Police make contact with nearly 44 million Americans annually in the United States (Reference Hyland, Langton and DavisHyland et al. 2015). While the overall rate of contact remained stable from 2002 to 2011, urban residents around the country experienced a substantial increase in investigative police stops, known as stop-and-frisks. In New York City specifically, the number of stop-and-frisks increased threefold from 2003 to 2009 and were disproportionately concentrated among racial and ethnic minorities (Reference MearesMeares 2014). Indeed, black NYC residents are approximately 2.5 times more likely to be stopped than white residents, net of germane factors including neighborhood context and crime rates (Reference Gelman, Fagan and KissGelman et al. 2007). Yet beyond the act of being stopped, less is known about whether inequality exists in terms of what happens once individuals are stopped.
As the state's legitimized form of physical coercion over citizens, racial disparities in police use of force are perhaps one of the most extreme examples of racial inequality. This is, in part, why accusations of racial bias in police use of force have been and continue to be a common focal point of civil unrest in the United States. From the 1960s in cities such as Detroit, Philadelphia, and Chicago, to the recent protests that coalesced under the #BlackLivesMatter moniker in response to the deaths of young black victims such as Michael Brown in Ferguson, Freddie Gray in Baltimore, and Laquan McDonald in Chicago, and others, accusations of unjustified use of force against black victims persist. Indeed, recent investigations by the Department of Justice into those cities found evidence of civil rights violations by police, as did investigations into Albuquerque, Cleveland, and Seattle police among others. Protesters assert that the well-documented racial inequalities in the likelihood of being stopped are exacerbated by policing bias in the likelihood that force is used during stops, and that the bias is particularly harmful for black youth. Public discourse focuses on civilian behavior during police encounters, with many suggesting that black people are more likely to be doing something wrong at the time than white people, thus precipitating police use of force. In this scenario, civilian behavior, not racial bias, is thought to drive police use of force. Despite these competing explanations for police use of force, no systematic research testing these propositions exists.
The dearth of research on this topic is in part due to data limitations; however, this has recently begun to change. Since the 1990s, data collection by police has become increasingly common, but agencies only began disseminating data in the last few years. The New York Police Department (NYPD) was among the first to publicly release detailed data on investigatory stops as part of a legal settlement (Daniels et al. v. City of New York 1999). Subsequent analyses of this data helped convince a federal judge to declare NYPD's stop-and-frisk policy unconstitutional due to racial bias (Floyd v. City of New York 2013). Even before that ruling, New York began to roll back the use of stop-and-frisk. Stops dropped from a high of 685,724 in 2011 to under 50,000 in 2014. While critics considered NYPD's shift away from stop-and-frisk and the court's ruling to be monumental victories, it is unknown whether this dramatic drop reduced racial inequality in police violence during stops.
We fill these voids in existing research, focusing specifically on New York City. We test the claim, re-energized by the Black Lives Matter movement, that black civilians, especially black youth, are more likely to be subject to physical force during a police encounter than white civilians, after adjusting for other factors related to police use of force. We also examine whether or not black individuals are more likely to experience police violence during stops that end in arrest and/or the recovery of contraband or a weapon than whites, as criminal behavior is a common alternative explanation for high profile instances of police use of force against black civilians. Additionally, we assess whether recent NYPD reforms to the use of investigatory stops as a policing practice and changes in officer training affect any observed inequalities in police use of force.
New York City is a compelling research setting because it is widely viewed as a model for proactive policing. In response to a crime wave in the early 1990s, the NYPD implemented an especially visible aggressive stop-and-frisk policy which was then expanded into the next decade. Moreover, there is little reason to expect New York to be an outlier in the broader pattern of police use of force in the United States. In fact, given that the NYPD was subject to some of the strongest early contemporary critiques of racial discrimination after the police shooting of Amadou Diallo in 1999, which the City settled via lawsuit, and that New York was under judicial oversight for racial disparities in stop-and-frisk during our observation period, the city may represent a conservative test of racial disparities in police use of force. On the other hand, New York was also the site of several large Black Lives Matter protests after Eric Garner's death and the subsequent acquittal of Officer Pantaleo. Regardless, a recent report analyzing police use of force in a multicity sample found similar patterns across jurisdictions ranging in size, demographics, and region. Although cities differ in the degree of racial inequality in police use of force, there is a general pattern of racial inequality across localities (Reference GoffGoff et al. 2016).
Prior Research on Police Stops and Use of Force
As the legal rulings regarding stop-and-frisk focused on its efficacy in finding weapons and preventing crime, so too has most of the research (for a review, see Reference MearesMeares 2014). However, the original case involved police using force against an individual (Terry) who had not yet performed a criminal act (he was preparing to commit armed robbery when stopped). Reference StuntzStuntz (1998) argues that the Supreme Court and researchers mistakenly focus on the legitimacy of the search and not when police use force against individuals. Stuntz's critique remains apt today, particularly because foundational scholars conceptualize police capacity for use of force as the defining characteristic of police work (Reference BittnerBittner 1970).
Although deadly police encounters are rare compared to police use of force in general, public interest may have steered researchers toward the most severe form of force. In the broadest sense, research finds that communities with greater racial inequality have higher rates of lethal violence by police (Reference Jacobs and O'BrienJacobs and O'Brien 1998, but for a recent exception, see Reference KlingerKlinger et al. 2016). Although recently, in part due to research showing that structural factors explain only a portion of police behavior and the rise in public awareness of specific incidents of police use of force, researchers shifted focus to police use of lethal force at the individual level.
These latest studies, which adapt data from the Bureau of Justice Statistics and/or crowdsourced data, show racial inequalities in who is killed by police (e.g., Guardian n.d.; mappingpoliceviolence.org, n.d.). For example, one study finds that unarmed black people are 3.5 times more likely to be shot by police than unarmed whites (Reference RossRoss 2015). Such research is useful for determining the scope of the issue, but conclusions are limited. For example, without a comparison group such as nonfatal encounters, scholars cannot identify how these fatal shootings differ from other police encounters. Moreover, as these data do not contain many details of the encounter, it is impossible to ascertain whether black individuals are more likely to be shot because of racial bias or due to some other reason. Indeed, there are a variety of competing explanations for why racial disparities may exist other than racial bias. While data limitations have hampered empirical work, scholars have long been theorizing about why observed disparities in police use of force might occur, highlighting a series of behavioral, contextual, and organizational factors (Reference Engel and CalnonEngel and Calnon 2004; Reference FriedrichFriedrich 1980; Reference Geller, Toch, Geller and TochGeller and Toch 1996; Reference Smith, Reiss and TonrySmith 1986; Reference Smith and AlpertSmith and Alpert 2007).
