Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-j824f Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-14T22:25:45.818Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Thought without Language: Thought without Awareness?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  12 April 2010

Extract

Some philosophers have laid down rather severe strictures on whether there can be thought without language. Wittgenstein asserted that ‘the limits of language…mean the limits of my world’ (1922, §5.62). Davidson (1984, p. 157) has argued that ‘a creature cannot have thoughts unless it is an interpreter of the speech of another’. Dummett (1978, p. 458) has interpreted some pronouncements as meaning that ‘the study of thought is to be sharply distinguished from the study of the psychological processes of thinking and…the only proper method of analysing thought consists in the analysis of language’. And there is also the position that thought has its own language that might exist even prior to or in the absence of natural language. But here I am going to concentrate on what might be possible in the absence of natural language. I do not know what it would mean to consider thinking in the absence of its own intrinsic language, a language of thought, if the two always co-exist.

Type
Papers
Copyright
Copyright © The Royal Institute of Philosophy and the contributors 1997

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Barbur, J. L., Harlow, J. A., Sahraie, A., Stoerig, P. and Weiskrantz, L. 1994. ‘Responses to Chromatic Stimuli in the Absence of VI: Pupillometric and Psychophysical Studies’, Optical Society of America Technical Digest 2, 312–15Google Scholar
Bisiach, E. 1988. ‘Language Without Thought’, in , Weiskrantz (ed.) (1988)Google Scholar
Blumstein, S., Milberg, W. and Shrier, R. 1982. ‘Semantic Processing in Aphasia: Evidence from an Auditory Lexical Decision Task’, Brain and Language 17, 301–15CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brent, P. J., Kennard, C. and Ruddock, K. H. 1994. ‘Residual Colour Vision in a Human Hemianope: Spectral Responses and Colour-Discrimination’, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B 256, 219–25Google Scholar
Butterworth, G. and Grover, L. 1988. ‘The Origins of Referential Communication in Human Infancy’, in , Weiskrantz (ed.) (1988)Google Scholar
Chapuis, N. and Varlet, C. 1987. ‘Shortcut by Dogs in Natural Surroundings’, Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 39B, 4964Google Scholar
Davidson, D. 1984. Inquiries into Truth and Interpretation. Oxford: Clarendon PressGoogle Scholar
Diamond, A. 1988. ‘Differences between Adult and Infant Cognition: Is the Crucial Variable Presence or Absence of Language?’ in , Weiskrantz (ed.) (1988)Google Scholar
Dummett, M. A. E. 1978. Truth and Other Enigmas. London: DuckworthGoogle Scholar
Frederici, A. D. 1982. ‘Syntactic and Semantic Processes in Aphasic Deficits: the Availability of Prepositions’, Brain and Language 15, 245–58Google Scholar
Gazzaniga, M. S. 1988. ‘The Dynamics of Cerebral Specialization and Modular Interactions’, in , Weiskrantz (ed.) (1988)Google Scholar
Hadamard, J. 1945. An Essay on the Psychology of Invention in the Mathematical Field. Princeton University PressGoogle Scholar
Herrnstein, R. J. 1984. ‘Objects, Categories, and Discriminative Stimuli’, in Roitblat, H. L., Bever, T. G. and Terrace, H. S. (eds.),Animal Cognition. Hillsdale, NJ: ErlbaumGoogle Scholar
Herrnstein, R. J. 1985. ‘Riddles of Natural Categorization’, in , Weiskrantz (ed.) (1985)Google Scholar
Hovland, C. I. 1951. ‘Human Learning and Retention’, in Stevens, S. S. (ed.), Handbook of Experimental Psychology. New York: WileyGoogle Scholar
Kertesz, A. 1988. ‘Cognitive Function in Severe Aphasia’, in , Weiskrantz (ed.) (1988)Google Scholar
Kellman, P. J. and Spelke, E. S. 1983. ‘Perception of Partly-Occluded Objects in Infancy’, Cognitive Psychology 15, 483524CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leslie, A. L. 1988. ‘The Necessity of Illusion: Perception and Thought in Infancy, in , Weiskrantz (ed.) (1988)Google Scholar
Leslie, A. L. and Keeble, S. 1987. ‘Do Six-month-old Infants Perceive Causality?Cognition 25, 265–88CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McAndrews, M. P., Glisky, E. L. and Schacter, D. L. 1987. ‘When Priming Persists: Long-lasting Implicit Memory for a Single Episode in Amnesic Patients’, Neuropsychologia 25, 497506CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mackintosh, N. J., Wilson, B. and Boakes, R. A. 1985. ‘Differences in Mechanisms of Intelligence among Vertebrates’, in , Weiskrantz (ed.) (1985)Google Scholar
Marcel, A. J. (forthcoming). ‘Blindsight and Shape Perception: Deficit of Visual Consciousness or of Visual Function?’, BrainGoogle Scholar
