Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-fbnjt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-10T09:03:50.796Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

An assessment of rapamycin for weakening binge-eating memories via reconsolidation: a pre-registered, double-blind randomised placebo-controlled experimental study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  18 November 2019

Katie Walsh
Affiliation:
Clinical, Educational and Health Psychology, University College London, Gower Street, London, WC1E 6BT
Georges Iskandar
Affiliation:
University College Hospital and University College Hospital at Westmoreland Street, London, UK
Sunjeev K. Kamboj
Affiliation:
Clinical, Educational and Health Psychology, University College London, Gower Street, London, WC1E 6BT
Ravi K. Das*
Affiliation:
Clinical, Educational and Health Psychology, University College London, Gower Street, London, WC1E 6BT
*
Author for correspondence: Ravi K. Das, E-mail: ravi.das@ucl.ac.uk

Abstract

Background

Maladaptive learning linking environmental food cues to high-palatability food reward plays a central role in overconsumption in obesity and binge eating disorders. The process of memory reconsolidation offers a mechanism to weaken such learning, potentially ameliorating over-eating behaviour. Here we investigated whether putatively interfering with synaptic plasticity using the mammalian target of rapamycin (mTOR) inhibitor, rapamycin, could weaken retrieved chocolate reward memories through blockade of reconsolidation

Methods

Seventy five healthy volunteers with a tendency to binge eat chocolate were randomised to retrieve chocolate reward memory under 10 mg rapamycin (RET + RAP, active condition), or placebo (RET + PBO), or they received 10 mg rapamycin without subsequent retrieval (NO RET + RAP). Indices of chocolate reward memory strength were assessed one week pre and post manipulation and at one month follow-up.

Results

Contrary to hypotheses, the RET + RAP group did not show any greater reduction than control groups on indices of motivational salience of chocolate cues, motivation to consume chocolate or liking of chocolate. Mild evidence of improvement in the RET + RAP group was found, but this was limited to reduced chocolate binge episodes and improved healthy food choices.

Conclusions

We did not find convincing evidence of comprehensive naturalistic chocolate reward memory reconsolidation blockade by rapamycin. The effects on chocolate bingeing and food choices may warrant further investigation. These limited positive findings may be attributable to insufficient interference with mTOR signalling with 10 mg rapamycin, or failure to destabilise chocolate memories during retrieval.

