Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-fbnjt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-10T08:24:02.217Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Characteristics and rates of mental health problems among Indian and White adolescents in two English cities

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

Nisha Dogra*
Affiliation:
Greenwood Institute of Child Health, School of Psychology, University of Leicester
Nadzeya Svirydzenka
Affiliation:
Greenwood Institute of Child Health, School of Psychology, University of Leicester
Pat Dugard
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, University of Dundee
Swaran P. Singh
Affiliation:
Mental Health & Wellbeing, University of Warwick, Coventry
Panos Vostanis
Affiliation:
Greenwood Institute of Child Health, School of Psychology, University of Leicester
*
Dr N. Dogra, Greenwood Institute of Child Health, School of Psychology, University of Leicester, Westcotes House, Westcotes Drive, Leicester LE3 0QU, UK. Email: nd13@le.ac.uk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Sampling techniques for national surveys have constrained the statistical power in estimating prevalence rates of child mental health problems in minority ethnic groups.

Aims

To establish the prevalence rates of mental health problems in ethnic Indian adolescents in England and compare these with matched White adolescents living in the same areas.

Method

A cross-sectional survey with oversampling of Indian adolescents aged 13–15 years of age.

Results

The sample size was 2900 (71% response rate) with 1087 (37%) Indian and 414 (14%) White adolescents. Ethnically Indian adolescents had lower rates of all types of mental

health problems (5% v. 13% and 21% v. 30% for abnormal Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire and Short Mood and Feelings Questionnaire scores, respectively) and substance misuse (18% v. 57%, 5% v. 15% and 6% v. 9% for regular alcohol, smoking and drug use, respectively), with the exception of eating disorders, compared with their White counterparts. The odds of an abnormal score on the mental health questionnaires were worse for White compared with Indian children irrespective of sociodemographic variables.

Conclusions

Factors relating to how Indian adolescents are parented or their social support networks may be influencing their mental health and may warrant further investigation.

Type
Papers
Copyright
Copyright © Royal College of Psychiatrists, 2013 

Approximately 20% of the world's children and adolescents are estimated to have mental health problems that require assessment and intervention, with similar types of psychopathology across cultures. The figure can only be an estimate given that data on child and adolescent mental health is variably reported in national health surveys. 1 Two surveys in Great Britain established that the point prevalence of mental health problems was 11.5% among 11- to 16-year-olds, 10.3% among girls and 12.6% among boys. Reference Green, McGinnity, Meltzer, Ford and Goodman2 Girls had higher rates of emotional problems, and boys had higher rates of conduct and hyperkinetic disorders. These results are similar to the rates found in Germany by the BELLA study, which established that 12.2% of 11- to 17-year-olds met the subclinical or clinical threshold on a standardised morbidity measure (9.5% for girls and 14.8% boys), with gender patterns on the distribution of specific problems similar to the UK population. Reference Ravens-Sieberer, Wille, Erhart, Bettge, Wittchen and Rothenberger3

Despite the emerging evidence regarding the level of child mental health need in the general population, sampling techniques have constrained the statistical power in estimating similar rates in minority ethnic groups. A trend in the Great Britain survey suggested that, in comparison with White British children, Indian children had lower prevalence of both conduct and emotional problems. Reference Green, McGinnity, Meltzer, Ford and Goodman2 An additional difficulty in interpreting existing data from general population surveys is the way of recording or measuring ethnicity, with relatively heterogeneous groups being grouped together. Reference Dogra, Singh, Svirydzenka and Vostanis4

The increasing diversity of society and the importance of developing services based on clear evidence require detailed knowledge about the specific needs of different ethnic groups. This was the rationale for the present study. Its objectives were: (a) to establish the prevalence rates of mental health problems in settled communities of ethnic Indian adolescents in England; (b) to compare these rates with those of matched White adolescents living in the same areas; and (c) to determine sociodemographic correlates of any differences found. In this paper we use the term Indian and White adolescents as identified by their ethnicity (by ethnicity we mean their background heritage and origins), not nationality. Indian adolescents include those with British, Indian and other nationalities, and White adolescents include other nationalities as well as British.

