Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-dk4vv Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-25T20:24:43.381Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Word and Nonword Repetition Abilities in Spanish Language: Longitudinal Evidence from Typically Developing and Late Talking Children

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 December 2017

Irene Rujas*
Affiliation:
Universidad Complutense (Spain).
Sonia Mariscal
Affiliation:
Universidad Nacional de Educación a Distancia (Spain).
Marta Casla
Affiliation:
Universidad Autónoma de Madrid (Spain).
Miguel Lázaro
Affiliation:
Universidad Complutense (Spain).
Eva Murillo
Affiliation:
Universidad Autónoma de Madrid (Spain).
*
*Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Irene Rujas. Departamento de Psicología Evolutiva y de la Educación de la Universidad Complutense. 28040 Madrid (Spain). E-mail: irenerujas@gmail.com

Abstract

This longitudinal study examined the early word and nonword repetition abilities of monolingual Spanish speaking children. We explored the role that word status, word length, and time play in repetition performance of children with different vocabulary levels. We also examined the predictive value of vocabulary level in repetition abilities. Thirty-seven children participated in this study: 15 late talkers and 22 typically developing children. Families completed the Spanish version of the MacArthur Communicative Developmental Inventory (MCDI) at age 2; children performed a word and nonword repetition task at three different moments, with a temporal interval of 6 months between Time 1 and Time 2, and eight months between Time 2 and Time 3, periods during which linguistic development takes place. We found significant effects for word status, word length, vocabulary level and time: words are repeated better than nonwords; one syllable items are easier to repeat than two and three syllable ones; the performance of late talking children is lower compared to typically developing children throughout the study; and repetition abilities improve longitudinally. In addition, early vocabulary level predicts subsequent repetition abilities and early nonword repetition abilities predict future nonword repetition performance.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Universidad Complutense de Madrid and Colegio Oficial de Psicólogos de Madrid 2017 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

How to cite this article:

Rujas, I., Mariscal, S., Casla, M., Lázaro, M., & Murillo, E. (2017). Word and nonword repetition abilities in Spanish language: Longitudinal evidence from typically developing and late talking children. The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 20. e72. Doi:10.1017/sjp.2017.69

