Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-dk4vv Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T06:13:22.606Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Lack of crucial information exacerbates barriers to mitigating human–elephant conflicts in rural Kenya

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  09 September 2024

Lynn Von Hagen*
Affiliation:
College of Forestry, Wildlife and Environment, Auburn University, Auburn, Alabama, USA
Bruce A. Schulte
Affiliation:
Western Kentucky University, Bowling Green, Kentucky, USA
Todd D. Steury
Affiliation:
College of Forestry, Wildlife and Environment, Auburn University, Auburn, Alabama, USA
Kelly Dunning
Affiliation:
College of Forestry, Wildlife and Environment, Auburn University, Auburn, Alabama, USA Current address: Haub School of Environment and Natural Resources, University of Wyoming, Laramie, Wyoming, USA
Mwangi Githiru
Affiliation:
Wildlife Works, Nairobi, Kenya
Sarah Zohdy
Affiliation:
College of Forestry, Wildlife and Environment, Auburn University, Auburn, Alabama, USA
Christopher A. Lepczyk
Affiliation:
College of Forestry, Wildlife and Environment, Auburn University, Auburn, Alabama, USA
*
*Corresponding author, lvonhagen@comcast.net
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Crop foraging by African savannah elephants Loxodonta africana negatively affects farmer livelihoods and support for conservation, yet affordable, sustainable and practical solutions remain elusive. To inform conservation priorities, our goal was to assess the hitherto little explored relationships between farmers’ views on agricultural damage and the socio-economic factors limiting their use of elephant deterrents. We tested our hypotheses associated with the demographic categories of age, education level, years spent farming, gender, exposure to information on deterrent methods, farm size, village and relevant combinations of these factors by surveying 206 respondents across six villages in rural Kenya and analysing the resulting data using an information theoretic approach. Respondents were almost four times more likely to use deterrents if exposed to the relevant information, and almost five times more likely to do so if they had secondary education as opposed to none. Farmers with a higher level of education were five times more likely to have received information on deterrents compared to those with no formal education. Participants who had not received information on deterrents were almost three times more likely to believe that they could implement deterrent methods. Respondents who stated that they could not implement deterrents overwhelmingly cited a lack of financial resources as the reason. Overall, we found that crucial information on reducing elephant crop foraging is not reaching the relevant stakeholders, and socio-economic factors such as education and exposure to information appear to limit uptake of protective measures. These insights are important for developing mitigation strategies and supporting the livelihoods of people affected by negative human–elephant interactions, and thus for effective elephant conservation. Our findings also have broader applications for practitioners seeking to understand barriers stakeholders face in their efforts to mitigate negative interactions with wildlife.

Type
Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of Fauna & Flora International

Introduction

Interactions between people and wildlife are increasing globally as a result of continued habitat loss and conversion of natural areas to agriculture (Young et al., Reference Young, Marzano, White, McCracken, Redpath and Carss2010; Redpath et al., Reference Redpath, Bhatia and Young2015; König et al., Reference König, Kiffner, Kramer-Schadt, Fürst, Keuling and Ford2020), leading to competition between wildlife and people over resources, including habitable space (Madden, Reference Madden2004; Seoraj-Pillai & Pillay, Reference Seoraj-Pillai and Pillay2017). Negative interactions or conflicts are particularly frequent in regions where rural communities live near protected areas or important wildlife features such as movement corridors (Western et al., Reference Western, Waithaka and Kamanga2015; Mc Guinness, Reference Mc Guinness2016; Pozo et al., Reference Pozo, LeFlore, Duthie, Bunnefeld, Jones and Minderman2020). One of the most common types of negative interaction involves agricultural damage (Hill, Reference Hill2000; Naughton-Treves & Treves, Reference Naughton-Treves, Treves, Woodroffe, Thirgood and Rabinowitz2005; McKee et al., Reference McKee, Shwiff and Anderson2021), whereby wildlife enters cultivated lands and consumes or damages crops as part of modified foraging strategies (Owen-Smith et al., Reference Owen-Smith, Fryxell and Merrill2010). Often termed crop raiding or crop foraging, this behaviour has been observed in a wide range of species (Krijger et al., Reference Krijger, Belmain, Singleton, Groot Koerkamp and Meerburg2017; Seoraj-Pillai & Pillay, Reference Seoraj-Pillai and Pillay2017; Gross et al., Reference Gross, Lahkar, Subedi, Nyirenda, Lichtenfeld and Jakoby2018; Hill, Reference Hill2018). However, in areas where wild elephants range, they are generally perceived as the primary cause of crop damage (Sukumar, Reference Sukumar1990; Osborn, Reference Osborn2004; Mackenzie & Ahabyona, Reference Mackenzie and Ahabyona2012).

The negative impacts of elephants and other wildlife can threaten the food security of affected communities and weaken support for conservation (Raphela & Pillay, Reference Raphela and Pillay2021; Salerno et al., Reference Salerno, Stevens, Gaughan, Hilton, Bailey and Bowles2021). Crop foraging incidents not only result in loss of livelihood for farmers but also affect human health and well-being as farmers may experience lost opportunity costs, fear and stress whilst protecting their farms (Barua et al., Reference Barua, Bhagwat and Jadhav2013). Negative human–elephant interactions are also a threat to conservation as some farmers retaliate against wildlife perceived as pests (Distefano, Reference Distefano2005; Treves et al., Reference Treves, Wallace and White2009; Davies et al., Reference Davies, Wilson, Hazarika, Chakrabarty, Das, Hodgson and Zimmermann2011). Farmers repeatedly experiencing crop foraging may become frustrated with wildlife authorities if they feel that the importance of their livelihoods is not being considered, potentially reducing their tolerance for coexisting with wildlife (Naughton-Treves & Treves, Reference Naughton-Treves, Treves, Woodroffe, Thirgood and Rabinowitz2005; Kansky & Knight, Reference Kansky and Knight2014).