Method

Recruitment and sample

We deliberately sought to oversample Indian adolescents to obtain a large enough group for reliable estimation of prevalence rates. For this, we stratified our sample by Indian ethnicity, the majority minority ethnic population in Leicester and West London based on Census data, 5-7 and approached schools that had significant numbers of adolescents (over 50%) of Indian ethnicity. There were no other inclusion criteria, although all the schools fulfilling the first criterion were in urban areas. The age group 13-15 years was selected because of the increase of mental health problems in adolescence, Reference Green, McGinnity, Meltzer, Ford and Goodman2 and the schools' suggestion not to involve pupils during their final examinations year.

Materials

All adolescents completed the following instruments.

Sociodemographic checklist

This included information on age, gender, young person's and parents' place of birth, religion, primary language and any other languages they spoke, nationality, whether they felt that they belonged to an ethnic group (and if so, were given a list to select from), whether they perceived they belonged to one culture or more (and to identify these), who they lived with, and whether they qualified for free school meals (as a measure of socioeconomic status). Ethnicity was not defined and the list used contained similar groups as used in the census.

Mental health assessment instruments

These were widely established and standardised measures of different types of mental health problems that are particularly common in adolescence.

  1. (a) The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ), Reference Goodman8 as a measure of general mental health problems, with a score of total problems, as well as subscales on conduct, emotional, hyperkinetic and peer relationships problems.

  2. (b) The Short Mood and Feelings Questionnaire (SMFQ), Reference Angold, Costello, Messer, Pickles, Winder and Silver9 a measure of depressive symptoms.

  3. (c) The SCOFF (abridged UK version), Reference Morgan, Reid and Lacey10 a measure of eating problems.

Questions were also asked on the use of drugs, alcohol and smoking. The questionnaire and supporting materials were piloted on a class of 24 adolescents of various ethnicities from one of the schools recruited for clarity and comprehension, which resulted in minor alterations to the language and procedure.

Research procedure

As the study did not involve patients, the project was approved by the University of Leicester Ethics Committee. Data collection took place between September 2010 and May 2011. The procedures varied slightly from school to school to fit in with the school's schedules. Each school received training from a researcher on how the questionnaires were to be administered. Following communication with their parents on the aims of the study, pupils were provided with an outline of the project and an opportunity to consider their participation. If they provided informed consent, they completed the measures, which were sealed and returned to the teacher.

Statistical analysis

Questionnaires were coded and entered onto SPSS 18 for Windows. The mental health questionnaires were dichotomised according to previously established cut-off scores that distinguished clinical from non-clinical cases, i.e. children who were likely to benefit from assessment and possible intervention. Scores of 0-5, or 6, or 7-10 for the emotional and hyperactivity SDQ subscales led to normal, borderline and abnormal categories respectively for these subscales. For the conduct subscale, scores of 0-3, 4 and 5-10 led to categorisation as normal, borderline and abnormal respectively. For peer problems, scores of 0-3 are considered normal, 4-5 borderline and 6-10 abnormal. The usual practice is that prosocial scores are scored separately and not included in the total SDQ and we followed that convention. A score of 6-10 is normal, 5 is borderline and any score lower than 4 abnormal. Total SDQ scores vary from 0 to 40 with 0-15, 16-19 and 20-40 falling in normal, borderline and abnormal ranges for developing or having mental health problems respectively. An SMFQ score of eight and above showed that the young person was at risk of depression or anxiety problems. Those children with scores of 3 and higher on a 0-5 SCOFF score range are at risk of having or developing an eating disorder. Chi-squared tests were completed for between-group comparisons.

