Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gbm5v Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-28T15:16:02.613Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Wolfgang Köhler’s the Mentality of Apes and the Animal Psychology of his Time

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 October 2014

Gabriel Ruiz*
Affiliation:
Universidad de Sevilla (Spain)
Natividad Sánchez
Affiliation:
Universidad de Sevilla (Spain)
*
*Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Gabriel Ruiz. Departamento de Psicología Experimental. Universidad de Sevilla. C/ Camilo José Cela S/N. 41018. Sevilla (Spain). E-mail: gruiz@us.es

Abstract

In 1913, the Anthropoid Station for psychological and physiological research in chimpanzees and other apes was founded by the Royal Prussian Academy of Sciences (Berlin) near La Orotava, Tenerife. Eugene Teuber, its first director, began his work at the Station with several studies of anthropoid apes’ natural behavior, particularly chimpanzee body language. In late 1913, the psychologist Wolfgang Köhler, the second and final director of the Station, arrived in Tenerife. During his stay in the Canary Islands, Köhler conducted a series of studies on intelligent behavior in chimpanzees that would become classics in the field of comparative psychology. Those experiments were at the core of his book Intelligenzprüfungen an Menschenaffen (The Mentality of Apes), published in 1921. This paper analyzes Köhler’s experiments and notions of intelligent behavior in chimpanzees, emphasizing his distinctly descriptive approach to these issues. It also makes an effort to elucidate some of the theoretical ideas underpinning Köhler’s work. The ultimate goal of this paper is to assess the historical significance of Köhler’s book within the context of the animal psychology of his time.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Universidad Complutense de Madrid and Colegio Oficial de Psicólogos de Madrid 2014 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ash, M. (1995). Gestalt psychology in German culture 1890–1967: Holism and the quest for objectivity. Cambridge and New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Berry, C. S. (1908). An experimental study of imitation in cats. Journal of Comparative Neurology and Psychology, 18, 125. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/cne.920180102 Google Scholar
Bischof, N. (1978). On the phylogeny of human morality. In Stent, G. (Ed.), Morality as a biological phenomenon (pp. 5374). Berlin, Germany: Abakon.Google Scholar
Bischof-Kohler, D. (1985). Zur phylogenese menschticher motivation [On the phylogeny of human motivation]. In Eckensberger, L. H. & Lantermann, E. D. (Eds.), Emotion und reflexivitat [Emotion and reflexivity] (pp. 347). Vienna, Austria: Urban & Schwarzenberg.Google Scholar
Boakes, R. A. (1984). From Darwin to behaviourism: Psychology and the animal mind. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press (Spanish translation published by Alianza Editoral, 1989).Google Scholar
Boutan, L. (1914). Les deux méthodes de l'enfant [The two methods of child]. Bordeaux, France: A. Saugnac et Cie.Google Scholar
Cacchione, T., & Krist, H. (2004) Recognizing impossible object relations: Intuitions about support in chimpanzees (pan troglodytes). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 118, 140148. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0735-7036.118.2.140 Google Scholar
Clayton, N. S., Bussey, T. J., & Dickinson, A. (2003). Can animals recall the past and plan for the future? Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4, 685691. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nrn1180 Google Scholar
Crawford, M. P., & Spence, K. W. (1939). Observational learning of discrimination problems by chimpanzees. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 27, 133147. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0061696 Google Scholar
Darwin, C. (1871). The descent of man, and selection in relation to sex. London, UK: John Murray (Spanish edition by E.D.A.F., 1989).Google Scholar
de Waal, F. (1993). La política de los chimpancés. El poder y el sexo entre los simios [Chimpanzee politics. Power and sex among simians]. Madrid, Spain: Alianza.Google Scholar
Dewsbury, D. A. (2000). Comparative cognition in the 1930s. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 7, 267283. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03212982 Google Scholar
Dewsbury, D. A. (2006). Monkey farm: A history of the Yerkes Laboratories of Primate Biology, Orange Park, Florida, 1930–1965. Lewisburg, PA: Bucknell University Press.Google Scholar
