stage assembly (New England Affiliated Technologies #1121-142, Lawrence, MA) that manipulated velocity profiles and locations of targets. Stimuli for visual motion tasks were presented using a CRT monitor (SONY, GDM-FW900 Multiscan, New York, NY) with subjects seated 27” distance from the monitor.

Eye movements were monitored using infrared reflection sensors mounted on spectacle frames (Applied Science Laboratories, Inc., Model 210, Bedford, MA) during eye movement and visual motion tasks. Subjects' heads were secured in a chinrest with a head strap to minimize head movement. An experimenter monitored eye movement activity throughout testing to ensure that participants were alert and performing tasks according to instructions. Eye movement recordings were digitized at 500 Hz with a 16 bit A/D converter (Dataq Instruments, DI-720, Akron, OH) and processed with a custom finite impulse response filter. Details of the data processing stream and algorithm used to classify eye movements are presented elsewhere (Takarae et al., Reference Takarae, Minshew, Luna, Krisky and Sweeney2004a).

Eye Movement Tasks

Foveofugal Step-ramp Pursuit Tracking Task

Targets were presented at center for 2–4 seconds, and then stepped 3 degrees to the left or right and immediately continued moving in the same direction away from center at a constant speed of 4, 8, 16, or 24 °/sec (Fig. 1). The target disappeared after reaching ±15°. One second later, the target reappeared at center to begin the next trial. The duration of central fixation, and the speed and direction of target movement, were pseudorandomly assigned for each trial.

Fig. 1. Schematic presentation of a foveofugal step ramp task. The diagram shows a time (horizontal axis) by target position (vertical axis) plot for a single trial of this task. The dashed line represents target movement with an initial 3° step followed by smooth movement towards the left (negative) 15-degree position. The line above illustrates a typical eye movement response in which an initial catch-up saccade (dotted line) to the target is followed by smooth pursuit.

In this paradigm, a saccade typically occurs approximately 200 msec after the onset of target motion so that the eyes can catch up to the moving target, followed immediately by smooth pursuit. The initial phase of the pursuit response (defined here as the first 100 msec of smooth pursuit after the end of the first catch-up saccade) is referred to as the open-loop stage because it is primarily driven by sensory input. Pursuit after that point, the closed-loop period, is driven primarily by internally generated feedback about performance accuracy and predictions about target motion. This task has been extensively used in studies of visual motion perception with non-human primates, and open-loop pursuit becomes severely impaired after lesions in extrastriate area MT (Newsome et al., Reference Newsome, Wurtz, Dursteler and Mikami1985). In contrast, damage to premotor areas, such as the frontal eye fields, tend to produce deficits that are consistent in the open- and closed-loop stages of visual pursuit (Lynch, Reference Lynch1987; Rivaud et al., Reference Rivaud, Muri, Gaymard, Vermersch and Pierrot-Deseilligny1994). Primary measures of performance were pursuit gain from the open-loop and closed-loop stages, which was defined as the ratio of average pursuit eye movement velocity over target velocity. This task consisted of a total of 32 trials (4 trials × 4 velocities × 2 directions).

Pure Ramp Task

Targets were presented at the central location for 2–4 seconds, and then swept to the left or right at a constant speed (4, 8, 16, 24, or 32 °/sec) and then terminated after reaching ±15 ° (Fig. 2). This task was designed to measure visually guided pursuit initiation, a parameter whose latency is prolonged by lesions to visual cortex (Heide et al., Reference Heide, Kurzidim and Kompf1996; Lisberger & Pavelko, Reference Lisberger and Pavelko1989). Thus, pursuit latency was used as a primary measure for the reactivity of visual pursuit systems to visual motion and was defined as the time for the eyes to achieve a pursuit velocity of at least 2 degrees/sec that was maintained for at least 20 msec. Average pursuit gain and the number of catch-up saccades made to compensate for slow pursuit were also obtained. Forty trials were performed (4 trials × 5 velocities × 2 directions). In both visual pursuit tasks, each trial began with presentation of a 0.5 sec auditory tone at center fixation concurrently with the presentation of the central visual target to alert participants to prepare for the next trial.

Fig. 2. Schematic presentation of a pure ramp task. The diagram shows a time (horizontal axis) by target position (vertical axis) plot for a trial with a target moving from center to a left (negative) 15-degree position. The dashed line shows target movement. The line above shows a trial where smooth pursuit is sometimes interrupted by a catch-up saccade that corrects for tracking error (shown in a dotted line).