Regression is often the method of choice for attempting to predict a variable that may be influenced by several other variables. It was not appropriate for our analysis as we were trying to predict categories (normal, borderline or abnormal) for our mental health status variables SDQ and SMFQ. The variables we expect to be associated with mental health status are also categorical: gender, whether they received free school meals, whether they see themselves as having a single cultural identity, who they live with, ethnic group. An extension to several variables of the chi-squared test of association in a two-way table is required and this is provided by a log-linear model. Reference Dugard, Todman and Staines11,Reference Howell12

A two-way table has only two possibilities: the row and column variables are independent, or they are associated. If there are three variables, then the row, column and layer variables may all be independent, or the row and column variables may be independent within each layer but the layer variable is associated with the row and the column variables, or there may be a three-way association, or there are other possibilities. Adding variables rapidly increases the possibilities. Log-linear models provide a general framework for dealing with such problems hence our justification for using these, rather than regression, models.

Results

Of 23 schools contacted with preliminary information about the project, 9 (5 in Leicester of which one was a girls-only school and 4 in London) participated, giving a response rate of 39%. The sample comprised of 2900 adolescents from a possible 4082 giving a response rate of 71%. The mean age was 13.97 years (s.d. = 0.7). As we surveyed school years 9 and 10, most of our sample was aged between 13 and 15 years (734 (25.3%) were 13; 1500 (51.7%) were 14; 636 (21.9%) were 15 and 6 (0.2%) were 16). Twenty-four (0.8%) did not provide information about their age. There were 1284 (44%) boys and 1610 (56%) girls (6 individuals did not provide information on gender). There were 1087 (38%) Indian and 414 (14%) White adolescents and it is these two groups we will be focusing on in the analysis. In the Indian sample there were 490 males (45.1%) and 597 females (54.9%). In the White British sample 148 (35.7%) were male and 266 (64.3%) were female. The mean age of the Indian group was 13.91 (s.d. = 0.68) and that of the White group 14.05 (s.d. = 0.7) years and for this reason age is not considered further. The analysis does not include White Other and focuses only on those who identified themselves as White British to reduce heterogeneity.

Relationship between sociodemographic factors and ethnicity

Online Table DS1 summarises the results of comparisons on demographic factors (for some variables the categories have been combined: for example, there are many main languages other than English and many nationalities other than British, but numbers are often in single figures and we have grouped them as shown in online Table DS1). For the family situation factor, the category ‘lives with both parents’ includes those living with their extended family, as there were almost identical proportions in the abnormal or borderline ranges of the mental health scales for participants in two-parent and extended families. Many of the results of these comparisons are expected: higher proportions of Indian than White adolescents have a main language other than English (χ2(1) = 23.16, P<0.001) and a nationality other than British (χ2(2) = 95.85, P<0.001). There was little overlap in religious group affiliations between Indian (57% Hindu, 27% Muslim) and White British adolescents (42% Christian, 40% agnostic/atheist). Similar proportions of the groups qualify for free school meals. Less than half of Indian but three-quarters of White adolescents' state they have a single cultural identity (χ2(1) = 61.19, P<0.001). A much higher proportion of Indian than White adolescents live with both parents or their extended family, and correspondingly fewer with a single-parent or stepfamily (χ2(2) = 124.50, P<0.001).

Ethnicity (self-identified) and reported mental health problems

Indian adolescents reported lower proportions in the abnormal and borderline SDQ range (Table 1), indicating lower rates of general mental health (behavioural and emotional) problems. A lower proportion of the Indian sample (21%) had scores in the abnormal SMFQ range (depressive symptoms) than the White sample (30%). There were no differences between the two groups on SCOFF relating to eating problems. A higher proportion of White adolescents tried alcohol and drugs and then went on to use them regularly (57% v. 18%). The differences were greatest for alcohol, but were also significant for smoking and illicit drugs.

