Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-fscjk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-28T22:18:09.021Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Memory of Medical Scenarios for End-of-Life Support Preferences

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  23 November 2018

Elvira García-Bajos*
Affiliation:
Universidad del País Vasco (Spain)
Malen Migueles
Affiliation:
Universidad del País Vasco (Spain)
*
*Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Elvira García-Bajos. Facultad de Psicología de la Universidad del País Vasco. Avenida Tolosa 70, San Sebastián, 20018 (Spain). E-mail: elvira.garcia@ehu.es

Abstract

We studied memory of health scenarios for end-of-life support decisions and stability of life support preferences. Psychology students (n = 36, age M = 27.25, SD = 6.21, 64% females) were administered the Life Support Preferences Questionnaire to assess their memory of six medical scenarios with different prognoses, care treatment, and end-of-life support choices. Recall, recognition, confidence and decision stability were assessed immediately and one month later. Correct recall decreased and incorrect recall increased from immediate to long-term recall, F(2, 68) = 74.38, p < .001, η2p = .69. In recall, participants spontaneously gave false information consistent with prior knowledge of illnesses and medical scenarios. Participants who had suffered a disease or serious accident did worse on correct recall, F(1, 34) = 6.59, p = .015, η2p = .16, and had more errors, F(1, 34) = 4.68, p = .038, η2p = .12, than participants who had not. In the recognition test there were no differences between hits and false alarms, showing the difficulty in discriminating between true and false contents. Confidence was greater for hits than for false alarms, F(1, 34) = 10.86, p = .002, η2p = .24, but this subjective measure did not seem to be a good predictor of accuracy because confidence was greater than the mean value for hits (p = .001, d = 1.74) and for false alarms (p = .001, d = 0.96). Long-term memory was quite poor and biased, but life support preferences did not change much.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Universidad Complutense de Madrid and Colegio Oficial de Psicólogos de Madrid 2018 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

This work was supported by the Spanish Ministerio de la Economía y la Competitividad (MINECO) under Grant PSI2015-63709-P (MINECO/FEDER, EU) and the Universidad del País Vasco under Grant GIU15/02

How to cite:

García-Bajos, E., & Migueles, M. (2018). Memory of medical scenarios for end-of-life support preferences. The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 21. e56. Doi:10.1017/sjp.2018.60