Type
Original Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2019

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Avena, NM and Bocarsly, ME (2012) Dysregulation of brain reward systems in eating disorders: neurochemical information from animal models of binge eating, bulimia nervosa, and anorexia nervosa. Neuropharmacology 63, 8796.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barak, S, Liu, F, Hamida, SB, Yowell, QV, Neasta, J, Kharazia, V, Janak, PH, Ron, D, Goltseker, K, Bolotin, L, Barak, S, Liu, F, Hamida, SB, Yowell, QV, Neasta, J, Kharazia, V, Janak, PH and Ron, D (2013) Disruption of alcohol-related memories by mTORC1 inhibition prevents relapse. Nature Neuroscience 16, 11111117.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bavassi, L, Forcato, C, Fernández, RS, De Pino, G, Pedreira, ME and Villarreal, MF (2019) Retrieval of retrained and reconsolidated memories are associated with a distinct neural network. Scientific Reports 9, 113.Google ScholarPubMed
Beck, AT, Steer, RA and Carbin, MG (1988) Psychometric properties of the Beck Depression Inventory: twenty-five years of evaluation. Clinical Psychology Review 8, 77100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benton, D, Greenfield, K and Morgan, M (1998) The development of the attitudes to chocolate questionnaire. Personality and Individual Differences 24, 513520.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blechert, J, Meule, A, Busch, NA and Ohla, K (2014) Food-pics: an image database for experimental research on eating and appetite. Frontiers in Psychology 5, 617.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cameron, JD, Chaput, J-P, Sjödin, AM and Goldfield, GS (2017) Brain on fire: incentive salience, hedonic hot spots, dopamine, obesity, and other hunger games. Annual Review of Nutrition 37, 183205.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cepeda-Benito, A, Gleaves, DH, Williams, TL and Erath, SA (2001) The development and validation of the state and trait food-cravings questionnaires. Behavior Therapy 31, 151173.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Das, RK, Lawn, W and Kamboj, SK (2015) Rewriting the valuation and salience of alcohol-related stimuli via memory reconsolidation. Translational Psychiatry 5, e645e645.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Das, RK, Gale, G, Hennessy, V and Kamboj, SK (2018 a) A prediction error-driven retrieval procedure for destabilizing and rewriting maladaptive reward memories in hazardous drinkers. Journal of Visualized Experiments 131, e56097e56097.Google Scholar
Das, RK, Walsh, K, Hannaford, J, Lazzarino, AI and Kamboj, SK (2018 b) Nitrous oxide may interfere with the reconsolidation of drinking memories in hazardous drinkers in a prediction-error-dependent manner. European Neuropsychopharmacology 28, 828840.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de la Fuente, V, Freudenthal, R and Romano, A (2011) Reconsolidation or extinction: transcription factor switch in the determination of memory course after retrieval. The Journal of Neuroscience: the Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience 31, 55625573.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Doyère, V, Dȩbiec, J, Monfils, MH, Schafe, GE and LeDoux, JE (2007) Synapse-specific reconsolidation of distinct fear memories in the lateral amygdala. Nature Neuroscience 10, 414416.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Drewnowski, A (2009) Energy density, palatability, and satiety: implications for weight control. Nutrition Reviews 56, 347353.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Erlanson-Albertsson, C (2005) How palatable food disrupts appetite regulation. Basic & Clinical Pharmacology & Toxicology 97, 6173.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Faliagkas, L, Rao-Ruiz, P and Kindt, M (2018) Emotional memory expression is misleading: delineating transitions between memory processes. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences 19, 116122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flavell, CR, Lambert, EA, Winters, BD and Bredy, TW (2013) Mechanisms governing the reactivation-dependent destabilization of memories and their role in extinction. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience 7, 111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Glover, EM, Ressler, KJ and Davis, M (2010) Differing effects of systemically administered rapamycin on consolidation and reconsolidation of context v. cued fear memories. Learning & memory (Cold Spring Harbor, N.Y.) 17, 577581.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gormally, J, Black, S, Daston, S and Rardin, D (1982) The assessment of binge eating severity among obese persons. Addicrive Behaviors 7, 4755.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hall, KD, Ayuketah, A, Brychta, R, Cai, H, Cassimatis, T, Chen, KY, Chung, ST, Costa, E, Courville, A, Darcey, V, Fletcher, LA, Forde, CG, Gharib, AM, Guo, J, Howard, R, Joseph, PV, McGehee, S, Ouwerkerk, R, Raisinger, K, Rozga, I, Stagliano, M, Walter, M, Walter, PJ, Yang, S and Zhou, M (2019) Ultra-processed diets cause excess calorie intake and weight gain: an inpatient randomized controlled trial of ad libitum food intake. Cell Metabolism, 30, 6777.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hon, T, Das, RK and Kamboj, SK (2016) The effects of cognitive reappraisal following retrieval-procedures designed to destabilize alcohol memories in high-risk drinkers. Psychopharmacology 233, 851861.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hormes, JM and Meule, A (2016) Psychometric properties of the English Food Cravings Questionnaire-Trait-reduced (FCQ-T-r). Eating Behaviors 20, 3438.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hyman, SE (2005) Addiction: a disease of learning and memory. American Journal of Psychiatry 162, 14141422.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kelley, AE and Berridge, KC (2002) The neuroscience of natural rewards: relevance to addictive drugs. The Journal of Neuroscience: the Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience 22, 33063311.