Gabucio, G. (1989). Las investigaciones de Norman R. F. Maier sobre la solución de problemas y la psicología de la Gestalt [Norman R. F. Maier’s research on problem-solving and Gestalt psychology]. In Quintana, J., Rosa, A., & Lafuente, E. (Eds), Psicología e historia, contribuciones a la investigación en historia de la psicología [Psychology and history, contributions to research on the history of psychology] (pp. 251258). Madrid, Spain: UAM.Google Scholar
Gallup, G. G. Jr. (1970). Chimpanzees: Self-recognition. Science, 167, 8687. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.167.3914.86 Google Scholar
Gondra, J. M. (2007 ). Mecanismos asociativos del pensamiento. La “obra magna” inacabada de Clark L. Hull [Associative mechanisms of thought. The unfinished “magnum opus” of Clark L. Hull]. Bilbao, Spain: Desclée de Brouwer.Google Scholar
Goodall, J. (1964). Tool-using and aimed throwing in a community of free-living chimpanzees. Nature, 201, 12641266. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/2011264a0 Google Scholar
Gulliksen, H., & Wolfle, D. L. (1938). A theory of learning and transfer. Psychometrika, 3, 127149. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF02288482 Google Scholar
Haggerty, M. E. (1909a). Imitation in monkeys. Journal of Comparative Neurology and Psychology, 19, 337455. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/cne.920190402 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haggerty, M. E. (1909b). Imitation in monkeys. Century Magazine, 78, 544555.Google Scholar
Haggerty, M. E. (1913). Plumbing the mind of apes. McClure’s Magazine, 41, 151154.Google Scholar
Henle, Mary. (1978). One man against the Nazis – Wolfgang Köhler. American Psychologist, 33, 939944.Google Scholar
Hernández, J. M. (n.d.) (a). La fundación del primer centro de investigaciones primatológicas del mundo: Wolfgang Köhler y la Casa Amarilla [The founding of the world’s first center for primate research: Wolfgang Köhler and the Yellow House]. Tenerife, Spain: Bubok.Google Scholar
Hernández, J. M. (n.d.) (b). El Ciprés, hacienda y camino, en La Orotava [Cypress, the property and location, in Orotava]. Tenerife, Spain: Bubok.Google Scholar
Hobhouse, L. T. (1901). Mind in evolution. London, UK: MacMillan and Co.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Honig, W. K. (1962). Prediction of preference, transposition, and transposition-reversal from the generalization gradient. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64, 239248. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0048290 Google Scholar
Hood, B. M., Hauser, M. D., Anderson, L., & Santos, L. (1999). Gravity biases in a non-human primate? Developmental Science, 2, 3541. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-7687.00051 Google Scholar
Hull, C. L. (1952a). Clark Leonard Hull. In Boring, E. G. (1952), A history of psychology in autobiography (Vol. 4. pp. 143162). Worcester, UK: Clark University Press.Google Scholar
Hull, C. L. (1952b). A behavior system. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Jaeger, S. (Ed.) (1988). Briefe von Wolfgang Köhler una Geitel Hans 1907–1920, mit zwei Arbeiten Köhlers, “Über Elektromagnetische Erregung des Trommelfelles” und “Intelligenzprüfungen am Orang” im Anhang. Passau, West Germany: Passavia Universitätsverlag.Google Scholar
Johnson, R. C., & Zara, R. C. (1960). Relational learning in young children. Journal of Comparative & Physiological Psychology, 53, 594597. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0042545 Google Scholar
Kendler, T. S. (1950). An experimental investigation of transposition as a function of difference between training and test stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 40, 552562. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0061286 Google Scholar
King, D. B., & Wertheimer, M. (2004). Max Wertheimer and Gestalt psychology. New Jersey, NJ: Transaction Publishers.Google Scholar
Kinnaman, A. J. (1902a). Mental life of two Macacus rhesus monkeys in captivity. – I. American Journal of Psychology, 13, 98148. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1412207 Google Scholar
Kinnaman, A. J. (1902b). Mental life of two Macacus rhesus monkeys in captivity. – II. American Journal of Psychology, 13, 173218.Google Scholar
Köhler, W. (1917). Aus der Anthropoidenstation auf Teneriffa.III. Intelligenzprüfungen an anthropoiden I. Abhandlungen der Preussische Akademie der Wissenschaften, 1213.Google Scholar
Köhler, W. (1918). Aus der Anthropoidenstation auf Teneriffa.IV. Nachweis einfacher strukturfunktionen beim schimpansen und beim haushuhn. Über eine neue methode zur untersuchung des bunten farbensystems. Berlin, Alemania: Verlag der Königlichen Akademie der Wissenschaften.Google Scholar
Köhler, W. (1921). Intelligenzprüfungen an menschenaffen. Berlin, Alemania: Springer-Verlag (Spanish translation published by Editorial Debate, 1989).Google Scholar
Köhler, W. (1925). The mentality of Apes. New York, NY: Harcourt, Brace & Company Inc.Google Scholar
Köhler, W. (1929). Gestalt psychology. New York, NY: Liveright.Google Scholar