Traditional methods to deter elephants include patrolling, guarding (Killion et al., Reference Killion, Ramirez and Carter2020), lighting fires, erecting fences constructed from locally available materials (Kassilly et al., Reference Kassilly, Tsingalia and Gossow2008; Osipova et al., Reference Osipova, Okello, Njumbi, Ngene, Western, Hayward and Balkenhol2018) and using devices to scare elephants (Gilsdorf et al., Reference Gilsdorf, Hygnstrom and VerCauteren2002). However, these often achieve only limited success as elephants can habituate to sounds or the presence of humans, and learn to overcome fences (Hoare, Reference Hoare2012; Mumby & Plotnik, Reference Mumby and Plotnik2018; Gross, Reference Gross2019). Information on deterrent methods is often provided by NGOs or wildlife agencies, which usually operate with limited funding and resources (Folke et al., Reference Folke, Hahn, Olsson and Norberg2005; Noga et al., Reference Noga, Security, Kolawole, Thakadu and Masunga2016; Galvin et al., Reference Galvin, Beeton and Luizza2018). Additionally, such traditional approaches often require human presence to deter elephants, which can be dangerous for farmers.

Modern deterrents can be more difficult to implement because the required materials are often not available locally and/or are expensive to purchase (Vogel et al., Reference Vogel, Songhurst, McCulloch and Stronza2022). However, methods using electric fencing, chili (Parker & Osborn, Reference Parker and Osborn2006; Hedges & Gunaryadi, Reference Hedges and Gunaryadi2010; Chang'a et al., Reference Chang'a, de Souza, Muya, Keyyu, Mwakatobe and Malugu2016), beehives (King, Reference King2010), solar lights (Adams et al., Reference Adams, Mwezi and Jordan2020) and metal fencing (Von Hagen et al., Reference Von Hagen, Kasaine, Githiru, Amakobe, Mutwiwa and Schulte2021) may achieve greater efficacy as they involve recurring negative stimuli. Furthermore, such methods require no human presence when elephants are present, making them safer and freeing up farmers to fulfil their other duties. Successful designs for deterrents consider elephant physiology, behaviour and cognition (Mumby & Plotnik, Reference Mumby and Plotnik2018), and the socio-economic limitations of farmers, to create mitigation techniques that are also resistant to elephant habituation (Naughton-Treves & Treves, Reference Naughton-Treves, Treves, Woodroffe, Thirgood and Rabinowitz2005; Dickman, Reference Dickman2010; Schulte, Reference Schulte, Schulte, Goodwin and Ferkin2016).

The attitudes and behaviours of farmers towards elephants, conservation initiatives and the uptake and use of elephant deterrents have been examined in several regions in Africa (Graham & Ochieng, Reference Graham and Ochieng2008; Noga et al., Reference Noga, Kolawole, Thakadu and Masunga2015; Vogel et al., Reference Vogel, Songhurst, McCulloch and Stronza2022). However, the socio-economic factors that determine whether or not farmers decide to implement deterrents have not been fully explored. Ageing tends to make people more risk averse (Okun & Siegler, Reference Okun and Siegler1976; Dohmen et al., Reference Dohmen, Falk, Huffman, Sunde, Schupp and Wagner2011), yet decision-making is often affected by local culture and can be highly variable (Rieger & Mata, Reference Rieger and Mata2015). Thus, older farmers may be hesitant to try new techniques or crop types but have often accumulated significant local knowledge of elephant movements and behaviours (Buchholtz et al., Reference Buchholtz, Fitzgerald, Songhurst, McCulloch and Stronza2020). Education levels are highly variable amongst rural farmers (Noga et al., Reference Noga, Kolawole, Thakadu and Masunga2015), and higher education levels can positively affect farmer productivity and the adoption of new farming techniques (Oduro-Ofori, Reference Oduro-Ofori2014). Understanding the factors that affect farming decisions is important to improve elephant conservation and management efforts, and provide benefits to farmers.

Given the gaps in knowledge about farmer decision-making regarding deterrent implementation, the threats to livelihoods of rural farmers in Kenya and the need to conserve threatened African elephants, our goal was to examine the relationships between socio-economic factors and crop damage by elephants. We developed three a priori hypotheses: Firstly, we hypothesized that age, education, exposure to information on deterrents and farm size would be positively associated with deterrent usage by farmers and that most would use traditional methods (deterrent-use hypothesis). Secondly, we hypothesized that most rural farming households had not been exposed to information on mitigating the impacts of crop foraging but that, amongst those who had, education would be positively correlated with receipt of any such information, particularly on fencing (deterrent-exposure hypothesis). Thirdly, we hypothesized that most farmers who believed that they could not implement deterrents (even if they had the relevant knowledge) would be constrained by limited economic resources and that only education level would be positively correlated with farmers who believed that they could implement deterrents (economic-barriers hypothesis). The evaluation of these hypotheses will inform conservation planning strategies for agencies aiming to improve food security for farmers whilst conserving elephants.