Visual Motion Perception Tasks

Of 123 subjects who completed the pursuit tasks, 110 subjects (46 controls, 37 subjects with autism who had language delay, and 27 subjects with autism who did not have language delay) completed the motion perception tasks. ANOVAs on age and Full-Scale IQ scores revealed no statistical difference across groups (F's < 1).

Dynamic Contrast Task

Participants were presented with gratings in two wedge-shaped windows (0.67 cycles per deg, 5° from center in the widest extent) on the left and right sides of the central fixation cross (0.85° wide; see Fig. 3a for illustration). During a trial, the grating in the left or right hemifield moved at 1°/s towards the left or right directions for 300 ms. On termination of the movement, participants were instructed to press a button to indicate the direction of movement regardless of the hemifield in which movement was presented. Visual fields in which movement occurred and direction of movement were pseudorandomly and randomly assigned respectively to each trial. Target movement was presented only after participants maintained their eye position within two degrees from center fixation for at least 1 sec. Target movement was terminated immediately if the participant moved their eyes from center fixation during the trial. The task resumed after the participant re-fixated on the center target. This was done to ensure that visual stimuli were presented in a similar location in the hemifield of interest across trials. The starting contrast level was 10%. Contrast levels of the gratings were reduced using the two down one up staircase procedure, which yields thresholds to achieve 71% accuracy (Levitt, Reference Levitt1971). Contrast levels were manipulated using 30% increments for the first 4 reversals then reduced to 15% increments thereafter until eight more reversals were observed. The staircase procedure was applied separately to left and right visual field trials to calculate discrimination thresholds for each hemifield separately. The average of the last six reversals for each staircase was used as an index of the perceptual threshold for visual motion discrimination.

Fig. 3. Schematic presentations of a) the Dynamic Contrast Task and b) the Motion Coherence Task used in the current study. Both figures illustrate the right hemifield trial with rightward movement.

Motion Coherence Task

Stimuli for the motion coherence task were presented in an identical manner except for the following. Participants were presented with a group of 100 randomly placed dots (0.15 degrees in diameter) in high contrast inside invisible circles (7.5 degrees diameter) located in each hemifield (Fig. 3b). In each trial, left or rightward movement occurred for 300 msec in one of the hemifields at 3.3 °/s. Each dot was displayed for 100 ms and was replaced with a new dot generated at a random location after the 100 ms period in order to prevent subjects from tracking specific dots. The task started at the 100 coherence level where all dots were moving in the same direction. The number of dots moving in the same direction was reduced using the staircase procedure described above to estimate the discrimination threshold.

Manual Motor Tasks

Finger Tapping Task

Participants placed their index finger on a key and were asked to tap the key as fast as they could. This task from the Halstead-Reitan battery was performed with the dominant and non-dominant hand in blocks of 10 s. A total of five blocks were performed with each hand, and the average number of taps per 10 s was used as a dependent measure.

Grooved Pegboard Task

Participants were presented with a 5 × 5 set of slots that had various orientations and were asked to insert a peg to each slot. Each peg had a ridge on one side, which required participants to rotate the peg to fit the slot. The task was performed with the dominant and non-dominant hand, and the time to complete the task with each hand was measured.

RESULTS

Eye Movement Tasks

Foveofugal Step-Ramp Pursuit Tracking Task

There was a significant Speed × Group interaction in open-loop pursuit gain, F(6,366) = 2.13, p < .05, which stemmed from lower gain at slower target speeds in the autism group with language delay compared to the autism group without language delay (Fig. 4). Neither group with autism was statistically different from the control group. No other statistical difference was identified.

Fig. 4. Pursuit gain during the open-loop stage from the foveofugal step ramp task. Target velocities toward the right are shown in positive numbers and those toward the left are shown in negative numbers along the X-axis. Pursuit gain was defined as a ratio of eye velocity over target velocity. For instance, pursuit gain of.8 indicates that eye velocity was 20% lower than the target velocity.