A recent study that adjusted for a range of these factors did not find racial disparities in police shootings in Houston, but did find disparities in police use of force in general (Reference FryerFryer 2016). However, Fryer compared police shootings to police encounters involving arrests in which police could have been legally justified in shooting (e.g., a person resisting arrest) and research suggests that these arrests are racialized; officers are more likely to perceive people of color as verbally abusive or noncompliant than whites (Reference Geller and FaganGeller and Fagan 2010). As a result, Fryer's analytic strategy likely obscured any racial disparity in police shootings.
Unfortunately, data limitations such as those described above are not new, rather, studying police use of force has always been challenging. Because of this, most of the literature debates how to measure it and achieve representative estimates of use of force (see Reference Garner, Maxwell and HerauxGarner et al. 2002; Reference Geller, Toch, Geller and TochGeller and Toch 1996). For instance, most early work relied on trained observers accompanying officers on their shifts and recording any use of force, which is susceptible to bias (e.g., Reference Terrill and MastrofskiTerrill and Mastrofski 2002). Similarly, other work relies on surveys of individuals in custody (Reference GarnerGarner et al. 1995, Reference Garner, Maxwell and Heraux2002). Additional research using surveys of the public or data collected by the police also lack enough information to rule out competing explanations (Reference Engel and CalnonEngel and Calnon 2004; Reference GoffGoff et al. 2016). Also, aside from crowdsourced studies, most research to date relies on data collected in the 1990s. Taken together, these limitations may explain why previous research on racial disparities in police use of force has historically been mixed (Reference GoffGoff et al. 2016; Reference Sun and PayneSun and Payne 2004; Reference Terrill and MastrofskiTerrell and Mastrofski 2002). The current study builds on previous work by (1) using contemporary data to assess disparities in police use of force, (2) using a comparison group, and (3) adjusting for competing explanations that prior research has not adequately considered, such as civilian behavior. We detail these explanations next.
Contextual Differences
A common rationale for racially unequal rates of police use of force is that black residents live in neighborhoods with higher poverty rates than white residents (Reference Smith, Reiss and TonrySmith 1986). Given that the risk of police violence is higher in disadvantaged neighborhoods (Reference Terrill and ReisigTerrill and Reisig 2003), black people may be more likely to experience police violence than white people because of where they live. Here, structural factors should explain any racial disparities in police use of force. In short, the combination of racial segregation and poverty concentration may explain any racial inequality in police shootings (Reference Massey and DentonMassey and Denton 1993). These race and class differences can also affect organizational strategies, such as whether or not to aggressively use stop-and-frisk as a policing practice, which could lead to higher or lower use of force during stops. Recent research finds that context did not explain the racial disparity, but rather moderated it. Consistent with racial threat theory, the racial disparity in police use of force is greatest in segregated precincts (Reference LevchakLevchak 2017).
Behavioral and Situational Differences
Racial disparities in crime rates are perhaps the default explanation for any observed inequalities in policing (Reference GoffGoff et al. 2016; Reference MacDonaldMacDonald 2011). For example, if police shootings occur at random during police interactions and black individuals are three times more likely to interact with police than white individuals because of a higher rate of involvement in violent crime (Reference Sampson and LauritsenSampson and Lauritsen 1997), we would expect black individuals to be three times as likely to be shot as whites. In this case, observed racial disparities in police shootings would be due to differential involvement in crime. As evidence, recent work shows that violent crime rates broken down by race decreases but does not eliminate the racial disparity in police use of force (Reference GoffGoff et al. 2016).
Furthermore, civilian behavior during the stop may also explain racial differences in police use of force. One way civilian behavior may influence this is whether the stop is productive or not. A productive stop is one that results in an arrest or finds a weapon or other contraband (i.e., drugs). Because civilians involved in criminal activity may resist the stop in the hopes of avoiding sanctions, productive stops may require police use of force more so than unproductive stops. In other words, racial differences in the stopped person's involvement in crime may explain any observed racial disparities. For instance, if black people are more likely to be arrested during investigatory stops, then accounting for the stop outcome might explain disparities in police use of force. On the other hand, the “hit rate” for finding weapons or drugs during a stop is lower for black than white civilians in NYC, thus, adjusting for the “success” of the stop could exacerbate any observed racial disparity in police use of force (Reference Gelman, Fagan and KissGelman et al. 2007). Unfortunately, data limitations prevent researchers from ascertaining the sequencing of police locating criminal activity and police use of force; police might exert force after finding a weapon or, use force and then discover contraband.
Other scholars emphasize different forms of civilian behavior (Reference DurnaDurna 2011; Reference FriedrichFriedrich 1980). This line of work suggests that any observed racial disparity in police use of force is due to the way black civilians behave compared to white civilians during a police encounter. Put differently, some research finds that black civilians are more hostile and noncompliant toward officers and such behaviors increase the likelihood of police use of force (Reference EngelEngel 2003; Reference Garner, Maxwell and HerauxGarner et al. 2002). Similarly, officers are more likely to use force when a civilian is suspected of a violent crime, and given racial disparities in violent crime, black civilians may be more likely to be suspected of such than white ones (Reference Worden, Geller and TochWorden 1996). In this case, unlike “hit rates,” police are more likely to report that black civilians were suspected of violent crime or noncompliant when stopped, thus accounting for these behaviors may decrease any observed racial disparity. Moreover, because these behaviors are racialized—officers employ racialized “scripts” for these kinds of behaviors and as a result are more likely to perceive black civilians as, for instance, noncompliant—adjusting for these behaviors will produce conservative estimates of any police violence disparities (Reference Geller and FaganGeller and Fagan 2010; Reference MuhammadMuhammad 2010). Like with the outcome of the stop, ascertaining the timing of civilian behavior and police violence is difficult and a common civilian complaint is that police induce arrest as a post hoc justification for their use of force.
Discrimination-Based Theories
In contrast to behavioral and contextual theories, others argue that any observed disparities in police use of force are the result of racial bias—that police perceive black civilians differently than whites. If after accounting for the theoretically and empirically important factors discussed above, we find racial disparities in police use of force, then racial bias may be present. Racial disparities in police use of force are produced at either the individual level or via spatial differences in policing practices. Because we adjust our analyses for the fact that policing strategies differ across neighborhoods and control for clustering at the precinct level, only individual (cop) bias remains (Reference Tomaskovic-Devey, Mason and ZingraffTomaskovic-Devey et al. 2004; Reference WarrenWarren et al. 2006).