Study area

The Kasigau Wildlife Corridor lies between Tsavo East and West National Parks in south-eastern Kenya (Fig. 1) and forms part of the Greater Tsavo Ecosystem. African savannah elephants Loxodonta africana are frequent crop foragers in this area, as are other herbivores such as the eland Tragelaphus oryx, and they are a source of conflict between community members and wildlife officials (Litoroh et al., Reference Litoroh, Omondi, Kock and Amin2012; Githiru et al., Reference Githiru, Mutwiwa, Kasaine and Schulte2017). The region is home to the largest elephant population in Kenya, consisting of c. 15,000 individuals (Waweru et al., Reference Waweru, Omondi, Ngene, Mukeka, Wanyonyi and Ngoru2021), and many elephants use the Corridor to move between the two national parks (Omondi et al., Reference Omondi, Bitok, Mukeka, Mayienda, R. and Litoroh2008; Ngene et al., Reference Ngene, Lala, Nzisa, Kimitei, Mukeka and Kiambi2017). Rukinga Wildlife Sanctuary (Rukinga) is one of the community ranches in the Corridor and is operated by Wildlife Works. Villagers in these areas are mostly subsistence farmers, and their income is c. KSH 1,500 (USD 15) per person per month. In years with lower crop yields 39% of the population drops below this income level. The area is characterized by a biannual rainfall pattern of rainy and dry seasons, and has been suffering from ongoing periods of drought (Kasaine & Githiru, Reference Kasaine and Githiru2016).

Fig. 1 The Kasigau Wildlife Corridor in Kenya, shown with its 14 community ranches and the locations of the six study villages.

We selected villages surrounding the Sanctuary to test our hypotheses, focusing on those that shared a boundary with the Sanctuary (where interactions occur often), were within 1 h drive of the centralized base in Rukinga (for ease of access) and comprised a majority of farming households experiencing frequent elephant interactions. Because of logistical and budgetary constraints we limited our selection to six villages: Itinyi and Kombomboro (combined because of population size and proximity, hereafter referred to as Itinyi), Bungule, Miasenyi, Kisimenyi, Buguta and Mwakwasinyi (Fig. 1).

Methods

We developed a questionnaire (Supplementary Material 1) with our local partners based on previous research with farmers experiencing elephant crop foraging (Hoffmeier-Karimi & Schulte, Reference Hoffmeier-Karimi and Schulte2015), taking into account local knowledge and customs, and people's experience with and knowledge of encounters with elephants. The survey contained 64 questions, both semi-structured and open-ended, 19 of which formed the basis of this study. The relevant questions focused on the knowledge and use of deterrent methods and attitudes and behaviours towards elephants (Table 1). To further localize our efforts, we enlisted the help of Hellen Kiute, a facilitator from the community, to conduct the survey sessions. In 2020, with the support of local government officials, we selected as participants 30–35 farmers from each village who were most affected by elephant crop foraging (a total of 206 participants from the six villages; Supplementary Table 1). To avoid gender bias, we selected c. 50% male and c. 50% female participants. We surveyed only one member per household to maintain sample independence.

Table 1 Questions administered to farmers from six villages in the Kasigau Wildlife Corridor, near Rukinga Wildlife Sanctuary, Kenya (Fig. 1), related to crop foraging by African savannah elephants Loxodonta africana.

We invited individuals to participate in a meeting occurring in their respective village in September 2020. At the meeting the facilitator administered a paper survey and was available to answer any queries and aid those who might be illiterate (White et al., Reference White, Jennings, Renwick and Barker2005), which was c. 15% of the participants (n = 31 across all villages). Participants completed the surveys independently of each other. To ensure construct validity, the facilitator orally defined the concepts of crop damage (i.e. the act of any animal entering a farm and consuming or trampling crops) and deterrents (i.e. any method used to prevent entry or frighten wildlife away from farms). These methods could involve both active deterrence such as yelling, waving a torch (flashlight) or patrolling, or passive methods such as any type of fencing. We subsequently transferred the data from the hardcopy surveys into a database for analysis.

We edited the data to create groupings for survey questions and to prepare the data for analysis (Supplementary Material 2), and we then selected models to evaluate our hypotheses (Table 2). We evaluated each variable of interest (age, education level, years farming, gender, exposure to deterrents, farm size and village) for collinearity with a robust variance inflation factor, and all factors were near 1.0, signifying no collinearity between these variables. We analysed the models using a generalized linear model that accounts for the non-normal distribution of response variables. We used a binomial distribution when creating logistic regression models. We compared our results using the Akaike information criterion corrected for small sample sizes (AICc) as a measure of fit. We also report adjusted pseudo-r 2 values, following a previously proposed approach (Zhang, Reference Zhang2017). We considered models with ΔAICc ≤ 2 (compared to the best model; Burnham & Anderson, Reference Burnham and Anderson2002) to be competitive and evaluated them using the explanatory values of model weights and adjusted pseudo-r 2 values. Top models are reported, but these are not indicators of hypothesis support as we evaluated each model independently according to respective model metrics, effect sizes and sociological meaning. For each top model we report effect sizes with 95% confidence intervals (CI) of significant coefficients (P < 0.150; Arnold, Reference Arnold2010) to further describe the significance of our data. We conducted all analyses in R 4.0.2 (R Core Team, Reference .2020).

Table 2 A priori models used to test hypotheses related to the use of deterrents to prevent crop damage by elephants in the six study villages. Note that farm size is a quadratic term (area size; indicated by superscript 2), so the relationship is not linear as for the other terms.

1 DE, deterrent-exposure hypothesis; DU, deterrent-use hypothesis; EB, economic-barriers hypothesis.

Results

Of the 206 respondents that completed the survey, the number of participants per village ranged from 29 to 37. The ratio of female to male participants was 53:47, although this varied by village (Supplementary Table 1). Respondents ranged in age from 18 to 85 years, with a mean age of 46 ± SD 14 years and household size ranged from 2 to 34 with a mean of 8 ± SD 4. Most respondents (64%) had a primary education level, 22% had completed secondary education, 7% had completed tertiary education and the remaining 7% had no formal education. The main source of income for 92% of respondents was farming.