As the girls-only school had a high proportion of White adolescents, the White sample had a higher proportion of girls (64%) than the Indian sample (55%). Rates were therefore compared by ethnicity and gender (Tables 2 and 3). White boys had significantly higher rates of borderline or abnormal SDQ total scores and were significantly more likely to use alcohol than Indian boys. For all measures, except the SCOFF, White girls had higher rates of problems and substance misuse than Indian girls. Hence, the White sample having more girls does not explain the rates for White children being higher.

Table 1 Rates of abnormal and borderline scores and substance use in Indian and White subgroupsFootnote a

Ethnic group, n (%)
Indian White χ2 (d.f.) P
Scale and score range
Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire total
    Normal 797 (82) 255 (70)
    Borderline 125 (13) 63 (17)
    Abnormal 48 (5) 46 (13)
    Total 970 (100) 364 (100) 3080 (2) <0.001
Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire prosocial
    Normal 884 (84) 312 (78)
    Borderline 104 (10) 59 (15)
    Abnormal 58 (6) 27 (7)
    Total 1046 (100) 398 (100) 814 (2) <0.05
Short Mood and Feelings Questionnaire total
    Normal 818 (79) 276 (70)
    Abnormal 221 (21) 116 (30)
    Total 1039 (100) 392 (100) 1095 (1) <0.01
SCOFF total
    Normal 927 (92) 356 (92)
    Abnormal 81 (8) 31 (8)
    Total 1008 (100) 387 (100) 0 (1) 1
Substance, usage
Drugs
    Never 940 (90) 341 (85)
    Once only 46 (4) 26 (6)
    Regular 63 (6) 35 (9)
    Total 1049 (100) 402 (100) 643 <0.05
Alcohol
    Never 709 (68) 108 (27)
    Once only 148 (14) 67 (17)
    Regular 190 (18) 229 (57)
    Total 1047 (100) 404 (100) 23806 <0.001
Smoking
    Never 892 (85) 268 (66)
    Once only 105 (10) 75 (19)
    Regular 55 (5) 61 (15)
    Total 1052 (100) 404 (100) 6557 <0.001

a. All percentages quoted in the results section are rounded to the nearest whole number. Not all respondents answered every question: for each analysis we have used all available data on the questions considered, so totals are not always the same. In reporting the use of drugs, alcohol and tobacco we have combined the categories from ‘use a few times a year’ to ‘use on a daily basis’ into a single category of ‘regular’ use because of the small numbers in some of the categories.

Gender characteristics among Indian adolescents

Within the Indian group, boys had significantly higher proportions of abnormal and borderline scores (χ2(2) = 6.92, P<005) than girls on the SDQ conduct subscale, but significantly lower proportions of abnormal and borderline scores on the emotional subscale (χ2(2) = 29.31, P<0001). There were no statistically significant differences between the two groups on the SDQ hyperkinetic or peer relationships subscales, or the SCOFF. The girls' higher SDQ emotional subscale scores were consistent with girls showing significantly higher rates on the SMFQ (χ2(1) = 19.25, P<0001) than boys. Indian boys were significantly more likely to smoke (χ2(2) = 14.14, P<0.01) and use illicit drugs (χ2(2) = 26.82, P<0.001) compared with their Indian female peers.

Relationship between sociodemographic factors and mental health problems

Age was not considered as a variable as all but one of our sample was aged between 13 and 15 years (all were from school years 9 and 10). Comparing Indian and White participants who say they identify with a single culture reveals that a significantly lower proportion of Indian adolescents were in the abnormal or borderline range of SDQ total (χ2(2) = 11.95, P<0.01) and SMFQ scores (χ2(1) = 5.98, P<0.05). Similar results are obtained when Indian and White adolescents who do not perceive that they belong to only one cultural group are analysed, although proportions differ significantly only for the SDQ total scores (χ2(2) = 16.88, P<0.001).