Köhler, W. (1938). The place of value in a world of facts. New York, NY: Liveright.Google Scholar
Köhler, W. (1940). Dynamics in psychology. New York, NY: Liveright.Google Scholar
Köhler, W. (1947). Gestalt psychology: An introduction to new concepts in modern psychology. New York, NY: Liveright.Google Scholar
Köhler, W. (1967). Gestalt psychology. In Benjamin, L. T. Jr. (Ed.) (1988), A history of psychology: Original sources and contemporary research (pp. 520527). New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Köhler, W. (1971). The scientists from Europe and their new environment. In Henle, M. (Ed.). The selected papers of Wolfgang Köhler (pp. 413435). New York, NY: Liveright.Google Scholar
Köhler, W. (1993). The mentality of orangs. International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 6, 189229.Google Scholar
Kressley-Mba, R. A. (2006). On the failed institutionalization of German comparative psychology prior to 1940. History of Psychology, 9, 5574. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1093-4510.9.1.55 Google Scholar
Lawrence, D. H., & DeRivera, J. (1954). Evidence for relational transposition. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 47, 465471. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0063235 Google ScholarPubMed
Lazarera, O. F., Miner, M., Wasserman, E. A., & Young, M. E. (2008). Multiple-pair training enhances transposition in pigeons. Learning & Behavior, 36, 174187.Google Scholar
Lazarera, O. F., Wasserman, E. A., & Young, M. E. (2005). Transposition in pigeons: Reassessing Spence (1937) with multiple discrimination training. Learning & Behavior, 33, 2246.Google Scholar
Ley, R. (1990). A whisper of espionage. Wolfgang Köhler and the apes of Tenerife. Garden City Park, NY: Avery Pub. Group.Google Scholar
Lück, H., & Jaeger, S. (1988). Wolfgang Köhler and the anthropoid research station on Tenerife Island. Revista de Historia de la Psicología, 9, 295308.Google Scholar
Maier, N. R. F. (1931). Reasoning and learning. Psychological Review, 38, 332346. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0069991 Google Scholar
Maier, N. R. F. (929l). Reasoning in white rats. Comparative Psychology Monographs, 6, 193.Google Scholar
Mas, M., & Hernández, J. (2005). Los monos de Tenerife. El primer laboratorio primatológico [The monkeys of Tenerife. The first primatology laboratory]. Santa Cruz de Tenerife, Spain: Ediciones Idea.Google Scholar
Needham, A., & Baillargeon, R. (1993). Intuitions about support in 4.5-month-old infants. Cognition, 47, 121148. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(93)90002-D Google Scholar
Pavlov, I. P. (1955). Selected works. Moscow, Rusia: Foreign Languages Publishing House.Google Scholar
Paxton, R., & Hampton, R. R. (2009). Tests of planning and the Bischof-Köhler hypothesis in rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). Behavioral Processes, 80, 238246.Google Scholar
Pearce, J. M. (1994). Similarity and discrimination: A selective review and a connectionist model. Psychological Review, 101, 587607. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0033-295X.101.4.587 Google Scholar
Pfungst, O. (1911). Clever Hans (the horse of Mr. von Osten): A contribution to experimental animal and human psychology (Trans. C. L. Rahn; originally pubished in German in 1907). New York, NY: Henry Holt.Google Scholar
Pfungst, O. (1912). Psychology der affen. In Schumann, F. (Ed.), Bericht über 5 Kongress für Experimentellen Psychologie (pp. 200205). Leipzig, Alemania: Barth.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1936). Le naissance de l’intelligence chez l’enfant [The origins of intelligence in children]. Neuchâtel, Suiza: Delachaux et Niestlé (Spanish translation published in Madrid, 1969, Aguilar Ed.) Google Scholar
Povinelli, D. J. (2000). Folk physics for apes: The chimpanzee’s theory of how the world works. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Riba, C. (1992). La comunicación animal. Un enfoque zoosemiótico [Animal communication. A zoosemiotic approach]. Barcelona, Spain: Anthropos.Google Scholar
Riley, D. A. (1958). The nature of effective stimuli in animal discrimination learning: Transposition reconsidered. Psychological Review, 65, 17. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0049060 Google Scholar
Roberts, W. A. (2002). Are animals stuck in time? Psychological Bulletin, 128, 473489. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0033-2909.128.3.473 Google Scholar
Romanes, G. J. (1888). Animal intelligence. New York, NY: Appleton & Co.Google Scholar
Rothmann, M. (1912). Über die errichtung einer Satation zur psychologischen und hirnphysiologischen erforschung der menschenaffen. Berliner Klinische Wochenschrift, 42, 19811985.Google Scholar
Sabater Pi, J. (1978). El chimpancé y los orígenes de la cultura [Chimpanzees and the roots of culture]. Barcelona, Spain: Anthropos.Google Scholar