During closed-loop pursuit on this task, we identified a significant Direction × Group interaction in pursuit gain, F(2,117) = 4.24, p < .05 (Fig. 5). Both the control group and autism group with language delay showed rightward advantage in performance while the autism group without language delay showed more symmetric performance. When the average gain from leftward and rightward trials were compared, the lateral difference was significant in the control group (t(45) = 3.77, p < .001) and autism group with language delay (t(40) = 3.57, p = .001), but not in the autism group without language delay (t < 1). The autism group without language delay had significantly lower gain in rightward trials than the control group, t(118) = 2.42, p < .05. Whereas we did not find differences between the control group and either autism groups during open-loop pursuit, which is sensitive to impairments in visual motion perception, we observed an absence of the typical left/right asymmetry in closed-loop pursuit in the autism group without language delay.

Fig. 5. Pursuit gain during the closed-loop stage from the foveofugal step ramp task.

Pure Ramp Task

There was a trend for the Group × Speed × Direction interaction in pursuit latency (Fig. 6), which is sensitive to disturbances in visual motion perception systems, with the autism group without language delay having longer latency than other groups in most target velocity conditions, F(8,308) =1.94, p = .053. There was also a trend for group differences in average pursuit gain, F(2,118) = 2.87, p = .061, with the autism group without language delay showing poorer performance than the control group (posthoc linear contrast, p < .05) (Fig. 7). There were also differences in the number of catch-up saccades that subjects made in order to keep up with the target, F(2,119) = 3.28, p < .05. Both autism groups made higher numbers of catch-up saccades than the control group (linear contrasts, p's < .05). Though pursuit gain was only marginally lower than the control group, the significantly higher number of catch-up saccades made during this task suggests that both autism groups had difficulty tracking visual targets.

Fig. 6. Pursuit latency during the pure ramp task.

Fig. 7. Average pursuit gain during the pure ramp task.

Visual Motion Perception Tasks

Visual thresholds obtained from the motion tasks were transformed by taking reciprocals to correct for non-linearity of the measures and skewness of the distributions, which is commonly performed in psychophysical studies (e.g., Chen et al., Reference Chen, Palafox, Nakayama, Levy, Matthysse and Holzman1999). After the transformation, Group × Hemifield ANOVAs were used to compare performance across groups and hemifields.

Group differences were detected when the threshold to discriminate directions of visual movement was assessed by varying the contrast of gratings on the Dynamic Contrast task, F(2,92) = 6.26, p < .01 (Fig. 8). Posthoc linear contrasts showed that the autism group with language delay had poorer performance than the control group (p = .001). There was no significant difference between the control group and the autism group without language delay. There was a trend for a Group × Hemifield interaction, F(2,92) = 2.59, p = .081. The marginal interaction was caused by a somewhat more asymmetric performance in the autism group without language delay, with poorer performance in the left than right hemifield (t(21)=2.40, p = .078) while the other two groups showed similar performances across hemifields.

Fig. 8. Performance on the Dynamic Contrast task. Transformed thresholds for left and right hemifield conditions are shown separately. Higher transformed thresholds represent superior sensitivity to discriminate visual motion.

When the threshold to discriminate directions of visual motion was measured by the motion coherence task, there was a trend for a group difference in performance, F(2,95) = 2.58, p = .081 (Fig. 9. The autism group with language delay had poorer performance than the control group (p < .05) whereas there was no difference between the control group and the autism group without language delay. No other statistical differences were found.

Fig. 9. Performance on the Motion Coherence task. Transformed thresholds for left and right hemifield conditions are shown separately. Higher transformed thresholds represent superior sensitivity to discriminate visual motion.

Correlational Analyses

In order to investigate possible relationships between impairments in visual pursuit and visual motion perception, we performed exploratory correlational analyses using a p < .05 threshold. Because the initial open-loop pursuit performance has the highest reliance on sensory information, we used open-loop gain from the foveofugal step ramp task (Fig. 4) and pursuit latency from the pure ramp task (Fig. 6) in these analyses. Significant negative correlations were observed between performance on the Dynamic Contrast task in the left hemifield and pursuit latency in leftward trials (i.e., lower sensitivity to visual motion predicted longer pursuit latency), in the autism group with language delay r(30) = −.44, p < .05 and in the autism group without language delay, r(22) = −.55, p < .01. This effect was not significant in typically developing individuals, and no other correlations were significant.

Correlations between the pursuit measures and manual motor skills were also computed because our previous work with an independent data set (Takarae et al., Reference Takarae, Minshew, Luna, Krisky and Sweeney2004a) demonstrated a significant intercorrelation between oculomotor and manual motor measures in autism. Means and standard deviations of the manual motor measures are reported in Table 1. Both autism groups were slower to complete the Grooved Pegboard task than the control group (F(2,92) = 11.40, p < .001) whereas the number of taps per 10 seconds from the Finger Tapping task was similar between groups. There was no significant Group × Hand interaction on these tasks.