Table 2 Comparison of Indian and White boys

Ethnic group, n (%)
Indian White χ2 (d.f.) P
Scale and score range
Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire total
    Normal 362 (84) 101 (75)
    Borderline 49 (11) 19 (14)
    Abnormal 21 (5) 14 (11)
    Total 432 (100) 134 (100) 6.73 (2) <0.05
Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire prosocial
    Normal 356 (76) 100 (70)
    Borderline 73 (16) 28 (20)
    Abnormal 38 (8) 15 (10)
    Total 467 (100) 143 (100) 2.31 (2) >0.05
Short Mood and Feelings Questionnaire total
    Normal 402 (85) 111 (79)
    Abnormal 72 (15) 29 (21)
    Total 474 (100) 140 (100) 2.40 (1) >0.05
SCOFF total
    Normal 429 (93) 132 (96)
    Abnormal 30 (7) 5 (4)
    Total 459 (100) 137 (100) 1.59 (1) >0.05
Substance, usage
Drugs
    Never 401 (85) 126 (86)
    Once only 26 (5) 9 (6)
    Regular 47 (10) 11 (8)
    Total 474 (100) 146 (100) 0.81 (2) >0.05
Alcohol
    Never 321 (68) 39 (27)
    Once only 70 (15) 26 (18)
    Regular 81 (17) 81 (55)
    Total 472 (100) 146 (100) 95.74 (2) <0.001
Smoking
    Never 381 (80) 111 (76)
    Once only 61 (13) 22 (15)
    Regular 33 (7) 13 (9)
    Total 475 (100) 146 (100) 1.24 (2) >0.05

a. All percentages quoted in the results section are rounded to the nearest whole number. Not all respondents answered every question: for each analysis we have used all available data on the questions considered, so totals are not always the same. In reporting the use of drugs, alcohol and tobacco we have combined the categories from ‘use a few times a year’ to ‘use on a daily basis’ into a single category of ‘regular’ use because of the small numbers in some of the categories.

Comparing Indian and White adolescents who identified English as their main language, there is the same pattern of lower proportions of Indian adolescents in the borderline or abnormal ranges of the SDQ total and the SMFQ, and using drugs, alcohol and tobacco as when we compare the full Indian and White samples, as the comparison is in effect the same given the majority had English as their main language. Again the proportions with abnormal SCOFF scores are similar for the two ethnic groups. The small numbers of those whose main language is not English means that chi-squared tests are not appropriate, but the same pattern can be seen in all the tables as we observed for those with English as the main language, or indeed for the ethnic groups as a whole. Within the British nationality group we find higher proportions of White than Indian adolescents in the abnormal or borderline ranges of SDQ total and SMFQ scores. Also, White adolescents had higher prevalence for regular alcohol use compared with Indian adolescents (58% and 16% respectively). For smoking the figures are 14% and 5%, and for drug use 8% and 7% (the difference in proportions is not significant for the latter).

Similarly, among those who qualified for free school meals, White adolescents showed higher abnormal ranges of SDQ total and SMFQ scores than their Indian counterparts. A higher proportion of White adolescents also exhibited regular alcohol consumption and smoking behaviour. Among those not qualifying for free school meals we found the same results except that this time significantly more White adolescents also used drugs.

Those living with either both parents or extended family have lower proportions in the abnormal or borderline ranges of the SDQ total, SMFQ and SCOFF than those living with a single parent, who in turn have lower proportions than those in stepfamilies. All these differences are significant at the 1% level and are considerable. Combining abnormal and borderline ranges to summarise SDQ total, the percentages are 18%, 28% and 42% for SDQ total, 21%, 29% and 44% for SMFQ, 7%, 11%, and 17% for SCOFF. There are also significant differences in proportions using drugs, alcohol and tobacco, with those in two-parent or extended families again at a clear advantage and those in stepfamilies with the highest rates of use.

The five variables that may be associated with mental health status are all binary except for who children live with. In this case, as there were only 73 living with stepfamilies, these were combined with single-parent families giving two categories: lives with both parents or extended family, lives with a single parent or stepfamily. This led to five binary variables, so 32 cells in a five dimensional table.