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, JLC, Nader, K and Schiller, D (2017) An update on memory reconsolidation updating. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 21, 531545.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Li, Y, Meloni, EG, Carlezon, WA, Milad, MR, Pitman, RK, Nader, K and Bolshakov, VY (2013) Learning and reconsolidation implicate different synaptic mechanisms. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 110, 47984803.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lowe, MR, Butryn, ML, Didie, ER, Annunziato, RA, Thomas, JG, Crerand, CE, Ochner, CN, Coletta, MC, Bellace, D, Wallaert, M and Halford, J (2009) The Power of Food Scale. A new measure of the psychological influence of the food environment. Appetite 53, 114118.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Merlo, E, Milton, AL, Goozée, ZY, Theobald, DE and Everitt, BJ (2014) Reconsolidation and extinction are dissociable and mutually exclusive processes: behavioral and molecular evidence. The Journal of Neuroscience 34, 24222431.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Merlo, E, Bekinschtein, P, Jonkman, S and Medina, JH (2015) Molecular mechanisms of memory consolidation, reconsolidation, and persistence. Neural Plasticity 2015, 12.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nader, K, Schafe, GE and Le Doux, JE (2000) Fear memories require protein synthesis in the amygdala for reconsolidation after retrieval. Nature 406, 722726.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Parsons, RG, Gafford, GM and Helmstetter, FJ (2006) Translational control via the mammalian target of rapamycin pathway is critical for the formation and stability of long-term fear memory in amygdala neurons. Journal of Neuroscience 26, 1297712983.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Patton, JH and Stanford, MS (1995) Factor structure of the Barratt impulsiveness scale. Journal of Clinical Psychology 51, 768774.3.0.CO;2-1>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Przybyslawski, J and Sara, SJ (1997) Reconsolidation of memory after its reactivation. Behavioural Brain Research 84, 241246.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roesler, R (2017) Molecular mechanisms controlling protein synthesis in memory reconsolidation. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 142, 3040.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rolls, ET (2011) Taste, olfactory and food texture reward processing in the brain and obesity. International Journal of Obesity 35, 550561.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rozin, P, Levine, E and Stoess, C (1991) Chocolate craving and liking. Appetite 17, 199212.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schulte, EM, Avena, NM and Gearhardt, AN (2015) Which foods may be addictive? the roles of processing, fat content, and glycemic load. PLOS ONE 10, e0117959.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shi, J, Jun, W, Zhao, LY, Xue, YX, Zhang, XY, Kosten, TR and Lu, L (2009) Effect of rapamycin on cue-induced drug craving in abstinent heroin addicts. European Journal of Pharmacology 615, 108112.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spielberger, CD, Gorsuch, RL and Lushene, RE (1970) Manual for the state-trait anxiety inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.Google Scholar
Stice, E, Figlewicz, DP, Gosnell, BA, Levine, AS and Pratt, WE (2013) The contribution of brain reward circuits to the obesity epidemic. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 37, 20472058.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stunkard, AJ and Messick, S (1985) The three-factor eating questionnaire to measure dietary restraint, disinhibition and hunger. Journal of Psychosomatic Research 29, 7183.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Suzuki, A, Josselyn, SA, Frankland, PW, Masushige, S, Silva, AJ and Kida, S (2004) Memory reconsolidation and extinction have distinct temporal and biochemical signatures. The Journal of Neuroscience 24, 47874795.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tremmel, M, Gerdtham, UG, Nilsson, PM and Saha, S (2017) Economic burden of obesity: a systematic literature review. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 14, 118.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tylka, TL and Van Diest, AM (2013) The Intuitive Eating Scale-2: Item refinement and psychometric evaluation with college women and men. Journal of Counseling Psychology 60, 137153.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ulijaszek, SJ (2007) Obesity: a disorder of convenience. Obesity Reviews 9(s1), 183187.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Valjent, E, Corbillé, A-G, Bertran-Gonzalez, J, Hervé, D and Girault, J-A (2006) Inhibition of ERK pathway or protein synthesis during reexposure to drugs of abuse erases previously learned place preference. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 103, 29322937.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Strien, T, Peter Herman, C, Engels, RCME, Larsen, JK and van Leeuwe, JFJ (2007) Construct validation of the Restraint Scale in normal-weight and overweight females. Appetite 49, 109121.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Volkow, ND, Wise, RA and Baler, R (2017) The dopamine motive system: implications for drug and food addiction. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 18, 741752.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wang, G-J, Volkow, ND and Fowler, JS (2002) The role of dopamine in motivation for food in humans: implications for obesity. Expert Opinion on Therapeutic Targets 6, 601609.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
WHO (2016) Obesity and Overweight: Fact Sheet. WHO Media Centre. https://www.who.int/news-room/fact-sheets/detail/obesity-and-overweight.Google Scholar
Supplementary material: File

Walsh et al. supplementary material

Walsh et al. supplementary material

Download Walsh et al. supplementary material(File)
File 20.8 KB