In the correlational analyses, no significant correlations were observed in the control group. In the autism group with language delay, time to complete the Grooved Pegboard task with the dominant hand was correlated with pursuit latency from the pure ramp task in rightward (r(30) = .40, p < .05) and leftward trials, r(30) = .48, p < .01. The time to complete measures with the non-dominant hand was also correlated with pursuit latency in rightward (r(30) = .40, p < .05) and leftward trials, r(30) = .39, p < .05. Additionally, performance on the Finger Tapping task with the non-dominant hand was correlated with open-loop pursuit gain from the foveofugal step ramp task in leftward trials, r(33) = .40, p < .05. In the autism group without language delay, there were no significant correlations between eye movement measures and manual neuropsychological test performance.

DISCUSSION

We examined visual motion perception, visual pursuit, and manual motor skills in high-functioning individuals who met ADOS and ADI criteria for autism. We observed pursuit impairments in both groups with autism though the pattern of impairments was somewhat different between them. Both autism groups had a general problem tracking moving targets as reflected in increased numbers of catch-up saccades and marginally reduced pursuit gain during the pure ramp task. The two autism groups, however, differed in two aspects. First, the autism group without language delay took marginally longer to initiate pursuit eye movement in response to visual motion in the pure ramp task. Increased pursuit latency, which is one of the primary chronic deficits observed after MT lesions (Heide et al., Reference Heide, Kurzidim and Kompf1996; Lisberger & Pavelko, Reference Lisberger and Pavelko1989), suggests that at least some aspects of pursuit impairments in this group might be related to altered sensory input. Second, this group showed abnormal asymmetry in closed-loop gain from the step ramp task in which they lacked the typical rightward bias seen in controls and the group with language delay. Reduced pursuit gain in rightward trials in the autism group without language delay might point to a unique alteration in the development of normal hemispheric specialization or a right hemisphere dysfunction as the pursuit system has an ipsiversive organization. In contrast, the group with language delay did not have increased pursuit latency or lateralized impairments in pursuit gain though they did have general problems in visual pursuit that were correlated with manual motor skills.

Previous studies have shown some inconsistency with regards to reports of pursuit deficits in autism. Scharre and Creedon (Reference Scharre and Creedon1992) and Rosenhall et al. (Reference Rosenhall, Johansson and Gillberg1988) documented difficulty in eliciting pursuit responses in the majority of subjects (29 out of 34 in Scharre and Creedon and 7 out of 11 children in Rosenhall et al.) whereas a more recent study by Kemner et al. (Reference Kemner, van der Geest, Verbaten and van Engeland2004) reported no statistical difference in pursuit accuracy in their subjects with PDD. Findings from these studies were inconclusive because of use of small samples or single measures to characterize subjects' performance. A large sample study by Takarae et al. (Reference Takarae, Minshew, Luna, Krisky and Sweeney2004a) (60 subjects with autism and 94 control subjects) used three different pursuit tasks to characterize multiple aspects of pursuit performance. They have found reduced pursuit gain in three different tasks during the closed-loop stage where pursuit performance is mostly supported by non-sensory factors, primarily predictions about target motion. They also found reduced pursuit gain during the open-loop stage where the performance is primarily driven by sensory information that was specific to trials in which subjects tracked targets to the right. The current study with another largely independent sample also found pursuit impairments in autism. Whereas patterns of impairments are somewhat different across studies, possibly related to variability within the autism population, the two large sample studies suggest that pursuit eye movement impairments are neurophysiological characteristics of autism.