For the deterrent-use hypothesis, model 12 (education level + exposure to deterrents) best described which farmers used deterrents (Table 3). Individuals exposed to information about deterrents were 3.65 (95% CI: 1.65–8.63) times more likely to use deterrents (P = 0.002). Respondents with secondary levels of education were 4.64 (95% CI: 1.21–20.75) times more likely to use deterrents compared to those with no education (P = 0.031). We also found that individuals with primary education were 1.94 (95% CI: 0.55–7.87) times more likely to use deterrents compared to those with no education, but this was not significant (P = 0.310). Education level alone (model 3) was the best-fitting model for farmers who had received information on deterrents (deterrent-exposure hypothesis). We found that respondents with tertiary education were 5.00 (95% CI: 0.89–40.97) times more likely to have received information on deterrents compared to those with no education (P = 0.087). For information specifically on fencing (deterrent-exposure hypothesis), the null model (1) provided the best fit (Table 4). For the economic-barriers hypothesis, exposure to deterrents (model 6) was the best-fitting model for those who believed they could use deterrents (Table 5). However, individuals who had not received information on deterrents were 2.78 (95% CI: 1.01–7.63) times more likely to believe that they could implement deterrents than those who had received such information (P = 0.046).

Table 3 The top five results from binomial generalized linear models for the deterrent-use hypothesis, evaluating which farmers from the six study villages were using deterrents, based on demographic variables (n = 189). Model descriptions and terms are presented in Table 2 and full model results are in Supplementary Table 4. For each model, the table shows the Akaike information criterion adjusted for small sample sizes (AICc), the difference in AICc from the best-performing model (ΔAICc), the adjusted pseudo-r 2 value, the Akaike weight (wi), log-likelihood (LL) and the number of variables (k).

Table 4 Results of binomial generalized linear models for the deterrent-exposure hypothesis, a two-part hypothesis evaluating whether farmers from the six study villages had been exposed to information on any type of deterrent information and specifically on fencing deterrents, based on demographic variables (n = 189). Model descriptions and terms are presented in Tables 2 and 3.

Table 5 Top five results from binomial generalized linear models for the economic-barriers hypothesis evaluating demographic factors of farmers from the six study villages who said that they definitely could implement deterrents (n = 98). Model descriptions and terms are presented in Tables 2 and 3, and full results are in Supplementary Table 5.

For the survey question associated with the deterrent-use hypothesis, 54% of respondents used at least one form of deterrent to prevent crop foraging. Regarding the deterrent-exposure hypothesis, 22% of respondents had received information on deterrent methods and 10% had received information specifically on fences. The types of deterrent methods about which villagers had received information were primarily traditional fencing methods (Supplementary Table 2), and most individuals (87%) who used deterrents used traditional types (Supplementary Table 3). Regarding the economic-barriers hypothesis, only 40% of respondents believed that they could invest in deterrents, and all who said they could not make such an investment (123 respondents) cited economic constraints as the reason.

Elephants were cited as the main reason behind the crop losses by 84% of respondents; 74% had actively chased elephants from their farms but few (6%) had attempted to harm the animals. When asked how much they fear elephants, 55% of respondents said they were very afraid, 23% were somewhat or a little bit afraid, 15% were not afraid at all and 7% were unsure. Only 16% of respondents had received information on how to safely live with elephants.

Discussion

Over half of the participants in this study used some type of elephant deterrent, of which the majority were of traditional types. Education level and exposure to relevant information were the prevalent variables in the top models for the deterrent-use hypothesis. However, the adjusted pseudo-r 2 values for these models indicated that not much of the variation in responses was explained by the variables. Effect sizes were more descriptive of the relationship between variables and demographic categories as farmers were almost four times more likely to use deterrents if they had been exposed to information and if they had higher education levels. The deterrent-exposure hypotheses was supported in that higher education levels (model 3) were related to receipt of general information on deterrents, and those with tertiary education were more likely to have received such information but not specific information on deterrent fencing, for which the null model provided the best fit (model 1). Finally, the economic-barriers hypothesis was not supported in terms of education level having an effect, but instead exposure to deterrent information (model 6) was associated with whether farmers believed that they could implement such deterrents. Those who had not received information (no exposure) were almost three times more likely to believe that they could implement such deterrents. This may seem counterintuitive, but appears to show initial over-confidence on the part of the farmers and aligns with our additional results and previous studies showing that once farmers understand the expense and effort required to implement and maintain deterrents, they may view such implementation unfavourably or realise they cannot afford the relevant materials (Noga et al., Reference Noga, Kolawole, Thakadu and Masunga2015; Vogel et al., Reference Vogel, Songhurst, McCulloch and Stronza2022). The prevalence and effect sizes of the variables of exposure and education across several models suggest that farmers receiving information and the variation in education appear to significantly affect usage and uptake behaviours. Why education level is important in this context is unclear, but it could be related to those with more education also having higher income levels, enabling them to afford deterrent materials.

Understanding local attitudes and contexts related to elephants and crop foraging is an important part of holistic approaches to addressing conflict. Respondents in the study area live in fear of elephants, and such fear can affect their health and well-being and incur lost opportunity costs (Barua et al., Reference Barua, Bhagwat and Jadhav2013; Mmbaga et al., Reference Mmbaga, Munishi and Treydte2017; Thondhlana et al., Reference Thondhlana, Redpath, Vedeld, van Eden, Pascual, Sherren and Murata2020). Another factor that could lead farmers to fear elephants was that most had never received information on how to safely live near elephants or interact with them. In addition, the majority of villagers blamed elephants for their crop losses despite significant drought having occurred and despite the presence of other pests (Karimi, Reference Karimi2009; Kasaine & Githiru, Reference Kasaine and Githiru2016). Fears and frustrations from crop losses could lead farmers to retaliate against elephants (Naughton-Treves & Treves, Reference Naughton-Treves, Treves, Woodroffe, Thirgood and Rabinowitz2005; Treves & Santiago-Ávila, Reference Treves and Santiago-Ávila2020), but only a small percentage of farmers stated they had attempted to harm elephants, although most respondents admitted to actively chasing elephants from their farms. This is a common way of deterring elephants (Fernando, Reference Fernando2015; Mariki et al., Reference Mariki, Svarstad and Benjaminsen2015; Montero-Botey et al., Reference Montero-Botey, Soliño, Perea and Martínez-Jauregui2021), but it is also dangerous. Our findings show that negative interactions with elephants in this area represent a threat to human health and livelihoods and pose concerns for elephant conservation.