Officer bias can be explicit, implicit, or both. Often referred to as the “bad apples” argument, it is unlikely that enough officers are overtly biased to produce systematic patterns in police violence alone (Reference WarrenWarren et al. 2006). Yet recent scandals involving the exchange of racist texts and memes among police from areas ranging from San Francisco to Ferguson to Fort Lauderdale suggest prejudice continues to thrive, and data on over 28,000 civilian complaints in Chicago show that a small percentage of officers are responsible for the majority of civilian complaints (Reference ArthurArthur 2015; Reference HernandezHernandez 2015; Reference RoblesRobles 2015). Similarly, analyses of Florida traffic stop ticketing patterns indicate that approximately 25 percent of Florida highway patrol officers are biased against black and Hispanic drivers (Reference Goncalves and MelloGoncalves and Mello 2017). Such explicitly biased policing may explain why black residents consistently report more negative police encounters than whites (Reference AndersonAnderson 1990; Reference BrunsonBrunson 2007; Reference Carr, Napolitano and KeatingCarr et al. 2007; Reference Weitzer and TuchWeitzer and Tuch 2004), and some early work suggests that black people are more likely to experience police use of force than white people (Reference Terrill and MastrofskiTerrill and Mastrofski 2002; Reference Worden, Geller and TochWorden 1996).
Racial bias, however, need not be explicit for policing to lead to racially disparate experiences. Viewing the behavior of black individuals as suspicious compared to similarly behaving white individuals is an example of implicit bias and may lead to more frequent violence against black people than white people. That is, an officer need not be actively aware that their perceptions of civilians vary by race. Indeed, research suggests that implicit biases affect officer performance (Reference CorrellCorrell et al. 2007; Reference Greenwald and KriegerGreenwald and Krieger 2006; Reference Plant and PeruchePlant and Peruche 2005). As stereotypes of black individuals are often specifically about physical threats to personal safety (Reference Quillian and PagerQuillian and Pager 2010), implicit bias may lead to unequal use of force. Implicit bias may also cause police to interpret behavior in racialized ways, as discussed above. Thus, using police reports of civilian behavior to adjust for bias in police use of force may underestimate any racial discrimination. Yet even using officer reports of police use of force in a quasi-experiment, Reference LegewieLegewie (2016) shows racial bias in New York City policing. Police violence increased against black civilians after black suspects fatally shot police officers, but no such increase occurred after white or Hispanic suspects fatally shot police.
Despite elevated public discourse on racial bias, particularly implicit bias, as well as continued national debate concerning the credibility of Black Lives Matter (BLM) and the subsequent counter groups All Lives Matter and Blue Lives Matter, few contemporary studies systematically examine whether police violence varies by race, net of other key factors that influence racial inequalities. Because police may perceive black civilians as more dangerous than white civilians, our first hypothesis is:
H1. Black individuals are more likely to be subjected to force, and specifically potential lethal force, during a police interaction than white individuals net of relevant factors including neighborhood characteristics, crime rates, civilian behavior, and the stop outcome.
While many of the civilian deaths that have sparked national conversation were during stops in which the individual had done nothing wrong, others were after police reported feeling afraid or had begun the process of arresting the individual. For example, Eric Garner was placed in a chokehold for illegally selling individual cigarettes and Tamir Rice was confronted by police who had been told he had a gun. As a result, public discourse has focused on whether the individual's behavior precipitated police violence, and that, in line with prior research on stop outcomes, once individual behavior is accounted for, any racial disparities should disappear. Despite this, scholarship consistently finds that black people receive harsher treatments than white people throughout the criminal justice system (Reference Morenoff, Tienda and RutterMorenoff 2005; Reference Ousey and LeeOusey and Lee 2008; Reference Steffensmeier, Ulmer and KramerSteffensmeier et al. 1998). Given this research and that officers interpret black civilian behavior differently than white behavior, officer reactions may be amplified when black individuals are violating the law (Reference Geller and FaganGeller and Fagan 2010). Therefore, rather than explaining any racial disparity, discrimination theories suggest that black individuals may be even more likely to experience force than whites when they are suspected of breaking the law.
Specifically, a black civilian violating the law may trigger stereotypes of the athletic and aggressive black man or the criminal “Black brute” for officers (Reference MillerMiller 1998). For example, Darren Wilson described Michael Brown as “Hulk Hogan like” to justify his use of deadly force and Betty Shelby's defense for shooting Terence Crutcher was her fear that he was heading toward his car to get a gun. Furthermore, research shows that such stereotypes persist; black people are perceived as more aggressive and physically formidable than white people (Reference Holbrook, Fessler and NavarreteHolbrook et al. 2016; Reference Wilson, Hugenberg and RuleWilson et al. 2017). Stereotypes of black people as more dangerous than white people, lead us to hypothesize that police will perceive any evidence of criminal activity as more threatening when stopping black civilians than white ones. Thus, our second hypothesis is:
H2. Any observed racial disparity in police use of force will be larger during stops when contraband is found or the stop resulted in an arrest, net of neighborhood characteristics, crime rates, and civilian behavior.
BLM activists, however, not only articulate accusations of broad racial inequality in police use of force, they also make more intersectional claims about how and why that inequality emerges. In particular, BLM protestors argue that police use of force is especially harmful for black youth and that a fear of police is learned very early in black communities. Although spurred by the death of 18-year-old Michael Brown, the BLM movement first emerged after 17-year-old Treyvon Martin's death in 2012. The centrality of age in this conflict is further illustrated in the viral videos of police violence against teens such as the viral video of a McKinney, TX, police officer kneeling on the back of a 14-year-old black child in a bikini at a pool party or the surveillance video of the shooting of 12-year-old Tamir Rice in a Cleveland park.
Extant research provides some support for this argument. Black students are more likely than white students to be suspended or expelled for otherwise equivalent disciplinary reasons in schools (Reference Gregory and WeinsteinGregory and Weinstein 2008; Reference Welch and PayneWelch and Payne 2010). This may, in part, be explained by racialized views of youth, as black boys are seen as older than their peers in photographs (Reference GoffGoff et al. 2014) and Americans prefer harsher punishments for juvenile behaviors when the perpetrator is black instead of white (Reference Pickett and ChiricosPickett and Chiricos 2012; Reference RattanRattan et al. 2012). While rarely a focus of research on police use of force, early work suggests that age influences police use of force (Reference ReissReiss 1968; Reference Terrill and MastrofskiTerrill and Mastrofski 2002). Indeed, recent scholarship calls for policing research to focus explicitly on kids, as legal socialization and legitimacy attitudes develop early in life (Reference Tyler and TrinknerTyler and Trinkner 2017). Taken together, if police have biases against black youth—for example, the officers who killed Tamir Rice claimed he looked older than his age—then the magnitude of the relationship between age and police use of force should be greater for black youth than white youth. If police systematically share those perceptions of black youth as older and more worthy of punishment than white youth, this will lead to police using more force against black youth than white peers. Thus, our third hypothesis, is:
H3. Any observed racial disparity in experiencing police force will be greatest for stops of teenagers and will decline with age.
