Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-grxwn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-01-07T18:31:11.366Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

A Markov Model for Discrimination Learning

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 January 2025

Richard C. Atkinson*
Affiliation:
University of California, Los Angeles

Abstract

A theory for discrimination learning which incorporates the concept of an observing response is presented. The theory is developed in detail for experimental procedures in which two stimuli are employed and two responses are available to the subject. Applications of the model to cases involving probabilistic and nonprobabilistic schedules of reinforcement are considered; some predictions are derived and compared with experimental results.

Type
Original Paper
Copyright
Copyright © 1958 The Psychometric Society

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

*

This research was supported by a grant from the National Science Foundation.

References

Atkinson, R. C. A stochastic model for rote serial learning. Psychometrika, 1957, 22, 8796.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Atkinson, R. C. and Suppes, P. An analysis of a two-person game situation in terms of statistical learning theory. J. exp. Psychol., 1958, 55, 369378.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Atkinson, R. C. and Suppes, P. An analysis of non-zero sum games in terms of a Markov model for learning. Tech. Rep. No. 9, Contract NR 171-034, Appl. Math. and Statist. Lab., Stanford Univ., 1957.Google Scholar
Bartlett, M. S. An introduction to stochastic processes, Cambridge: Cambridge Univ. Press, 1955.Google Scholar
Burke, C. J. and Estes, W. K. A component model for stimulus variables in discrimination learning. Psychometrika, 1957, 22, 133145.Google Scholar
Bush, R. R. and Mosteller, F. A model for stimulus generalization and discrimination. Psychol. Rev., 1951, 58, 413423.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bush, R. R. and Mosteller, F. Stochastic models for learning, New York: Wiley, 1955.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Estes, W. K. and Burke, C. J. A theory of stimulus variability in learning. Psychol. Rev., 1953, 60, 276286.Google ScholarPubMed
Estes, W. K. and Burke, C. J. Application of a statistical model to simple discrimination learning in human subjects. J. exp. Psychol., 1955, 50, 8188.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Estes, W. K., Burke, C. J., Atkinson, R. C., and Frankmann, J. P.. Probabilistic discrimination learning. J. exp. Psychol., 1957, 54, 233239.Google ScholarPubMed
Feller, W. An introduction to probability and its applications, New York: Wiley, 1950.Google Scholar
Fréchet, M. Recherches théoriques modernes sur le calcul des probabilités, Vol. 2, Paris: Gauthier-Villars, 1938.Google Scholar
Kemeny, J. G. and Snell, J. L. Markov processes in learning theory. Psychometrika, 1957, 22, 221230.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Popper, J. and Atkinson, R. C. Discrimination learning in a verbal conditioning situation. J. exp. Psychol., 1958, 56, 2125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Restle, F. A theory of selective learning with probable reinforcements. Psychol. Rev., 1957, 64, 182191.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wyckoff, L. B. Jr. The role of observing responses in discrimination behavior. Psychol. Rev., 1952, 59, 437442.CrossRefGoogle Scholar