Sokal, M. M. (1984). The Gestalt psychologists in behaviorist America. The American Historical Review, 89, 12401263. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1867042 Google Scholar
Sokal, M. M. (1994). Gestalt psychology in America in the 1920s and 1930s. In Poggi, S., (Ed.), Gestalt psychology: Its origins, foundations and influence: An international workshop (pp. 87119). Firenze, Italy: Leo S. Olschki.Google Scholar
Sokolowsky, A. (1908). Beobachtungen über die psyche der menschenaffen. Frankfurt, Alemania: NFV.Google Scholar
Spence, K. W. (1934). Visual acuity and its relation to brightness in chimpanzees and man. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 18, 333361. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0075291 Google Scholar
Spence, K. W. (1936). The nature of discrimination learning in animals. Psychological Review, 43, 427449. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0056975 Google Scholar
Spence, K. W. (1937). The differential response in animals to stimuli varying within a single dimension. Psychological Review, 44, 430444. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0062885 Google Scholar
Spence, K. W. (1939). The solution of multiple choice problems by chimpanzees. Comparative Psychology Monographs, 15.Google Scholar
Spence, K. W., & Yerkes, R. M. (1937). Weight, growth and age in chimpanzee. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 22, 263279. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ajpa.1330220204 Google Scholar
Teuber, M. L. (1994). The founding of the primate station, Tenerife, Canary Islands. American Journal of Psychology, 107, 551581. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1423000 Google Scholar
Thorndike, E. L. (1898). Animal intelligence: An experimental study of the associative processes in animals. Psychological review, monograph supplements, New York, NY: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Thorndike, E. L. (1901). The mental life of the monkeys. Psychological Monograph, 3, i57. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0092994 Google Scholar
Tolman, E. C. (1925). Behaviorism and purpose. Journal of Philosophy, 22, 3641. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/2015202 Google Scholar
Tolman, E. C. (1932a). Lewin’s concept of vectors. Journal of General Psychology, 7, 315.Google Scholar
Tolman, E. C. (1932b). Purposive behavior in animals and men. New York, NY: Century.Google Scholar
Tolman, E. C. (1933). Gestalt and sign-gestalt. Psychological Review, 40, 391411. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0075737 Google Scholar
Tolman, E. C. (1948). Cognitive maps in rats and men. Psychological Review, 55, 189208. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0061626 Google Scholar
Tolman, E. C. (1952). Edward Chace Tolman. In Boring, E. G., Langfeld, H. S., Werner, H., & Yerkes, R. M. (Eds.), A history of psychology in autobiography (Vol. 4., pp. 323339). Worcester, UK: Clark University Press.Google Scholar
Trewin, S. A. (2007). Robert Yerkes’ multiple-choice apparatus, 1913–1939. The American Journal of Psychology, 120, 645660.Google Scholar
Tulving, E. (1983). Elements of episodic memory. London, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Watson, J. B. (1910). Imitation in monkeys. The Journal of Philosophy, Psychology and Scientific Methods, 7, 357359 http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/2010681 Google Scholar
Wertheimer, M. (1912). Experimentelle Studien über das Sehen Bewegung. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 61, 247250.Google Scholar
Wills, S., & Mackintosh, N. J. (1999). Relational learning in pigeons? Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 52B, 3152.Google Scholar
Windholz, G. (1984). Pavlov vs. Köhler. Pavlov's little-known primate research. Pavlovian Journal of Biological Science, 19, 2331.Google Scholar
Yerkes, R. M. (1909). Imitation among animals: Do animals imitate one another voluntarily? Century Magazine, 78, 395403.Google Scholar
Yerkes, R. M. (1914). The study of human behavior. Science, 39, 625633. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.39.1009.625 Google Scholar
Yerkes, R. M. (1916a). Provision for the study of monkeys and apes. Science, 43, 231234. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.43.1103.231 Google Scholar
Yerkes, R. M. (1916b). Ideational behavior of monkeys and apes. Psychology, 2, 639642. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2.11.639 Google Scholar
Yerkes, R. M. (1932). Robert Mearns Yerkes. Psychobiologist. In Murchison, C. (Ed.), A history of psychology in autobiography (Vol. 2., pp. 381407). Worcester, UK: Clark University Press.Google Scholar
Yerkes, R. M., & Learned, B. W. (1925). Chimpanzee intelligence and its vocal expressions. Baltimore, MD: Williams & Wilkins.Google Scholar