Proactive Policing and Reforms in New York City
As a policing policy, stop-and-frisk dates back to Terry v. Ohio (1968), the Supreme Court case in which police were given the ability to detain and frisk an individual for whom they had “reasonable suspicion” has been, is, or will be engaged in a criminal activity and may be armed and “presently dangerous.” In the late 1990s, the NYPD began employing this proactive strategy in response to an unprecedented crime wave. At its peak, from 2006 to 2012, the NYPD instigated over half a million stops every year, or one stop per minute for 6 years.
As a result of hundreds of citizens, primarily people of color, being stopped daily and low and unequal rates of finding illegal weapons during those stops, a federal 2013 ruling found that New York's aggressive use of stop-and-frisk was racially discriminatory. Earlier in 2013, NYPD also voluntarily shifted away from stop-and-frisk as a policing tactic. Overall, stops in the city dropped from averaging over half a million stops a year to under 50,000 in 2014. In addition to dramatically reducing the number of stops and increasing awareness surrounding racial disparities in stops, the NYPD also revised its training to promote more civil and less confrontational behavior during stops. This provides a unique opportunity to test whether or not organizational policy changes can reduce any observed racial disparities in police use of force.
On the one hand, decreasing stop-and-frisk and revising officer training may reduce any observed disparities. For instance, research on the “driving while Black” phenomenon shows that racial disparities in traffic stops dropped after states passed laws prohibiting the use of racial profiling (Reference Tomaskovic-Devey and WarrenTomaskovic-Devey and Warren 2009). Similarly, Reference FyfeFyfe (1982) found that police use of lethal force decreased after administrative policies were enacted in the 1970s.
Meanwhile, other work suggests that reforms cannot change unconscious bias by individual actors without significant de-escalation/racial bias training (Reference CorrellCorrell et al. 2002). Although the NYPD did implement new training protocol specifically concerning stops, this training did not address racial bias. Instead, it focused on nonconfrontational techniques. Indeed, accusations of racial bias by the NYPD have continued since the training tactics change. For example, Eric Garner's death came after the policy change and was caused by a chokehold that violated NYPD policies.
In sum, while the NYPD dramatically reduced the use of investigatory stops and revised its training, this does not necessarily imply that racial disparities in use of force declined as a result. In light of the NYPD's changes and existing work showing that police organizational reform can reduce observed racial disparities, our fourth hypothesis is:
H4. In the wake of Floyd v. City of New York, NYPD's reduction in stops, and changes in officer training, any observed racial inequality in police use of force decreased beginning in 2013.
Analytic Strategy
We employ a series of strategies to test our four hypotheses concerning the relationships between race, civilian behavior, age, and police use of force and whether reforms have impacted any observed disparities. To assess hypothesis 1 that observed racial disparities are not explained by contextual or behavioral explanations, we first estimate the likelihood of experiencing police violence during NYPD investigative stops while adjusting for those explanations.Footnote 1 Since racial tensions often center on whether the victim was involved in criminal activity at the time and racial stereotypes may be especially pertinent in these circumstances, we next divide our sample into two groups to test hypothesis 2: (1) productive stops, during which officers arrested the civilian and/or seized a weapon or other contraband, and (2) stops that concluded without arrest or seizures. This directly tests whether or not behavioral differences—specifically, post hoc identifications of criminal behavior—explain or amplify any observed racial disparities. Furthermore, examining disparities in analyses restricted to stops in which police did not report any criminal activity avoids any potential sequencing issues between civilian behavior and police use of force.
Next, we report marginal effects to test our age hypothesis that black youth are more likely to experience police violence than white youth (hypothesis 3). Then, to test whether recent organizational changes to investigatory stops reduced inequalities in police use of force (hypothesis 4), we estimate separate pre-reform models (2007–2012) and post-reform models (2013–2014). As our data are from police self-reports and any justifications for the use of force are reported only after force was used, our estimates of inequalities in police violence are conservative and should be viewed as a baseline.
Data
The NYPD began collecting data on stops more than 20 years ago; however, it was not until the class action lawsuit settlement of Daniels et al. v. City of New York in 1999 that the department agreed to make the data publicly available. NYPD officers are trained to fill out a form (a UF-250) for any civilian interaction in which an officer has “reasonable suspicion” that a person is involved in criminal activity, regardless of the outcome of that stop. We analyze all stops of black or white civilians recorded between 2007 and 2014 with valid information, producing a sample of over 2 million stops.Footnote 2 UF-250s include a variety of details about the stop such as the location, use of force, and the behaviors and characteristics of the stopped individual.Footnote 3 To incorporate police precinct and census data, we geocode recorded addresses and merge them with publicly available maps of NYC's 77 precincts and census tracts created as part of a WNYC (NYC public radio) project on the location of low-level marijuana arrests. We then include census level demographics from the 2010 census and the 2008–2012 American Community Survey (ACS) 5-year sample. Table 1 shows descriptive statistics for all variables included in the analysis.
Our dependent variables include two measures of police use of force. Consistent with prior research (Reference LegewieLegewie 2016; Terrill and Mastrofski 2002), we first examine whether or not a stop includes any use of force, which includes a range of physical force from “hands on suspect” to “pointing firearm at suspect” (1 = any force). Roughly 1/5th of all stops include some form of physical force, most commonly placing hands on suspect, handcuffing them, or putting the suspect against the wall/car. Although this is a standard measure and the NYPD specifically defines these actions as force, the process of arresting a civilian may, by definition, include force, which complicates temporal ordering (see Kaminski et al. 2015). We address this in two ways. First, as described above, we split the sample by whether or not the stop was productive or not (i.e., resulted in arrest/found contraband). If we find similar racial disparities in stops during which police did not find any criminal activity or materials (before or after using force) and in stops during which police do find such evidence, this would demonstrate that arrests are not driving racial disparities in use of force. We also examine a much narrower form of force—drawing and/or pointing a service weapon—which is not a part of arrest protocol.
As police officers are trained only to pull their firearm if they are prepared to shoot an individual and firearms represent the only lethal force option on the UF-250 form, we also model a subset of our first dependent variable in which an officer pulls their firearm and/or points the firearm at a suspect as a separate outcome (1 = gun drawn or pointed). This potentially lethal form of force represents 0.5 percent of all recorded stops and roughly 90 percent of the time in which an officer uses any available weapon listed on the UF-250 form (gun, pepper spray, and baton).Footnote 4 , Footnote 5 As with any force, we also split the sample by the stop outcome when estimating disparities in potential lethal force.