The present study also examined motion perception in autism and identified impairments in this behavioral domain. Although in contrast to pursuit studies where alterations in pursuit initiation and lateral asymmetry were largely restricted to the group without early language delay, motion processing deficits were relatively specific to the group with early language delay. The autism group without a history of early language delay had much milder impairments, with statistically marginal reduction in motion sensitivity only in the left hemifield during the Dynamic Contrast task. Unlike some previous studies (Milne et al., Reference Milne, Swettenham, Hansen, Campbell, Jeffries and Plaisted2002; Spencer et al., Reference Spencer, O'Brien, Riggs, Braddick, Atkinson and Wattam-Bell2000), our study did not find deficits in motion perception using a motion coherence task. This pattern of findings is inconsistent with the view that there is a selective impairment in higher level motion processing in autism, whereas lower level processing is spared (Bertone et al., Reference Bertone, Mottron, Jelenic and Faubert2003). Past findings that used motion coherence tasks have been inconsistently documenting impairments in autism (de Jonge et al., Reference de Jonge, Kemner, de Haan, Coppens, van den Berg and van England2007; Milne et al., Reference Milne, White, Campbell, Swettenham, Hansen and Ramus2006), suggesting heterogeneity in sensitivity to visual motion in autism. Some studies have suggested that impairments in visual motion perception are limited to those with autism, and not found in Asperger's Disorder (Spencer & O'Brien, Reference Spencer and O'Brien2006; Tsermentseli et al., Reference Tsermentseli, O'Brien and Spencerin press). Similar to these findings, though all our subjects met criteria for Autistic Disorder based on DSM-IV criteria, we found more severe impairments in visual motion perception in the autism group with language delay.

These observations, along with different patterns of performance during visual pursuit tasks, suggest that early language delay might be associated with different patterns of brain development in autism spectrum disorders. Our correlational analyses showed relationships between pursuit performance and manual motor skills only in the autism group with language delay, suggesting a more generalized pattern of sensorimotor deficits in this subgroup of participants with autism. In contrast to this more generalized pattern of sensorimotor deficit, the autism group without language delay had abnormal asymmetry in some aspects of visual motion perception and visual pursuit, which point to disturbances in right hemisphere. The inference of selective right hemisphere dysfunction in this subgroup is based on the observed impairments in rightward pursuit and marginal impairments in motion perception in the left hemifield, as sensorimotor control of smooth pursuit is ipsiversively organized while visual sensory systems have contralateral organization (Ilg & Schumann, Reference Ilg and Schumann2007; Shi et al., Reference Shi, Friedman and Bruce1998).

Observations from the current study provide evidence for a neurophysiological differentiation between individuals with autism in their associated visual sensory and sensorimotor impairments based on their history of early language delay. Those with language delay had more severe problems in visual motion perception, and their pursuit impairments were associated with more generalized disturbances in motor or sensorimotor systems as well as sensory processing disturbances. Those without a history of language delay showed abnormal asymmetries in performance during some aspects of pursuit and visual motion perception consistent with right hemisphere dysfunction and did not show correlations between pursuit and manual motor performance. Thus, brain dysmaturation in autism with language delay may be associated with more general alteration of brain development including premotor systems in frontal cortex, while autism without language delay may be associated with alteration of development of more posterior cortical areas, possibly more pronounced in the right hemisphere. The potential clinical relevance and genetic implications for this differentiation remain to be explored in future studies.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

Information that is presented in this manuscript and the manuscript itself has not been published elsewhere either electronically or in print. Authors do not have any financial or other conflicts that could affect this manuscript. This research was funded by an NICHD Collaborative Program of Excellence in Autism (HD35469) and grants MH01433 and NS33355 from the National Institute of Health, and the National Alliance for Autism Research (now Autism Speaks).