We demonstrated that exposure to relevant information plays an important role in the usage of deterrents, which has broad implications for conservation management. Most respondents had never received information on ways to prevent crop foraging, and most of the information they had received was on traditional measures; very few had received information specifically about different types of fencing (usually modern methods). Not only is information rarely reaching villagers but, when it does, it focuses on methods that are potentially less effective and more time-consuming (although perhaps more easily implemented in practice). Only 18% of respondents used modern methods such as deploying solar lights along fencing, planting chili peppers (an unpalatable crop; Osborn & Parker, Reference Osborn and Parker2003) or installing Kasaine metal strip fences (Von Hagen et al., Reference Von Hagen, Kasaine, Githiru, Amakobe, Mutwiwa and Schulte2021). This low uptake rate of modern deterrents demonstrates the need for outreach efforts that provide up-to-date information on such methods.

All respondents who believed that they could not implement deterrents cited a lack of financial resources as the reason, which is consistent with previous research (Vedeld et al., Reference Vedeld, Jumane, Wapalila and Songorwa2012; Seoraj-Pillai & Pillay, Reference Seoraj-Pillai and Pillay2017). This is a key factor that is often overlooked in mitigation plans. Our findings in this community suggest that if the deterrent methods are too labour intensive or require too many resources, or if there is a lack of community cooperation, then they may fail even if they have been implemented successfully in other areas (Osborn & Parker, Reference Osborn and Parker2003; Sitati & Walpole, Reference Sitati and Walpole2006; Graham & Ochieng, Reference Graham and Ochieng2008). That financial constraints influence the deterrent usage of farmers has also been shown in recent research from Botswana (Vogel et al., Reference Vogel, Songhurst, McCulloch and Stronza2022).

Other than exposure and education level, demographic variables had little explanatory power in the models, probably because of the respondent population being essentially homogeneous in their views. Some respondents may have provided answers they thought would be viewed favourably, potentially introducing social desirability bias (Chung & Monroe, Reference Chung and Monroe2003). Several respondents did not fully answer the questions, and some gave contradictory answers, indicating that some questions may have been unclear; we eliminated these responses from our analysis (Supplementary Material 2). Despite these limitations, our findings are relevant and applicable for agencies managing human–elephant interactions.

In our study area in the Kasigau Wildlife Corridor in Kenya, we found that vital information for reducing elephant crop foraging is not reaching local farmers and that multiple barriers exist regarding the uptake of deterrents. Our findings suggest several key management recommendations. Firstly, increased outreach efforts to farmers are needed to share knowledge on deterrent approaches, especially in remote areas. Secondly, community leaders are needed who can help encourage farming communities to incorporate existing deterrents or who can create and share sustainable solutions that are effective and practical within the local context. Thirdly, farmers need information on how to live safely near elephants, to decrease both the fear and the risk associated with elephant encounters. A combination of these efforts could increase local food security, tolerance of elephants and support for conservation programmes. Without additional educational and financial resources, elephant crop foraging is likely to persist. However, our study provides insights that could help practitioners address some previously little examined economic and social concerns of farmers related to the use of elephant deterrents.

Author contributions

Study design: LVH, BAS, KD, MG, SZ, CAL; fieldwork: LVH; data analysis: LVH, TDS, KD, CAL; writing: LVH, BAS, MG, CAL.

Acknowledgements

We thank the collaborators at Wildlife Works, including the community outreach team, staff scientists, rangers and office personnel, and Hellen Kiute, who facilitated local stakeholder interactions. LVH was supported by grants from the International Elephant Foundation, the Elephant Manager's Association and Auburn University, and the project was facilitated in conjunction with the Earthwatch Institute's Elephants and Sustainable Agriculture in Kenya project. We appreciate the trust bestowed upon us by the Kenyan people, including village representatives and participants from the communities that welcomed us.

Conflicts of interest

None.

Ethical standards

This research abided by the Oryx guidelines on ethical standards. Survey and consent procedures were approved by Auburn University's Institutional Review Board panel (Protocol no. 20-440 EX 2009) and Strathmore University's Institutional Ethics Review Committee (Approval no. SU-IERC0877/20) in Kenya. The research was conducted under the umbrella of Wildlife Works' PIC/MAT agreement with Kenya Wildlife Service and with approval from NACOSTI (Kenya's science agency, License no. NACOSTI/P/20/2292).

Data availability

Data are available in the Supplementary Material and on request from the corresponding author.