Our focal independent variables include indicators for race and age. In our data, police used force in approximately 22.3 percent of stops involving a black individual compared to 16.4 percent of stops with whites. Importantly, because our sample includes all stops reported, this disparity (and all of our results) is net of the substantial racial inequality in the likelihood of being stopped by police (Reference ChauhanChauhan et al. 2015; Reference Epp, Maynard-Moody and Haider-MarkelEpp et al. 2014). To facilitate understanding of the age-use of force dynamic, we divide age into nine categories ranging from ages 10 to 14—there are more than 36,000 stops of black youth aged 10–14 in our sample alone—to age 50 plus, which serves as the reference group. This allows us to capture any nonlinear associations between age, race, and police use of force and focus on teenage experiences (hypothesis 3).Footnote 6 Overall, more black teenagers experienced force during a stop than whites as a whole in our sample. We also adjust for gender as more than 90 percent of stops were of men (male = 1) and height, which is measured in inches.Footnote 7
To estimate inequalities in police violence, we incorporate an extensive set of controls including civilian behavior, stop characteristics, crime rates, policing strategies, and neighborhood characteristics. A key independent variable is the outcome of the stop—specifically whether the stop was successful in (1) finding a weapon (which could explain the police use of force during the stop), (2) finding other contraband (primarily drugs), or (3) resulted in an arrest (Reference GarnerGarner et al. 1995). These categories are not mutually exclusive; for instance, a person might be arrested because police find contraband during a stop. In later models, we split the sample by the outcome of the stop. We include three measures of civilian behavior during the stop: officer reports that the stopped individual (1) made verbal threats, (2) refused to comply with orders, and (3) was suspected of a violent crime (Reference Smith, Reiss and TonrySmith 1986; Reference Terrill and MastrofskiTerrill and Mastrofski 2002; Reference Worden, Geller and TochWorden 1996). Like with the stop outcome, these measures are reported after the stop concludes. We also adjust for three additional stop characteristics that prior research suggests are related to police use of force (Reference Grogger and RidgewayGrogger and Ridgeway 2006; Reference LevchakLevchak 2017): the length of time that the officer observed the civilian before making the stop (0–60 minutes), whether or not the stop occurred at night (1 = yes), and the stop duration (0–60 minutes).Footnote 8
Beyond individual or stop characteristics, we adjust for precinct level felony and misdemeanor arrest rates, because the NYPD often modifies policing strategies based on local crime rates. For example, areas that experienced crime spikes saw an increase in police officers as part of the NYPD's COMPSTAT program (i.e., hot spot policing). We use current year precinct arrest rates as NYPD policing was responsive, generally, to contemporary crime (2-week periods) and not to crime rates in prior years.Footnote 9
Additionally, we include indicators of precinct stop rates, as previous research on racial inequalities in stop rates found that precincts varied in their stop rates, but not necessarily in the racial inequalities in who is stopped (Reference Gelman, Fagan and KissGelman et al. 2007). To do so, we identified precincts in which the stop rate was more than one s.d. higher than the average precinct as “high-stop” areas (n = 9) and precincts with at least 0.7 s.d. fewer stops as “low-stop” precincts (n = 10; none reached the one s.d. cutoff in that direction). Since the use of investigative stops varies over time, we also include indicators for year in our analyses (results not shown, available upon request).
Drawing on the neighborhood effects literature and building on existing use of force research, we also adjust for a variety of neighborhood characteristics including measures of population, population density, racial composition of the area, male median age, poverty, education, vacancy, and unemployment (Reference LevchakLevchak 2017; Reference Smith and HolmesSmith and Holmes 2014).
Results
First, we estimate the likelihood of stops involving any police use of force as well as potential lethal force across our entire sample population using logistic regression with robust standard errors to adjust for clustering by precinct.Footnote 10 The results, which appear in Table 2, show that black civilians have 27 percent higher odds of experiencing force during a stop than white civilians and 28 percent higher odds of officers drawing their guns (OR = 1.27 and 1.28, respectively). Importantly, these findings are net of alternative explanations including civilian behavior, the success of the stop, local crime rates, and neighborhood context. This supports hypothesis 1 that black individuals are more likely to experience force, including potential lethal force, at the hands of police than white individuals.
Notes: *p < .05; **p < .01; ***p < .001.
Additionally, the results in Table 2 provide preliminary support for the notion that younger civilians are more likely to experience both any force and potential lethal force than older civilians. The results also indicate that demographic characteristics beyond race and age are associated with police violence. Gender, in particular has a strong relationship with police use of force; men have 64 percent higher odds of experiencing force than women (OR = 1.64). However, model 2 shows that men are no more likely than women to have guns drawn on them during stops. Because officers are trained to draw their weapons when they are prepared to shoot and kill, the results suggest that stereotypes regarding traditional femininity that may protect women from, and during, police encounters, disappear when police perceive a heightened threat.
Turning to police justifications for force, the results also show that civilian behavior and context matter. The odds of police using force are more than twice as high for stops of civilians suspected of a violent crime (OR = 2.39) and nearly quintuple for police pulling their guns (OR = 4.94). Civilian noncompliance with orders is also strongly associated with both police using force and drawing a gun. Stops during which civilians verbally threatened officers also have higher odds of officers using force including drawing a gun, although to a lesser extent than the other reported behaviors.
Not surprisingly, the outcome of the stop is also strongly associated with usage of force. Stops that end in arrest are associated with an increase in the odds of police using force by 217 percent (OR = 3.17) and the odds that police draw their weapons by 200 percent (OR = 3.00). Understandably, stops in which police find weapons are more likely to involve the use of force and finding a weapon is strongly related to police drawing their service weapons. Contraband, in comparison, has a weaker association with the odds of police using force and was not related to police drawing their weapons.
The results also indicate that local context is inconsistently related to police use of force. In particular, precinct stop rates are not associated with police use of force with the exception of stops in high stop rate precincts and potential lethal force; such stops are less likely to involve police drawing their guns. It may be that extensive practice of officers performing stops and civilians being stopped de-escalates the risk of potential lethal force in high stop precincts. Alternatively, the aggressive use of investigatory stops may primarily increase the number of low-risk stops, reducing the overall likelihood of police drawing their weapons. Stops in precincts with higher rates of misdemeanor arrests, on the other hand, are more likely to include force; for every 10 additional misdemeanor arrests in a precinct, the odds of a stop including force increases by 2 percent (OR = 1.002).