References

REFERENCES

Alarcon, M., Cantor, R.M., Liu, J., Gilliam, T.C., & Geschwind, D.H. (2002). Evidence for a language quantitative trait locus on chromosome 7q in multiplex autism families. American Journal of Human Geneics, 70, 6071.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bertone, A., Mottron, L., Jelenic, P., & Faubert, J. (2003). Motion perception in autism: A “complex” issue. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15, 218225.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bradford, Y., Haines, J., Hutcheson, H., Gardiner, M., Braun, T., Sheffield, V., Cassavant, T., Huang, W., Wang, K., Vieland, V., Folstein, S., Santangelo, S., & Piven, J. (2001). Incorporating language phenotypes strengthens evidence of linkage to autism. American Journal of Medical Genetics (Neuropsychiatric Genetics), 105, 539547.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chen, Y., Palafox, G.P., Nakayama, K., Levy, D.L., Matthysse, S., & Holzman, P.S. (1999). Motion perception in schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 56, 149154.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Jonge, M.V., Kemner, C., de Haan, E.H., Coppens, J.E., van den Berg, T.J., & van England, H. (2007). Visual information processing in high-functioning individuals with autism spectrum disorders and their parents. Neuropsychology, 21, 6573.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dursteler, M.R. & Wurtz, R.H. (1988). Pursuit and optokinetic deficits following chemical lesions of cortical areas MT and MST. Journal of Neurophysiology, 60, 940965.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldberg, M.C., Lasker, A.G., Zee, D.S., Garth, E., Tien, A., & Landa, R.J. (2002). Deficits in the initiation of eye movements in the absence of a visual target in adolescents with high functioning autism. Neuropsychologia, 40, 20392049.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heide, W., Kurzidim, K., & Kompf, D. (1996). Deficits of smooth pursuit eye movements after frontal and parietal lesions. Brain, 119, 19511969.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ilg, U.J. (1997). Slow eye movements. Progress in Neurobiology, 53, 293329.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ilg, U.J. & Schumann, S. (2007). Primate area MST-l is involved in the generation of goal-directed eye and hand movements. Journal of Neurophysiology, 97, 761771.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson, M.H. (1990). Cortical maturation and the development of visual attention in early infancy. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 2, 8195.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kemner, C., van der Geest, J.N., Verbaten, M.N., & van Engeland, H. (2004). In search of neurophysiological markers of pervasive developmental disorders: Smooth pursuit eye movements? Journal of Neural Transmission, 111, 16171626.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levitt, H. (1971). Transformed up-down methods in psychoacoustics. Journal of the Acoustic Society of America, 49, 467477CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lisberger, S.G. & Pavelko, T.A. (1989). Topographic and directional organization of visual motion inputs for the initiation of horizontal and vertical smooth-pursuit eye movements in monkeys. Journal of Neurophysiology, 61, 173185.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lord, C., Risi, S., Lambrecht, L., Cook, E.H. Jr., Leventhal, B.L., DiLavore, P.C., Pickles, A., & Rutter, M. (2000). The autism diagnostic observation schedule-generic: A standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30, 205223.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lord, C., Rutter, M., Goode, S., Heemsbergen, J., Jordan, H., Mawhood, L., & Schopler, E. (1989). Autism diagnostic observation schedule: A standardized observation of communicative and social behavior. Journal of Autism & Developmental Disorders, 19, 185212.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lord, C., Rutter, M., & Le Couteur, A. (1994). Autism diagnostic interview-revised: A revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism & Developmental Disorders, 24, 659685.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luna, B., Doll, S.K., Hegedus, S.J., Minshew, N.J., & Sweeney, J.A. (2007). Maturation of executive function in autism. Biological Psychiatry, 61, 474481.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lynch, J.C. (1987). Frontal eye field lesions in monkeys disrupt visual pursuit. Experimental Brain Research, 68, 437441.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Milne, E., Swettenham, J., Hansen, P., Campbell, R., Jeffries, H., & Plaisted, K. (2002). High motion coherence thresholds in children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 43, 255263.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Milne, E., White, S., Campbell, R., Swettenham, J., Hansen, P., & Ramus, F. (2006). Motion and form coherence detection in autistic spectrum disorder: Relationship to motor control and 2:4 digit ratio. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36, 225237.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Minshew, N.J., Goldstein, G., & Siegel, D.J. (1997). Neuropsychologic functioning in autism: Profile of a complex information processing disorder. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 3, 303316.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Minshew, N.J., Sung, K., Jones, B.L., & Furman, J.M. (2004). Underdevelopment of the postural control system in autism. Neurology, 63, 20562061.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Molloy, C.A., Dietrich, K.N., & Bhattacharya, A. (2003). Postural stability in children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 33, 643652.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mostofsky, S.H., Goldberg, M.C., Landa, R.J., & Denckla, M.B. (2000). Evidence for a deficit in procedural learning in children and adolescents with autism: Implications for cerebellar contribution. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 6, 752759.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Müller, R.A., Cauich, C., Rubio, M.A., Mizuno, A., & Courchesne, E. (2004). Abnormal activity patterns in premotor cortex during sequence learning in autistic patients. Biological Psychiatry, 56, 323332.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Newsome, W.T., Wurtz, R.H., Dursteler, M.R., & Mikami, A. (1985). Deficits in visual motion processing following ibotenic acid lesions of the middle temporal visual area of the macaque monkey. Journal of Neuroscience, 5, 825840.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Redcay, E. (2008). The superior temporal sulcus performs a common function for social and speech perception: Implications for the emergence of autism. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Review, 32, 123142.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rinehart, N.J., Bradshaw, J.L., Brereton, A.V., & Tonge, B.J. (2001). Movement preparation in high-functioning autism and Asperger disorder: A serial choice reaction time task involving motor reprogramming. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31, 7988.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rivaud, S., Muri, R.M., Gaymard, B., Vermersch, A.I., & Pierrot-Deseilligny, C. (1994). Eye movement disorders after frontal eye field lesions in humans. Experimental Brain Research, 102, 110120.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosenhall, U., Johansson, E., & Gillberg, C. (1988). Oculomotor findings in autistic children. Journal of Laryngology and Otology, 102, 435439.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scharre, J.E. & Creedon, M.P. (1992). Assessment of visual function in autistic children. Optometry and Vision Science, 69, 433439.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shi, D., Friedman, H.R., & Bruce, C.J. (1998). Deficits in smooth-pursuit eye movements after muscimol inactivation within the primate's frontal eye field. Journal of Neurophysiology, 80, 458464.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spencer, J., O'Brien, J., Riggs, K., Braddick, O., Atkinson, J., & Wattam-Bell, J. (2000). Motion processing in autism: Evidence for a dorsal stream deficiency. Neuroreport, 11, 27652767.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spencer, J.V. & O'Brien, J.M. (2006). Visual form-processing deficits in autism. Perception, 35, 10471055.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tager-Flusberg, H. & Joseph, R.M. (2003). Identifying neurocognitive phenotypes in autism. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society London B: Biological Sciences, 358, 303314.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Takarae, Y., Minshew, N.J., Luna, B., Krisky, C.M., & Sweeney, J.A. (2004a). Pursuit eye movement deficits in autism. Brain, 127, 25842594.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Takarae, Y., Minshew, N.J., Luna, B., & Sweeney, J.A. (2004b). Oculomotor abnormalities parallel cerebellar histopathology in autism. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 75, 13591361.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tsermentseli, S., O'Brien, J.M., & Spencer, J.V. (in press). Comparison of Form and Motion Coherence Processing in Autistic Spectrum Disorders and Dyslexia. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders.Google Scholar
Williams, D.L., Goldstein, G., & Minshew, N.J. (2006). Neuropsychologic functioning in children with autism: Further evidence for disordered complex information-processing. Child Neuropsychology, 12, 279298.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zilbovicius, M., Meresse, I., Chabane, N., Brunelle, F., Samson, Y., & Boddaert, N. (2006). Autism, the superior temporal sulcus and social perception. Trends in Neuroscience, 29, 359366.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Demographic and neuropsychological characteristics of participants