Footnotes

The supplementary material for this article is available at doi.org/10.1017/S0030605323001795

References

Adams, T., Mwezi, I. & Jordan, N.R. (2020) Panic at the disco: solar-powered strobe light barriers reduce field incursion by African elephants Loxodonta africana in Chobe District, Botswana. Oryx, 55, 739746.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arnold, T.W. (2010) Uninformative parameters and model selection using Akaike's Information Criteria. Journal of Wildlife Management, 74, 11751178.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barua, M., Bhagwat, S. & Jadhav, S. (2013) The hidden dimensions of human–wildlife conflict: health impacts, opportunity and transaction costs. Biological Conservation, 157, 309316.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buchholtz, E.K., Fitzgerald, L.A., Songhurst, A., McCulloch, G.P. & Stronza, A.L. (2020) Experts and elephants: local ecological knowledge predicts landscape use for a species involved in human-wildlife conflict. Ecology and Society, 25, 26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burnham, K.P. & Anderson, D.R. (2002) Model Selection and Multimodal Inference: A Practical Information-Theoretic Approach, Second edition. Springer-Verlag, New York, USA.Google Scholar
Chang'a, A., de Souza, N., Muya, J., Keyyu, J., Mwakatobe, A., Malugu, L. et al. (2016) Scaling-up the use of chili fences for reducing human-elephant conflict across landscapes in Tanzania. Tropical Conservation Science, 9, 921930.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chung, J. & Monroe, G. (2003) Exploring social desirability bias. Journal of Business Ethics, 44, 291302.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davies, T.E., Wilson, S., Hazarika, N., Chakrabarty, J., Das, D., Hodgson, D.J. & Zimmermann, A. (2011) Effectiveness of intervention methods against crop-raiding elephants. Conservation Letters, 4, 346354.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dickman, A.J. (2010) Complexities of conflict: The importance of considering social factors for effectively resolving human–wildlife conflict. Animal Conservation, 13, 458466.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Distefano, E. (2005) Human-wildlife Conflict Worldwide: Collection of Case Studies, Analysis of Management Strategies and Good Practices. Food and Agricultural Organization, Rome, Italy.Google Scholar
Dohmen, T., Falk, A., Huffman, D., Sunde, U., Schupp, J. & Wagner, G. (2011) Individual risk attitudes: measurement, determinants and behavioral consequences. Journal of European Economic Association, 9, 522550.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fernando, P. (2015) Managing elephants in Sri Lanka: where we are and where we need to be. Ceylon Journal of Science, 44, 111.Google Scholar
Folke, C., Hahn, T., Olsson, P. & Norberg, J. (2005) Adaptive governance of social-ecological systems. Annual Review of Environment and Resources, 30, 441473.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Galvin, K.A., Beeton, T.A. & Luizza, M.W. (2018) African community-based conservation: A systematic review of social and ecological outcomes. Ecology and Society, 23, 39.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gilsdorf, J.M., Hygnstrom, S.E. & VerCauteren, K.C. (2002) Use of frightening devices in wildlife damage management. Integrated Pest Management Reviews, 7, 2945.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Githiru, M., Mutwiwa, U., Kasaine, S. & Schulte, B. (2017) A spanner in the works: human–elephant conflict complicates the food–water–energy nexus in drylands of Africa. Frontiers in Environmental Science, 5, 69.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Graham, M.D. & Ochieng, T. (2008) Uptake and performance of farm-based measures for reducing crop raiding by elephants Loxodonta africana among smallholder farms in Laikipia District, Kenya. Oryx, 42, 7682.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gross, E.M. (2019) Tackling Routes to Coexistence: Human-Elephant Conflict in Sub-Saharan Africa. GIZ Partnership against Poaching and Illegal Wildlife Trade, Bonn, Germany.Google Scholar
Gross, E.M., Lahkar, B.P., Subedi, N., Nyirenda, V.R., Lichtenfeld, L.L. & Jakoby, O. (2018) Seasonality, crop type and crop phenology influence crop damage by wildlife herbivores in Africa and Asia. Biodiversity and Conservation, 27, 20292050.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hedges, S. & Gunaryadi, D. (2010) Reducing human–elephant conflict: do chillies help deter elephants from entering crop fields? Oryx, 44, 139146.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hill, C.M. (2000) Conflict of interest between people and baboons: crop raiding in Uganda. International Journal of Primatology, 21, 299315.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hill, C.M. (2018) Crop foraging, crop losses, and crop raiding. Annual Review of Anthropology, 47, 377394.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoare, R. (2012) Lessons from 15 years of human–elephant conflict mitigation: management considerations involving biological, physical and governance issues in Africa. Pachyderm, 51, 6074.Google Scholar
Hoffmeier-Karimi, R.R. & Schulte, B.A. (2015) Assessing perceived and documented crop damage in a Tanzanian village impacted by human-elephant conflict (HEC). Pachyderm, 56, 5160.Google Scholar
Kansky, R. & Knight, A.T. (2014) Key factors driving attitudes towards large mammals in conflict with humans. Biological Conservation, 179, 93105.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karimi, R.R. (2009) An assessment of perceived crop damage in a Tanzanian village impacted by human-elephant conflict and an investigation of deterrent properties of African elephant (Loxodonta africana) exudates using bioassays. MSc thesis. Western Kentucky University, Bowling Green, USA.Google Scholar
Kasaine, S. & Githiru, M. (2016) Sasenyi Valley Baseline Household Survey: Earthwatch Project. Wildlife Works, Nairobi, Kenya.Google Scholar
Kassilly, F.N., Tsingalia, H.M. & Gossow, H. (2008) Mitigating human-wildlife conflicts through wildlife fencing: a Kenyan case study. Wildlife Biology in Practice, 4, 3038.Google Scholar
Killion, A.K., Ramirez, J.M. & Carter, N.H. (2020) Human adaptation strategies are key to co-benefits in human–wildlife systems. Conservation Letters, 14, e12769.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
King, L.E. (2010) The interaction between the African elephant (Loxodonta africana africana) and the African honeybee (Apis mellifera scutellata) and its potential application as an elephant deterrent. PhD thesis. University of Oxford, Oxford, UK.Google Scholar
König, H.J., Kiffner, C., Kramer-Schadt, S., Fürst, C., Keuling, O. & Ford, A.T. (2020) Human–wildlife coexistence in a changing world. Conservation Biology, 34, 786794.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krijger, I.M., Belmain, S.R., Singleton, G.R., Groot Koerkamp, P.W.G. & Meerburg, B.G. (2017) The need to implement the landscape of fear within rodent pest management strategies. Pest Management Science, 73, 23972402.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Litoroh, M., Omondi, P., Kock, R. & Amin, R. (2012) Conservation and Management Strategy for the Elephant in Kenya. Kenya Wildlife Service, Nairobi, Kenya.Google Scholar
Mackenzie, C.A. & Ahabyona, P. (2012) Elephants in the garden: financial and social costs of crop raiding. Ecological Economics, 75, 7282.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Madden, F. (2004) Creating coexistence between humans and wildlife: global perspectives on local efforts to address human–wildlife conflict. Human Dimensions of Wildlife, 9, 247257.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mariki, S.B., Svarstad, H. & Benjaminsen, T.A. (2015) Elephants over the cliff: explaining wildlife killings in Tanzania. Land Use Policy, 44, 1830.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mc Guinness, S.K. (2016) Perceptions of crop raiding: effects of land tenure and agro-industry on human–wildlife conflict. Animal Conservation, 19, 578587.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McKee, S.C., Shwiff, S.A. & Anderson, A.M. (2021) Estimation of wildlife damage from federal crop insurance data. Pest Management Science, 77, 406416.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mmbaga, N.E., Munishi, L.K. & Treydte, A.C. (2017) Balancing African elephant conservation with human well-being in Rombo Area, Tanzania. Advances in Ecology, 2017, 4184261.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Montero-Botey, M., Soliño, M., Perea, R. & Martínez-Jauregui, M. (2021) Exploring rangers’ preferences for community-based strategies to improve human-elephant coexistence in African natural corridors. Animal Conservation, 24, 982993.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mumby, H.S. & Plotnik, J.M. (2018) Taking the elephants’ perspective: remembering elephant behavior, cognition and ecology in human-elephant conflict mitigation. Frontiers in Ecology and Evolution, 2010, 3389.Google Scholar