At the census tract level, context continues to matter for some factors more than others. Although the magnitude of these associations may seem small, these covariates vary from 0 to 100. Thus a 10 percent increase in percent black or Latino residents is associated with a 4.2 percent increase in the odds of police drawing their gun. This is consistent with prior research (Reference LevchakLevchak 2017; Reference Smith and HolmesSmith and Holmes 2014), but there is no relationship with use of any force. In contrast, neighborhood poverty is associated with lower odds of police drawing their guns. However, the vacancy rate is associated with higher rates of police use of force, which demonstrates that the neighborhood context beyond the racial demographics of the area is associated with police use force. In sum, and in line with previous research and hypothesis 1, although the context of the stop is associated with the likelihood of police using force during a stop, a substantial racial disparity persists net of measures of neighborhood and precinct context.Footnote 11
Civilian Behavior and Police Use of Force
To more rigorously test the claim that racial differences in criminal behavior may explain racial disparities in police use of force, examine variation in stop type, and assess the robustness of our findings, we next split our sample by whether or not the stop was a “hit” in finding an arrestable offense, weapon, or other contraband. This tests hypothesis 2 that the racial disparity in the use of force will grow when police uncover criminal activity. Table 3 reports these results. Model 1 shows the results for whether police used any force in stops that did not result in an arrest or weapon or other contraband found. As over 90 percent of stops in our data fit this criterion, it is not surprising that these results are nearly identical to those in Table 2.
Notes: *p < .05; **p < .01; p < .001.
Standard errors adjusted for clustering at the precinct level.
Controls not shown include length of time observed prior to stop, stop duration, low stop precinct, high stop precinct, felony arrest rate, misdemeanor arrest rate, census tract population, population density, % black/Hispanic, male median age, poverty rate, education level, vacancy rate, unemployment rate, and year.
Still, it is striking that when police do not report any criminal behavior by stopped civilians, black individuals experience 29 percent higher odds of experiencing force than white individuals. Turning to gun use in model 2, the results are again consistent with those in Table 2, albeit the magnitude of the race relationship is slightly smaller (OR = 1.20 compared to 1.28 in Table 2). Nonetheless, together these results clearly demonstrate that the racial disparity persists even when individuals are not arrested nor found with contraband. Concerning age, the results show that, like with Table 2, young civilians are more likely to experience any police violence than older persons when no laws are broken, but this does not extend to potential lethal force. However, we caution that such comparisons may be biased by residual heterogeneity between models and not due to substantive differences (Reference MoodMood 2010). Thus, we graph marginal effects to show the substantive association among race, age, the outcome of the stop, and police use of force shortly.
Before showing the marginal effects, we turn to models 3 and 4 that restrict the sample to stops resulting in arrest or seizure of weapons or other contraband. These are relatively rare—only 145,807 of the 2,085,775 stops were considered productive by this measure. Nevertheless, there is evidence supporting hypothesis 2. Although the racial disparity is smaller for any force when police arrest individuals or find contraband and/or weapons compared to when they are innocent (model 3 to 1), the disparity grows monumentally for potential lethal force from 20 to 60 percent higher odds (model 2 to 4). As further evidence that the race of the civilian is directly related to potential lethal force and not a proxy for behavioral differences, the odds ratios for civilian behavior decrease. Most dramatically, being suspected of a violent crime is far less predictive of having a gun drawn for “successful” stops, dropping from 5.57 to 3.61 between models 2 and 4.
Table 3 also shows that the magnitude of the age association decreased for any force between innocent and productive stops but increased considerably across models for stops where police pull their guns. That is, age appears more consequential for potential lethal force when police uncover criminal activity. Regarding gender, men are more likely to experience force regardless of the stop outcome, except for potential lethal force when police find no criminal activity. In this case, men are actually less likely to experience potential lethal force than women. This may be due to how police record stops; each individual stopped gets their own UF-250 report. In the data, women are more likely to be stopped in the company of others and it may be that the behavior of these other people affects how police treat the whole group (Reference Stuart and BenezraStuart and Benezra 2017). In sum, the results in Table 3 reveal that although race matters across stop outcomes, race is an especially powerful predictor of potential lethal force when police uncover criminal behavior, which supports hypothesis 2. The age associations are less consistent but like with race, are strongest for police drawing their guns during productive stops.
Methodologists, however, caution that comparisons across samples may be invalid for logistic regression coefficients (Reference LongLong 2009; Reference MoodMood 2010). Additionally, we have yet to examine how the intersection of race and age influences police violence. Comparing the marginal effect of a variable of interest across samples is a more robust method for testing differences across samples, which we do in Figures 1 and 2. Specifically, based off the models in Table 3, these figures display the marginal effect of race at every age in our sample by whether the stop was productive or not, testing both hypotheses 2 and 3.
The results in Figure 1 show support for the BLM claim that race is associated with police use of force even after considering alternative explanations such as civilian behavior, criminal involvement, and contextual factors (hypothesis 1). Counter to hypothesis 2 that the racial disparity in use of force should be larger during stops that result in arrest or the seizure of contraband or weapons, the marginal effect of race does not differ by whether or not the stop was productive. For both productive and nonproductive stops, black civilians are more than three percentage points more likely to be subjected to force during a stop than white civilians. In other words, the racial disparity in use of force is not related to racial differences in the “hit rate” of stops—in fact, black civilians are less likely to be arrested or be found with contraband or weapons when stopped than whites. Overall, this marginal effect indicates that more instances of force against black civilians are unexplained by contextual or behavioral differences than instances of force against white civilians.
Additionally, there are no age differences for either stop outcome. That is, Figure 1 does not support hypothesis 3; young black civilians are no more likely to experience police violence than white youth. Instead, black civilians are more likely to experience police use of force than whites regardless of their age. Next, we turn to the marginal effect results for police use of potential lethal force.
Figure 2 shows the marginal effect of race on police drawing and/or pointing their guns at the stopped individual. Here, we find strong evidence of hypothesis 2 that the racial disparity in police escalation of force is greater for productive stops. In other words, and consistent with research on other criminal justice outcomes such as arrests and sentencing (Reference Ousey and LeeOusey and Lee 2008; Reference Steffensmeier, Ulmer and KramerSteffensmeier et al. 1998), black civilians are treated more harshly than otherwise similar white civilians when doing something wrong. Where the marginal effect is below 0.1 percentage points for stops in which the civilian was not arrested and no contraband or weapons were seized, it is greater than 0.4 percentage points at every age for stops that resulted in arrests and/or seizures. These differences are statistically significant. In short, black civilians are more likely to experience potential lethal force when no criminal behavior is uncovered and the likelihood increases at least four fold when officers locate criminal activity.
Furthermore, although age is not related to the marginal effect of race for stops where no criminal behavior is found, age is strongly associated with the marginal effect of race for stops that do result in arrest or seizures. Teens between the ages of 15 and 19 years experienced the largest racial disparity of 0.82 percentage points which falls to 0.43 for stops of black civilians over the age of 50. Thus, the age relationship is more nuanced than our original hypothesis. The racial disparity in police use of force is blind to age except when police arrest or seize contraband from young black individuals and is limited to police drawing their firearms.