References

Aguado, G., Cuetos-Vega, F., Domezáin, M. J., & Pascual, B. (2006). Repetición de pseudopalabras en niños españoles con trastorno específico del lenguaje: Marcador psicolingüístico [Repetition of pseudo-words in Spanish children with specific language disorder: a psycholinguistic marker]. Revista de Neurología, 43(1), 201208.Google Scholar
American Psychiatric Association (APA) (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4 th ed., Rev. ed.) Washington, DC: Author.Google Scholar
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). DSM–5. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. Arlington, TX: Author.Google Scholar
Bishop, D. M. V., Holt, G., Line, E., McDonald, D., McDonald, S., & Watt, H. (2012). Parental phonological memory contributes to prediction of outcome of late talkers from 20 months to 4 years: A longitudinal study of precursors of specific language impairment. Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders, 4, 3. https://doi.org/10.1186/1866-1955-4-3 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bowey, J. A. (2001). Nonword repetition and young children’s receptive vocabulary: A longitudinal study. Applied Psycholinguistics, 22(3), 441469. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716401003083.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bosch, L. (2003). Evaluación fonológica del habla infantil [Phonological assessment of children’s speech]. Barcelona, Spain: Masson.Google Scholar
Chiat, S., & Roy, P. (2007) The Preschool Repetition Test: An evaluation of performance in typically developing and clinically referred children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50, 429443. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2007/030) Google Scholar
Coady, J. A., & Evans, J. L. (2008). Uses and interpretations of non-word repetition tasks in children with and without specific language impairments (SLI). International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 43(1), 140. https://doi.org/10.1080/13682820601116485 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Conti-Ramsden, G. (2003). Processing and linguistic markers in young children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, & Hearing Research, 46(5):10291037. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2003/082).Google Scholar
Conti-Ramsden, G., Botting, N., & Faragher, B. (2001). Psycholinguistic markers for specific language impairment (SLI). Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42, 741748. https://doi.org/10.1111/1469-7610.00770.Google Scholar
D’Odorico, L., Assanelli, A., Franco, F., & Jacob, V. (2007). A follow-up study on Italian late talkers: Development of language, short-memory, phonological awareness, impulsiveness, and attention. Applied Psycholinguistics, 28, 157169. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716407070087.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dale, P., Price, T., Bishop, D., & Plomin, R. (2003). Outcomes of early language delay: I. Predicting persistent and transient language difficulties at 3 and 4 years. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 46, 544560. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2003/044).Google Scholar
Dispaldro, M., Leonard, L. B., & Deevy, P. (2013). Real-word and nonword repetition in Italian-speaking children with specific language impairment: A study of diagnostic accuracy. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 56(1), 323336. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2012/11-0304).Google Scholar
Dollaghan, C., & Campbell, T. F. (1998). Nonword repetition and child language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 41, 11361146. https://doi.org/10.1044/jslhr.4105.1136.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ebert, K. D., Kalanek, J., Cordero, K. N., & Kohnert, K. (2008). Spanish nonword repetition stimuli development and preliminary results. Communication Disorders Quarterly, 29(2), 6774. https://doi.org/10.1177/1525740108314861 Google Scholar
Ellis Weismer, S., & Evans, J. (2002). The role of processing limitations in early identification of specific language impairment. Topics in Language Disorders, 22, 1529. https://doi.org/10.1097/00011363-200205000-00004.Google Scholar
Fenson, L., Bates, E., Dale, P., Goodman, J., Reznick, J. S., & Thal, D. (2000). Reply: Measuring variability in early child language: Don’t shoot the messenger. Child Development, 71(2), 323328. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00147.Google Scholar
Fenson, L., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., Thal, D., Bates, E., Hartung, J. P., ... Reilly, J. S. (1993). The MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory: Words and sentences. San Diego, CA: Singular.Google Scholar
Fernald, A., & Marchman, V. A. (2012). Individual differences in lexical processing at 18 months predict vocabulary growth in typically developing and late-talking toddlers. Child Development, 83(1), 203222. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01692.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frisch, S. A., Large, N. R., & Pisoni, D. B. (2000). Perception of wordlikeness: Effects of segment probability and length on the processing of nonwords. Journal of Memory and Language, 42(4), 481496. https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1999.2692.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gathercole, S. (2006). Complexities and constraints in nonword repetition and word learning. Applied Psycholinguistics, 27, 599613. https://doi.org/10.1017/S014271640606053X.Google Scholar
Gathercole, S. E., & Adams, A. M. (1993). Phonological working memory in very young children. Developmental Psychology, 29, 770778. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.29.4.770.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D. (1989). Evaluation of the role of phonological STM in the development of vocabulary in children: A longitudinal study. Journal of Memory and Language, 28(2), 200213. https://doi.org/10.1016/0749-596X(89)90044-2.Google Scholar
Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D. (1996). The Children’s Test of Nonword Repetition. London, UK: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Gathercole, S. E., Service, E., Hitch, G. J., Adams, A. M., & Martin, A. J. (1999). Phonological short term memory and vocabulary development: Further evidence on the nature of the relationship. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 13(1), 6577. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1099-0720(199902)13:1%3C65::AID-ACP548%3E3.0.CO;2-O.3.0.CO;2-O>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Girolametto, L., Wiigs, M., Smyth, R., Weitzman, E., & Pearce, P. (2001). Children with a history of expressive vocabulary delay: Outcomes at 5 years of age. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 10, 258369. https://doi.org/10.1044/1058-0360(2001/030) Google Scholar
Girbau, D., & Schwartz, R. G. (2007). Nonword repetition in Spanish speaking with Specific Language Impairment (SLI). International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 42(1), 5975. https://doi.org/10.1080/13682820600783210.Google Scholar
Gutiérrez-Clellen, V. F., & Simón-Cerejido, G. (2010). Using nonword repetition tasks for the identification of language impairment in Spanish-English-speaking children: Does the language of assessment matter? Learning Disabilities Research and Practice 25, 4858. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-5826.2009.00300.x.Google Scholar