Naughton-Treves, L. & Treves, A. (2005) Socio-ecological factors shaping local support for wildlife: crop-raiding by elephants and other wildlife in Africa. In People and Wildlife: Conflict or Coexistence? (eds Woodroffe, R., Thirgood, S., & Rabinowitz, A.), pp. 252277. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ngene, S., Lala, F., Nzisa, M., Kimitei, K., Mukeka, J., Kiambi, S. et al. (2017) Aerial Total Count of Elephants, Buffalo and Giraffe in the Tsavo-Mkomazi Ecosystem. Kenya Wildlife Service, Nairobi, Kenya, and Tanzania Wildlife Research Institute, Arusha, Tanzania.Google Scholar
Noga, S.R., Kolawole, O.D., Thakadu, O. & Masunga, G. (2015) Small farmers’ adoption behaviour: uptake of elephant crop-raiding deterrent innovations in the Okavango Delta, Botswana. African Journal of Science, Technology, Innovation and Development, 7, 408419.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Noga, S.R., Security, F., Kolawole, O.D., Thakadu, O.T. & Masunga, G.S. (2016) Claims and counterclaims: institutional arrangements and farmers’ response to the delivery and adoption of innovations in the Okavango Delta, Botswana. Journal of Agricultural Education and Extension, 23, 121139.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oduro-Ofori, E. (2014) Effects of education on the agricultural productivity of farmers in the Offinso municipality. International Journal of Development Research, 4, 19511960.Google Scholar
Okun, M.A. & Siegler, I.C. (1976) Relation between preference for intermediate risk and adult age in men: a cross cultural validation. Developmental Psychology, 12, 565566.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Omondi, P., Bitok, E.K., Mukeka, J., Mayienda, R., & Litoroh, M. (2008) Total Aerial Count of Elephants and Other Large Mammal Species of Tsavo/Mkomazi Ecosystem. Kenya Wildlife Service and African Wildlife Foundation, Nairobi, Kenya.Google Scholar
Osborn, F.V. (2004) Seasonal variation of feeding patterns and food selection by crop-raiding elephants in Zimbabwe. African Journal of Ecology, 42, 322327.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Osborn, F.V. & Parker, G.E. (2003) Towards an integrated approach for reducing the conflict between elephants and people: a review of current research. Oryx, 37, 16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Osipova, L., Okello, M.M., Njumbi, S.J., Ngene, S., Western, D., Hayward, M.W. & Balkenhol, N. (2018) Fencing solves human-wildlife conflict locally but shifts problems elsewhere: A case study using functional connectivity modelling of the African elephant. Journal of Applied Ecology, 55, 26732684.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Owen-Smith, N., Fryxell, J.M. & Merrill, E.H. (2010) Foraging theory upscaled: the behavioural ecology of herbivore movement. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 365, 22672278.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Parker, G.E. & Osborn, F. (2006) Investigating the potential for chilli Capsicum spp. to reduce human-wildlife conflict in Zimbabwe. Oryx, 40, 343346.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pozo, R.A., LeFlore, E.G., Duthie, A.B., Bunnefeld, N., Jones, I.L., Minderman, J. et al. (2020) A multispecies assessment of wildlife impacts on local community livelihoods. Conservation Biology, 35, 297306.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
R Core Team (2020) R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria., r-project.org [accessed February 2024].Google Scholar
Raphela, T.D. & Pillay, N. (2021) Explaining the effect of crop-raiding on food security of subsistence farmers of KwaZulu Natal, South Africa. Frontiers in Sustainable Food Systems, 5, 111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Redpath, S.M., Bhatia, S. & Young, J. (2015) Tilting at wildlife: reconsidering human–wildlife conflict. Oryx, 49, 222225.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rieger, M. & Mata, R. (2015) On the generality of age differences in social and nonsocial decision making. Journal of Gerontology, 70, 200212.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salerno, J., Stevens, F., Gaughan, A., Hilton, T., Bailey, K., Bowles, T. et al. (2021) Wildlife impacts and changing climate pose compounding threats to human food security. Current Biology, 31, 50775085.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schulte, B.A. (2016) Learning and applications of chemical signals in vertebrates for human–wildlife conflict mitigation. In Chemical Signals in Vertebrates 13 (eds Schulte, B.A., Goodwin, T.E. & Ferkin, M.H.), pp. 499510. Springer, Cham, Switzerland.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seoraj-Pillai, N. & Pillay, N. (2017) A meta-analysis of human–wildlife conflict: South African and global perspectives. Sustainability (Switzerland), 9, 121.Google Scholar
Sitati, N.W. & Walpole, M.J. (2006) Assessing farm-based measures for mitigating human-elephant conflict in Transmara District, Kenya. Oryx, 40, 279286.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sukumar, R. (1990) Ecology of the Asian elephant in southern India. II. Feeding habits and crop raiding patterns. Journal of Tropical Ecology, 6, 3353.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thondhlana, G., Redpath, S.M., Vedeld, P.O., van Eden, L., Pascual, U., Sherren, K. & Murata, C. (2020) Non-material costs of wildlife conservation to local people and their implications for conservation interventions. Biological Conservation, 246, 108578.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Treves, A. & Santiago-Ávila, F.J. (2020) Myths and assumptions about human-wildlife conflict and coexistence. Conservation Biology, 34, 811818.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Treves, A., Wallace, R.B. & White, S. (2009) Participatory planning of interventions to mitigate human–wildlife conflicts. Conservation Biology, 23, 15771587.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vedeld, P., Jumane, A., Wapalila, G. & Songorwa, A. (2012) Protected areas, poverty and conflicts. A livelihood case study of Mikumi National Park, Tanzania. Forest Policy and Economics, 21, 2031.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vogel, S., Songhurst, A., McCulloch, G. & Stronza, A. (2022) Understanding farmers’ reasons behind mitigation decisions is key in supporting their coexistence with wildlife. People and Nature, 4, 13051318.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Von Hagen, L., Kasaine, S., Githiru, M., Amakobe, B., Mutwiwa, U.N. & Schulte, B.A. (2021) Metal strip fences for preventing African elephant (Loxodonta africana) crop foraging in the Kasigau Wildlife Corridor, Kenya. African Journal of Ecology, 59, 293298.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Waweru, J., Omondi, P., Ngene, S., Mukeka, J., Wanyonyi, E., Ngoru, B. et al. (2021) National Wildlife Census 2021 Report. Wildlife Research and Training Institute and Kenya Wildlife Service, Nairobi, Kenya.Google Scholar
Western, D., Waithaka, J. & Kamanga, J. (2015) Finding space for wildlife beyond national parks and reducing conflict through community-based conservation: the Kenya experience. Parks, 21, 5162.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
White, P.C.L., Jennings, N.V., Renwick, A.R. & Barker, N.H.L. (2005) Questionnaires in ecology: a review of past use and recommendations for best practice. Journal of Applied Ecology, 42, 421430.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Young, J.C., Marzano, M., White, R.M., McCracken, D.I., Redpath, S.M., Carss, D.N. et al. (2010) The emergence of biodiversity conflicts from biodiversity impacts: characteristics and management strategies. Biodiversity and Conservation, 19, 39733990.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zhang, D. (2017) A coefficient of determination for generalized linear models. The American Statistician, 71, 310316.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Fig. 1 The Kasigau Wildlife Corridor in Kenya, shown with its 14 community ranches and the locations of the six study villages.