Although the magnitude of these marginal effects may seem small, this is at least in part because this sample comprises investigatory stops that did not rise to the level of probable cause; hence, there are relatively few instances of police drawing or pointing their guns. Yet the marginal effect of race is not only significantly different from zero at all ages for both categories of stops, but also substantively meaningful because of the absolute number of stops performed by police. To demonstrate, we calculate a counterfactual: how many fewer stops of black civilians would include force if black civilians were treated the same as white civilians at the hands of police?
To calculate our counterfactual, we multiply the number of stops of black civilians in each age category in our data by the marginal effect for each age category. If black civilians were treated identically to whites, we estimate that 61,207 fewer stops of black civilians (95 percent CI ±22,793) would have included force and 1,814 fewer stops of black civilians (95 percent CI ±533) would have involved police gun use. In comparison, fewer than 50,000 total white civilians were subjected to police force during a stop during our observation period and fewer than 1,200 had guns drawn on them. Put differently, there would have been 21 (±7.8) fewer uses of force on black civilians per day and 4.4 (±1.3) fewer guns drawn on black civilians per week over our 8 years observation period.
Organizational Reforms and Police Use of Force
The results discussed above show that during investigatory stops, almost two dozen additional black civilians were subject to police violence every day and more than four had guns drawn on them, compared to white civilians’ likelihoods of experiencing force during a stop. Yet those numbers are for our entire observation period, 2007–2014, and NYPD implemented reforms which reduced the number of stops from over half a million every year before 2013 to just over 50,000 in 2014. Although the reforms also included trainings designed to promote professionalism during stops and reduce the reliance on physical force, these trainings were not specifically geared toward reducing the racial disparity in police use of force. Therefore, while the frequency of stops declined, it remains unknown if the racial disparity in police violence also decreased thanks to those organizational reforms.
Table 4 presents the results testing our hypothesis that organizational strategies reduce racial disparities in officer use of force. All covariates are included but not shown—full tables are available upon request. Here, we compare logistic regressions before and after NYPD substantially reduced its use of stop-and-frisk policing from over 1,800 stops per day to 130 stops per day on average. In short, we find mixed evidence that organizational reforms reduced disparities in police use of force. On the one hand, models 1 and 2 in Table 4 show that black civilians had 26 percent higher odds of experiencing any force during a stop compared to whites before reform and 37 percent higher odds afterward. This increase is not statistically significant when we examine the difference in the marginal effect of race pre and post reform, but the results clearly suggest that the racial disparity did not decrease after organizational reform as hypothesized. The pattern of results concerning age is similar to race; age inequalities persist after reforms and the magnitude for the majority of the age associations increased somewhat post reforms.
Notes: *p < .05; **p < .01; ***p < .001.
Standard errors adjusted for clustering at the precinct level.
Controls not shown include length of time observed prior to stop, night stop, stop duration, low stop precinct, high stop precinct, felony arrest rate, misdemeanor arrest rate, census tract population, population density, % black/Hispanic, male median age, poverty rate, education level, vacancy rate, unemployment rate, and year.
On the other hand, the results for potential lethal use of force, which appear in models 3 and 4 in Table 4, provide preliminary evidence of progress in racial inequality. Before the practice was ruled unconstitutional and before police training changed, black civilians had 29 percent higher odds of having police draw or point their firearms at them compared to white civilians. After these changes, black civilians are no longer more likely to experience potential lethal force than white individuals. Similar to our results for any force, the change in the marginal effect of race between pre and post reform is not significantly different. However, this might be due to sample size as only 870 stops of black civilians included potential lethal force post reform compared to 136 stops of whites post reform.
While the racial disparity decreased, the overall rate of police gun use increased from 0.4 percent of stops pre reform to 0.7 percent of stops after reform. This increase is a logical result of a reform that reduced the rate of stops, as individuals aroused greater suspicion from police to precipitate the stop after reforms (although we did not see the same association for any use of force that may be due to training reforms to de-escalate confrontation during stops). In other words, because innocent civilians were less likely to be stopped after reform, the gun rate increased.
Turning to the age results in models 3 and 4, we see that stops of individuals aged 20–44 were more likely to include potential lethal force than individuals aged 50 and older from 2007 to 2012. As with race, these associations no longer exist post reforms, but the decrease is not statistically significant and this may be due to sample size. Taken together, the results in Table 4 reveal that both age and racial inequalities appear to have decreased some post reforms, but this only applies to the most serious form of force (drawing a firearm) and the change is not statistically significant.
Despite these mixed findings, our data indicate that these reforms fundamentally altered police use of force through the dramatic drop in the number of stops in which police used force. Furthermore, approximately 22.3 percent of stops before 2013 included force compared to 18.4 percent of stops after 2013. Because black people were disproportionately stopped, black people benefitted from the reforms more so than white people. Indeed, we estimate that every year after reform, 51,017 black and 6,410 fewer white civilians were subjected to police violence because of those reforms. Taken together, the overall effect of court mandated and NYPD initiated reforms is substantially fewer instances of racially disparate uses of force or guns being drawn during a postreform year than in a typical prereform year, even though the marginal effect of race did not significantly change in our models.
Discussion
One hundred years ago, the National Association for the Advancement of Colored People protests against lynching, police misconduct, and legalized violence against Black Americans demanded social change. In the 1960s, rumors of police misconduct were behind many of the riots and rebellions in cities across the country. Most recently, the deaths of black citizens during encounters with police returned police brutality and racial inequality to national attention and the growth of the BLM movement.
However, as a part of the criminal justice system, policing has not been the focus of social science research as much as the study of inequality via mass incarceration (Reference Wakefield and UggenWakefield and Uggen 2010). Yet, policing inequalities are at the root of legal cynicism that helps perpetuate a cycle of violence and distrust of government in communities (Reference Carr, Napolitano and KeatingCarr et al. 2007; Reference Desmond, Papachristos and KirkDesmond et al. 2016; Reference Tyler, Fagan and GellerTyler et al. 2014). For example, after Chicago police shot and killed her son and neighbor, Janet Cooksey was not only angry at the police, but also at her son's father because she thought he should have called her instead of 911. In her words, “[police] are supposed to serve and protect us and yet they take the lives” (Reference CoatesCoates 2015). Similarly, Betty Jones’ (the neighbor who was shot) brother already doubted any official investigation's legitimacy, saying “we all know how that will turn out.” Strikingly, he made this statement just days before a grand jury decided not to indict Cleveland police officers for Tamir Rice's death on the recommendation of the county prosecutor.