Marshall, J. and Halligan, P. 1988. ‘Blindsight and Insight in Visuo-spatial Neglect’, Nature 336, 766–77CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Milberg, W. and Blumstein, S. E. 1981. ‘Lexical Decision and Aphasia: Evidence for Semantic Processing’, Brain and Language 14, 371–85CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Newcombe, F. 1987. ‘Psychometric and Behavioural Evidence: Scope, Limitations, and Ecological Validity’, in Levin, H. S., Grafman, J. and Eisenberg, H. M. (eds.), Neurobehavioral Recovery from Head Injury. Oxford University PressGoogle Scholar
Olton, D. S. 1979. ‘Mazes, Maps, and Memory’, American Psychologist 34, 583–96CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Piaget, J. 1951. Play, Dreams and Imitation in Childhood. London: William HeinemannGoogle Scholar
Premack, D. 1988. ‘Minds With and Without Language’, in , Weiskrantz (ed.) (1985)Google Scholar
Povinelli, D. J., Eddy, T. J. 1996. ‘Chimpanzees. Joint Visual Attention’, Psychological Science 7, 129–35CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosenthal, D. 1993. ‘Thinking that one Thinks’, in Davies, M. and Humphreys, G. W. (eds.), Consciousness. Psychological and Philosophical Essays. Oxford: BlackwellGoogle Scholar
Rundle, B. 1995. ‘Thinking’, in Honderich, T. (ed.), The Oxford Companion to Philosophy. Oxford University PressGoogle Scholar
Spelke, E. S. 1988. ‘The Origins of Physical Knowledge’, in , Weiskrantz (ed.) (1988)Google Scholar
Sperry, R. W. 1974. ‘Lateral Specialization in the Surgically-Separated Hemispheres’, in Schmitt, F. O. and Worden, F. G. (eds.), The Neurosciences: Third Study Program. Cambridge, MA: MIT PressGoogle Scholar
Stoerig, P. and Cowey, A. 1992. ‘Wavelength-Sensitivity in Blindsight’, Brain 115, 425–44CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Terrace, H. S. 1985. ‘Animal Cognition: Thinking Without Language’, in , Weiskrantz (ed.) (1985)Google Scholar
Thinus-Blanc, C. 1988. ‘Animal Spatial Cognition’, in , Weiskrantz (ed.) (1988)Google Scholar
Tolman, E. C. 1948. ‘Cognitive Maps in Rats and Men’, Psychological Review 55, 189208CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tolman, E. C. and Honzik, C. H. 1930. ‘Degrees of Hunger-Reward and Non-Reward in Maze-Learning by Rats’, University of California Publications in Psychology 4, 241–56Google Scholar
Tranel, D. and Damasio, A. R. 1985. ‘Knowledge Without Awareness: An Autonomic Index of Facial Recognition by Prosopagnosics’, Science 228, 1453–55CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tyler, L. K. 1988. ‘Spoken Language Comprehension in a Fluent Aphasic Patient’, Cognitive Neuropsychology 5, 375400CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tyler, L. K. 1992. ‘The Distinction between Implicit and Explicit Language Function: Evidence from Aphasia’, in Milner, A. D. and Rugg, M. D. (eds.), The Neuropsychology of Consciousness. London: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
Warrington, E. K. and Weiskrantz, L. 1968. ‘New Method of Testing Long-term Retention with Special Reference to Amnesic Patients’, Nature 217, 972–74CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Warrington, E. K. and Weiskrantz, L.1978. ‘Further Analysis of the Prior Learning Effect in Amnesic Patients’, Neuropsychologia 26, 169–76Google Scholar
Weiskrantz, L. 1985. ‘Introduction: Categorization, Cleverness and Consciousness’, in , Weiskrantz (ed.) (1985)Google Scholar
Weiskrantz, L. 1986. Blindsight: A Case Study and Implications. Oxford University PressGoogle Scholar
Weiskrantz, L. 1991. ‘Disconnected Awareness for Detecting, Processing, and Remembering in Neurological Patients’, The Hughlings Jackson Lecture. Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine 84, 466–70CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weiskrantz, L. 1997. Consciousness Lost and Found: A Neuropsychological Exploration. Oxford University PressGoogle Scholar
Weiskrantz, L. (ed.). 1985. Animal Intelligence. Oxford: Clarendon PressGoogle Scholar
Weiskrantz, L. 1988. Thought without Language. Oxford University PressGoogle Scholar
Weiskrantz, L. and Saunders, R. C. 1984. ‘Impairments of Visual Object Transforms in Monkeys’, Brain 107, 1033–72CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weiskrantz, L., Warrington, E. K., Sanders, M. D. and Marshall, J. 1974. ‘Visual Capacity in the Hemianopic Field following a Restricted Occipital Ablation’, Brain 97, 709–28CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wertheimer, M. 1945. Productive Thinking. New York: Harper and RowGoogle Scholar
Wittgenstein, L. 1922. Tractatus Logico-Philosophicus. London: Routledge and Kegan PaulGoogle Scholar
Young, A. W. and De Haan, E. H. F. 1992. ‘Face Recognition and Awareness after Brain Injury’, in Milner, A. D. and Rugg, M. D. (eds.), The Neuropsychology of Consciousness. London: Academic PressGoogle Scholar