Figure 1

Table 1 Questions administered to farmers from six villages in the Kasigau Wildlife Corridor, near Rukinga Wildlife Sanctuary, Kenya (Fig. 1), related to crop foraging by African savannah elephants Loxodonta africana.

Figure 2

Table 2 A priori models used to test hypotheses related to the use of deterrents to prevent crop damage by elephants in the six study villages. Note that farm size is a quadratic term (area size; indicated by superscript 2), so the relationship is not linear as for the other terms.

Figure 3

Table 3 The top five results from binomial generalized linear models for the deterrent-use hypothesis, evaluating which farmers from the six study villages were using deterrents, based on demographic variables (n = 189). Model descriptions and terms are presented in Table 2 and full model results are in Supplementary Table 4. For each model, the table shows the Akaike information criterion adjusted for small sample sizes (AICc), the difference in AICc from the best-performing model (ΔAICc), the adjusted pseudo-r2 value, the Akaike weight (wi), log-likelihood (LL) and the number of variables (k).

Figure 4

Table 4 Results of binomial generalized linear models for the deterrent-exposure hypothesis, a two-part hypothesis evaluating whether farmers from the six study villages had been exposed to information on any type of deterrent information and specifically on fencing deterrents, based on demographic variables (n = 189). Model descriptions and terms are presented in Tables 2 and 3.

Figure 5

Table 5 Top five results from binomial generalized linear models for the economic-barriers hypothesis evaluating demographic factors of farmers from the six study villages who said that they definitely could implement deterrents (n = 98). Model descriptions and terms are presented in Tables 2 and 3, and full results are in Supplementary Table 5.

Supplementary material: File

Von Hagen et al. supplementary material

Von Hagen et al. supplementary material
Download Von Hagen et al. supplementary material(File)
File 100.8 KB