Figure 1

Fig. 1. Schematic presentation of a foveofugal step ramp task. The diagram shows a time (horizontal axis) by target position (vertical axis) plot for a single trial of this task. The dashed line represents target movement with an initial 3° step followed by smooth movement towards the left (negative) 15-degree position. The line above illustrates a typical eye movement response in which an initial catch-up saccade (dotted line) to the target is followed by smooth pursuit.

Figure 2

Fig. 2. Schematic presentation of a pure ramp task. The diagram shows a time (horizontal axis) by target position (vertical axis) plot for a trial with a target moving from center to a left (negative) 15-degree position. The dashed line shows target movement. The line above shows a trial where smooth pursuit is sometimes interrupted by a catch-up saccade that corrects for tracking error (shown in a dotted line).

Figure 3

Fig. 3. Schematic presentations of a) the Dynamic Contrast Task and b) the Motion Coherence Task used in the current study. Both figures illustrate the right hemifield trial with rightward movement.

Figure 4

Fig. 4. Pursuit gain during the open-loop stage from the foveofugal step ramp task. Target velocities toward the right are shown in positive numbers and those toward the left are shown in negative numbers along the X-axis. Pursuit gain was defined as a ratio of eye velocity over target velocity. For instance, pursuit gain of.8 indicates that eye velocity was 20% lower than the target velocity.

Figure 5

Fig. 5. Pursuit gain during the closed-loop stage from the foveofugal step ramp task.

Figure 6

Fig. 6. Pursuit latency during the pure ramp task.

Figure 7

Fig. 7. Average pursuit gain during the pure ramp task.

Figure 8

Fig. 8. Performance on the Dynamic Contrast task. Transformed thresholds for left and right hemifield conditions are shown separately. Higher transformed thresholds represent superior sensitivity to discriminate visual motion.

Figure 9

Fig. 9. Performance on the Motion Coherence task. Transformed thresholds for left and right hemifield conditions are shown separately. Higher transformed thresholds represent superior sensitivity to discriminate visual motion.