To date, however, there have been few systematic analyses of whether or not there is racial inequality in police use of force today. Moreover, no research exists to our knowledge on how the productivity of a police stop, or age, is associated with the racial disparity in police use of force. Using NYPD's UF-250 forms, precinct, and census data, we find that BLM claims are largely borne out by the data, even after controlling for the outcome of the stop, other civilian behavior, and neighborhood and precinct context. Specifically, police treat black individuals more harshly than whites (hypothesis 1). Additionally, the racial disparity in police drawing their weapons is higher for stops that resulted in arrest and/or seizure of contraband or weapons, suggesting that police perceive black individuals engaging in criminal behavior as more threatening than white individuals in similar circumstances (hypothesis 2). Yet this is limited to potential lethal force; whether a stop was productive or not had no effect on the racial disparity in police use of force more broadly. Furthermore, the racial disparity in gun use during productive stops was greatest for black youth and young adults—when young black civilians are found in violation of the law during stops, they are more likely to experience potentially lethal force than young white civilians as well as older Black and White civilians (hypothesis 3). There were no age inequities by race among stops that did not include or uncover any illicit behavior.
In sum, black individuals are not only more likely to be stopped by police, they are also more likely to be subjected to police use of force during that interaction, and more likely to be seen as threatening to officers, resulting in a greater rate of police drawing their weapons. This finding is notable for its consistency across our models: black civilians who were found to be engaging in criminal behavior were more likely to be subjected to force, as were black civilians who were not engaging in such behavior. That was true across all age groups and true before and after organizational reform (the only exception is potential lethal force post reform, although that is driven by a small sample of whites who experienced that form of violence after 2013). Those findings are not only in the same direction, but also the magnitude of the racial disparity is consistent (with the exception that black youth who were arrested or found with contraband were more likely to experience potential lethal force).
Although the marginal effects appear small in magnitude, across the more than 1.5 million stops of black civilians during the 8 years’ time period, these racial disparities resulted in approximately 61,000 more cases of police force against black civilians and 1,800 more cases of police drawing their weapons against black civilians than would have occurred if black civilians were treated identically to whites. Those numbers are greater than the total number of whites who were subjected to force or weapons being drawn on them in our sample.
Concerning inequalities more broadly, our findings indicate that police perceptions of race are a powerful form of inequality in police violence, over and above age. Although the youthfulness of black civilians subjected to police violence has been highlighted by the media and BLM advocates, our analyses show that age is only a factor in the likelihood of experiencing potential lethal force and even then, race continues to trump age. That is, police treat consistently treat civilians differently by skin tone more so than age.
Our findings not only show the potential benefits but also the limitations of organizational reform efforts to reduce racial disparities. Unfortunately, reforms to the regularity of stop-and-frisk and training did not significantly reduce the racial disparity in the likelihood of experiencing police violence as hypothesis 4 had predicted, nor inequities in age. This is consistent with research arguing that in-depth training on racial bias is necessary to reduce disparities, which studies have shown reduces police implicit bias (Reference CorrellCorrell et al. 2002, Reference Correll2007; Reference Greenwald and KriegerGreenwald and Krieger 2006; Reference Plant and PeruchePlant and Peruche 2005). Yet our findings also reveal that reforms have been beneficial; when the NYPD reduced the use of stop-and-frisk and instituted changes in training, approximately 57,000 fewer stops, the majority of which targeted black people, included force by police annually.
This study moves our understanding of inequalities in criminal justice forward, but our results do come with limitations. First, our data come from NYC, a global city with a highly visible and aggressive stop-and-frisk policy. However, other research using multicity samples found similar patterns, indicating that our findings are not exceptions to the general pattern of police use of force (Reference GoffGoff et al. 2016). Additionally, so as to have a proper comparison group of otherwise similar interactions, these results are only of investigative stops and not, for example, exhaustive of the types of death that sparked the contemporary social movement, specifically when police are called to a scene (e.g., Tamir Rice and Laquan McDonald) or as part of a traffic stop (e.g., Walter Scott and Philandro Castile). Nonetheless, the results do show racial inequity in how police respond to reasonable suspicion and provide evidence that a person's race, age, and behavior are associated with differential risks of police violence. These findings are in line with laboratory research that young black civilians are seen as physically larger and more threatening (Reference Wilson, Hugenberg and RuleWilson et al. 2017).
At the same time, our analyses cannot determine the time ordering of civilian behavior and police use of force, such as whether the arrest or seizures are what lead to heightened sense of risk by police or are instead post hoc justifications for force. Thus, examining the timing of police use of force and civilian behavior is a fruitful avenue for future research. Additionally, while we focused on race and age in this article, our results suggest that gender is also key to understanding police–civilian dynamics. Thus, future research should explore on how police perceptions of gender affect this racialized disparity, especially in light of the Say Her Name movement. Finally, although we adjust for a wide variety of competing explanations and relevant factors, our methods cannot prove discriminatory practices by the NYPD (for quasi-experimental evidence of discriminatory patterns by NYPD, see Reference LegewieLegewie 2016). This is another important direction for future research.
In addition to testing racial disparities in policing using other methods and data that could more directly test for bias, future research should explore the consequences of experiencing police violence. Across the country, police killed roughly 1,000 people in 2015, sparking nationwide protests and costing cities millions in settlements (Reference Elinson and DanElinson and Frosch 2015; Reference KindyKindy et al. 2015). Nonetheless, the greatest result of police violence may be an unwillingness to see or treat the police as agents of justice and safety but instead as potential threats to one's health and well-being (Reference Desmond, Papachristos and KirkDesmond et al. 2016; Reference Tyler, Fagan and GellerTyler et al. 2014). Importantly, our analyses indicate that ending stop-and-frisk lowered the rate of police interactions, but not necessarily the racial inequality in the use of force during those police interactions. Future research should explore how to reduce the disparities in experiencing police violence that exist above and beyond the disparities in being stopped.
Perhaps not surprisingly, recent Pew surveys show a large racial gap in trust in the police and hope for future improvement in police–community relations (Reference DrakeDrake 2015). BLM articulates specific reasons for that gap in police trust around police violence against black people, specifically teens. Research indicates that the experience of a police interaction is critical to understanding civilian satisfaction with police (Reference SkoganSkogan 2005; Reference Weitzer and TuchWeitzer and Tuch 2005) but had not measured how unequal those police interactions are. That experience is racially unequal even when adjusting for the outcomes of a stop; contact with police is fundamentally different for Black and White residents. That difference in initial contact may be an underappreciated source of inequality in criminal justice outcomes and subsequent inequalities, including mental health, employment, and education (Reference Geller, Jeffrey, Tom and LinkGeller et al. 2014; Reference HarknettHarknett 2015). Although the majority of police interactions for individuals of all races do not involve violence, the perception that police violence disproportionately targets black civilians has greatly harmed police–community relations. Unfortunately, that perception is reality.