Heilmann, J., Ellis Weismer, S., Evans, J., & Hollar, C. (2005). Utility of the MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventory in identifying language abilities of late talking and typically developing children. American Journal of Speech and Language Pathology, 14, 4051. https://doi.org/10.1044/1058-0360(2005/006) CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoff, E., Core, C., & Bridges, K. (2008). Non-word repetition assesses phonological memory and is related to vocabulary development in 20- to 24-month-olds. Journal of Child Language, 35, 114. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000908008751 Google Scholar
Howell, P. (2010). Recovery from stuttering. New York, NY: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Howell, P., Tang, K., Tuomainen, O., Chan, K., Beltran, K., Mirawdeli, A., & Harris, J. (2017). Identification of fluency and word-finding difficulty in samples of children with diverse language backgrounds. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders 52(5), 595611. https://doi.org/10.1111/1460-6984.12305 Google Scholar
Jones, G., Tamburelli, M., Watson, S. E., Gobet, F., & Pine, J. M. (2010). Lexicality and frequency in specific language impairment: Accuracy and error data from two nonword repetition tests. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 53, 16421655. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2010/09-0222).Google Scholar
Lázaro, M., Rujas, I., Montero, I., Casla, M., & Murillo, E. (in press). Baremación de una tarea de repetición de pseudopalabras para la evaluación del desarrollo léxico. [Validation of a scale for a nonword repetition task to assess lexical development]. Anales de Psicología.Google Scholar
Law, J., Boyle, J., Harris, F., Harkness, A., & Nye, C. (2000). Prevalence and natural history of primary speech and language delay: Findings from a systematic review of the literature. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 35, 165188.Google Scholar
López Ornat, S., Gallego, C., Gallo, P., Karosou, A., & Mariscal, S. (2005). Inventario de Desarrollo Comunicativo MacArthur. Adaptación española [MacArthur Community Development Inventory. Spanish adaptation]. Madrid, Spain: TEA Ediciones.Google Scholar
Mariscal, S., & Gallego, C. (2013). La imitación como herramienta para investigar y evaluar el desarrollo lingüístico temprano: Un estudio piloto de repetición de palabras y pseudopalabras [Imitation as a tool for research and assessment of early language development: A pilot study of word and non-word repetition]. Revista de Investigación en Logopedia, 3(1), 5375.Google Scholar
Mckean, C., Letts, C., & Howard, D. (2013). Developmental change is key to understanding primary language impairment: the case of phonotactic probability and nonword repetition. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Reseaqrch, 56(5), 15791594. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2013/12-0066) CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mckean, C., Letts, C., & Howard, D. (2014). Triggering word learning in children with Language Impairment: The effect of phonotactic probability and neighborhood density. Journal of Child Language, 41(6), 12241248. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000913000445.Google Scholar
Paul, R., & Jennings, P. (1992). Phonological behavior in toddlers with slow expressive language development. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 35(1), 99 107. https://doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3501.99 Google Scholar
Rescorla, L. (1989). The Language Development Survey: A screening tool for delayed language in toddlers. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 54, 587599. https://doi.org/10.1044/jshd.5404.587.Google Scholar
Rescorla, L. (2011). Late talkers: Do good predictors of outcome exist? Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 17(2), 141150. https://doi.org/10.1002/ddrr.1108.Google Scholar
Rescorla, L., & Schwartz, E. (1990). Outcomes of toddlers with expressive language delay. Applied Psycholinguistics, 11, 393407. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716400009644 Google Scholar
Rice, M. L., Taylor, C. L., & Zubrick, S. R. (2008). Language outcomes of 7-year-old children with or without a history of late language emergence at 24 months. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 51(2), 394407. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2008/029) CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rispens, J., & Baker, A. (2012). Nonword repetition: The relative contributions of phonological short-term memory and phonological representations in children with language and reading impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 55(3), 683694. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2011/10-0263) CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roy, P., & Chiat, S. (2004). A prosodically controlled word and nonword repetition task for 2-to 4-year-olds evidence from typically developing children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 47(1), 223234. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2004/019) Google Scholar
Schlichting, L. (2005). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test—III NL. Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Harcourt Test Publishers.Google Scholar
Stoel-Gammon, C. (2011). Relationships between lexical and phonological development in young children. Journal of Child Language, 38(1), 134. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000910000425 Google Scholar
Stokes, S., & Klee, T. (2009a). The diagnostic accuracy of a new test of early nonword repetition for differentiating late talking children from typically developing children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 52(4), 872882. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2009/08-0030) Google Scholar
Stokes, S., & Klee, T. (2009b). Factors that influence vocabulary development in two-year-old children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 50(4), 498505. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.01991.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stokes, S., Moran, C., & George, A. (2013). Nonword repetition and vocabulary use in toddlers. Topics in Language Disorders, 13(3), 221265. https://doi.org/10.1097/TLD.0b013e31829d038c Google Scholar
Thal, D. J., Bates, E., Goodman, J., & Jahn-Samilo, J. (1997). Continuity of language abilities: An exploratory study of late and early-talking toddlers. Developmental Neuropsychology, 13(3), 239273. https://doi.org/10.1080/87565649709540681 Google Scholar
Thal, D. J., Miller, S., Carlson, J., & Vega, M. M. (2005). Nonword repetition and language development in 4-year-old children with and without a history of early language delay. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 48(6), 14811495. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2005/103).Google Scholar
Williams, A. L., & Elbert, M. (2003). A prospective longitudinal study of phonological development in late talkers. Language, Speech and Hearing Services in Schools, 34, 138153. https://doi.org/10.1044/0161-1461(2003/012).Google Scholar
Zubrick, S. R., Taylor, C. L., Rice, M. L., & Slegers, D. W. (2007). Late language emergence at 24 months: An epidemiological study of prevalence, predictors, and covariates. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50(6), 15621592. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2007/106).Google Scholar