References

Allmark, P. (2002). Death with dignity. Journal of Medical Ethics, 28, 255257. https://doi.org/10.1136/jme.28.4.255CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Auriemma, C. L., Nguyen, C. A., Bronheim, R., Kent, S., Nadiger, S., Pardo, D., & Halpern, S. D. (2014). Stability of end-of-life preferences: A systematic review of the evidence. JAMA Internal Medicine, 174, 10851092. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamainternmed.2014.1183CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barrio-Cantalejo, I. M., Bailón-Gómez, R. M., Cámara-Medina, M. C., Carmona-Cabezas, M. A., Martínez Cruz, M. E., & Quesada-Lupiáñez, P. (2008). Validación del cuestionario de preferencias de soporte vital (LSPQ) para su uso en la población española [Validation of the life-support preferences questionnaire (LSPQ) for its use in Spain]. Atención Primaria, 40, 345349. https://doi.org/10.1157/13124127CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barrio-Cantalejo, I. M., Simón-Lorda, P., Molina-Ruiz, A., Herrera-Ramos, F., Martínez-Cruz, E., Bailon-Gómez, R. M., … Peinado-Gorlat, P. (2013). Stability over time in the preferences of older persons for life-sustaining treatment. Journal of Bioethical Inquiry, 10, 103114. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11673-012-9417-4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Brown, G. K. (1996). Manual for the Beck Depression Inventory –II. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation. [Spanish transl. Inventario de depresión de Beck (2nd Ed.). Manual. Buenos Aires: Paidós; 2006].Google Scholar
Beland, D. K., & Froman, R. D. (1995). Preliminary validation of a measure of life support preferences. Journal of Nursing Scholarship, 27, 307310. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1547-5069.1995.tb00893.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berntsen, D. (2002). Tunnel memories for autobiographical events: Central details are remembered more frequently from shocking than for happy experiences. Memory & Cognition, 30, 10101020. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03194319CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bookwala, J., Coppola, K. M., Fagerlin, A., Ditto, P. H., Danks, J. H., & Smucker, W. D. (2001). Gender differences in older adults’ preferences for life-sustaining medical treatments and end-of-life values. Death Studies, 25, 127149. https://doi.org/10.1080/07481180126202CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, A. D., Addis, D. R., Romano, T. A., Marmar, C. R., Bryant, R. A., Hirst, W., & Schacter, D. L. (2014). Episodic and semantic components of autobiographical memories and imagined future events in post-traumatic stress disorder. Memory, 22, 595604. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2013.807842CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, S. C., & Park, D. C. (2002). Role of age and familiarity in learning health information. Educational Gerontology, 28, 695710. https://doi.org/10.1080/03601270290099723CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chirlaque, M. D., Salmerón, D., Ardanaz, E., Galcerán, J., Martínez, R., Marcos-Grágera, R., … Navarro, C. (2010). Cancer survival in Spain: Estimate for nine major cancers. Annals of Oncology, 21, iii21iii29. https://doi.org/10.1093/annonc/mdq082CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, G., & Java, R. (1995). Memory for medical history: Accuracy of recall. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 9, 273288. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.2350090402CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Conway, M. A., Singer, J. A., & Tagini, A. (2004). The self and autobiographical memory: Correspondence and coherence. Social Cognition, 22, 491529. https://doi.org/10.1521/soco.22.5.491.50768CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Craik, F. I., & McDowd, J. M. (1987). Age differences in recall and recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 13, 474479. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.13.3.474Google Scholar
D’Argembeau, A., & van der Linden, M. (2004). Phenomenal characteristics associated with projecting oneself back into the past and forward into the future: Influence of valence and temporal distance. Consciousness and Cognition, 13, 844858. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2004.07.007CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Denton, M., Prus, S., & Walters, V. (2004). Gender differences in health: A Canadian study of the psychosocial, structural and behavioral determinants of health. Social Science & Medicine, 58, 25852600. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2003.09.008CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ditto, P. H., Danks, J. H., Smucker, W. D., Bookwala, J., Coppola, K. M., Dresser, R., … Zyzanski, S. (2001). Advance directives as acts of communication: a randomized controlled trial. Archives of Internal Medicine, 161, 421430. http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/archinte.161.3.421CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ditto, P. H., Hawkins, N. A., & Pizarro, D. A. (2005). Imagining the end of life: On the psychology of advance medical decision making. Motivation and Emotion, 29, 475496. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-006-9017-xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ditto, P. H., Smucker, W. D., Danks, J. H., Jacobson, J. A., Houts, R. M., Fagerlin, A., … Gready, R. M. (2003). Stability of older adults’ preferences for life-sustaining medical treatment. Health Psychology, 22, 605615. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-6133.22.6.605CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Donaldson, W. (1992). Measuring recognition memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 121, 275277. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.121.3.275CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 175191. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03193146CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
García-Bajos, E., & Migueles, M. (2013). An integrative study of autobiographical memory for positive and negative experiences. Spanish Journal of Psychology, 16, 111. https://doi.org/10.1017/sjp.2013.103CrossRefGoogle Scholar
García-Bajos, E., & Migueles, M. (2017). Retrieval of past and future positive and negative autobiographical experiences. Cognition and Emotion, 31, 12601267. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2016.1204988CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
García-Bajos, E., Migueles, M., & Aizpurua, A. (2017). Age-based positivity effects in imagining and recalling future positive and negative autobiographical events. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1700. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01700CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Godwin, Y. (2000). Do they listen? A review of information retained by patients following consent for reduction mammoplasty. British Journal of Plastic Surgery, 53, 121125. https://doi.org/10.1054/bjps.1999.3220Google ScholarPubMed
Gready, R. M., Ditto, P. H., Danks, J. H., Coppola, K. M., Lockhart, L. K., & Smucker, W. D. (2000). Actual and perceived stability of preferences for life-sustaining treatment. Journal of Clinical Ethics, 11, 334346.Google ScholarPubMed
Johnson, M. K. (1997). Source monitoring and memory distortion. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 352, 17331745. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.1997.0156CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kessels, R. P. (2003). Patients’ memory for medical information. Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine, 96, 219222. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/014107680309600504Google ScholarPubMed
Ley, P. (1979). Memory for medical information. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 18, 245255. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8260.1979.tb00333.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McGuire, L. C. (1996). Remembering what the doctor said: Organization and adults’ memory for medical information. Experimental Aging Research, 22, 403428. https://doi.org/10.1080/03610739608254020CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Migueles, M., & García-Bajos, E. (2012). The power of script knowledge and selective retrieval in the recall of daily activities. The Journal of General Psychology, 139, 100113. https://doi.org/10.1080/00221309.2012.663817CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Onion, C. W. R., & Slade, P. D. (1995). Depth of information processing and memory for medical facts. Medical Teacher, 17, 307313. https://doi.org/10.3109/01421599509008321CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ruttley, A., & Reid, S. (2006). Depression in physical illness. Psychiatry, 5, 8992. https://doi.org/10.1383/psyt.2006.5.3.89CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schacter, D. L. (2012). Constructive memory: Past and future. Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience, 14, 718.Google ScholarPubMed
Sharman, S. J. (2011). Current negative mood encourages changes in end-of-life treatment decisions and is associated with false memories. Cognition and Emotion, 25, 132139. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931003612064CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sharman, S. J., Garry, M., Jacobson, J. A., Loftus, E. F., & Ditto, P. H. (2008). False memories for end-of-life decisions. Health Psychology, 27, 291296. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-6133.27.2.291CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Snodgrass, J. G., & Corwin, J. (1988). Pragmatics of measuring recognition memory: Applications to dementia and amnesia. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 117, 3450. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.117.1.34CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stenzel, N. M., Vaske, I., Kühl, K., Kenn, K., & Rief, W. (2015). Prediction of end-of-life fears in COPD–hoping for the best but preparing for the worst. Psychology & Health, 30, 10171034. https://doi.org/10.1080/08870446.2015.1014816CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Szpunar, K. K., Addis, D. R., & Schacter, D. L. (2012). Memory for emotional simulations: Remembering a rosy future. Psychological Science, 23, 2429. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797611422237CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Templer, D. I. (1970). The construction and validation of a Death Anxiety Scale. The Journal of General Psychology, 82, 165177. https://doi.org/10.1080/00221309.1970.9920634CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van Wijmen, M. P. S., Pasman, H. R. W., Widdershoven, G. A., & Onwuteaka-Philipsen, B. D. (2014). Motivations, aims and communication around advance directives: A mixed-methods study into the perspective of their owners and the influence of a current illness. Patient Education and Counseling, 95, 393399. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pec.2014.03.009CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Walker, W. R., & Skowronski, J. J. (2009). The Fading affect bias: But what the hell is it for? Applied Cognitive Psychology, 23, 11221136. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.1614CrossRefGoogle Scholar