Table 3 Comparison of Indian and White girls

Ethnic group, n (%)
Indian White χ2 (d.f.) P
Scale and score range
Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire total
    Normal 435 (81) 154 (67)
    Borderline 76 (14) 44 (19)
    Abnormal 27 (5) 32 (14)
    Total 538 (100) 230 (100) 23.23 (2) <0.001
Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire prosocial
    Normal 528 (91) 212 (83)
    Borderline 31 (5) 31 (12)
    Abnormal 20 (4) 12 (5)
    Total 579 (100) 255 (100) 13.04 (2) <0.01
Short Mood and Feelings Questionnaire total
    Normal 416 (74) 165 (65)
    Abnormal 149 (26) 87 (35)
    Total 565 (100) 252 (100) 5.63 (1) <0.05
SCOFF total
    Normal 498 (91) 224 (90)
    Abnormal 51 (9) 26 (10)
    Total 549 (100) 250 (100) 0.24 (1) >0.05
Substance, usage
Drugs
    Never 539 (94) 215 (84)
    Once only 20 (3) 17 (7)
    Regular 16 (3) 24 (9)
    Total 575 (100) 256 (100) 21.83 (2) <0.001
Alcohol
    Never 388 (67) 69 (27)
    Once only 78 (14) 41 (16)
    Regular 109 (19) 148 (57)
    Total 575 (100) 258 (100) 139.69 (2) <0.001
Smoking
    Never 511 (87) 157 (61)
    Once only 44 (8) 53 (21)
    Regular 22 (4) 48 (19)
    Total 577 (100) 258 (100) 87.25 (2) <0.001

a. All percentages quoted in the results section are rounded to the nearest whole number. Not all respondents answered every question: for each analysis we have used all available data on the questions considered, so totals are not always the same. In reporting the use of drugs, alcohol and tobacco we have combined the categories from ‘use a few times a year’ to ‘use on a daily basis’ into a single category of ‘regular’ use because of the small numbers in some of the categories.

The SDQ and SMFQ indicate mental health status. The next step examines how the variables discussed above are associated with mental health status, which in binary terms can be viewed as no abnormal scores on the SDQ or SMFQ or at least one abnormal score, thus giving a summary mental health indicator. In total there are six variables, which gives a six-way table to see how the variables relate to each other. As with a two-way table and the chi-squared test, we were interested in the interactions or associations in our six-way table. As SPSS provides for this facility, backward elimination was used to identify the interactions that were significant. (There are two goodness-of-fit tests, Pearson and likelihood ratio, which indicate a poor fit if they reach significance: our significance levels are 0.86 and 0.72 respectively indicating that the model is effective.) The model presented includes all interactions among gender, ethnic group, who the young person lives with and whether they have a single cultural identity. In addition, it includes all interactions among school meals, who they live with and the mental health indicator, and the interactions between the mental health indicator and gender, and between the mental health indicator and ethnic group. After allowing for all other interactions, there is still an association between mental health indicator and ethnic group that is significant at the 5% level (P = 0.016).

The log-linear model provides expected cell counts for each of the 64 cells in the six-way table: these can be seen with observed cell counts in online Table DS2. For any combination of variables (such as male, White, free school meals, single cultural identity, lives with both parents or extended family), the odds of an abnormal score on the mental health indicator is the ratio of expected numbers in this combination of categories with and without an abnormal score on the mental health indicator (1.313/4.031 or 0.3257 for this example). Using the same example categories except for Indian replacing White, the ratio of expected numbers of male, Indian, free school meals, single cultural identity, lives with both parents or extended family, with and without an abnormal score as the mental health indicator, 3.208/14.363 or 0.2234. The odds are worse for the White group (0.3257) than for the Indian adolescents (0.2234) who are otherwise similar. In fact, the odds ratio White/Indian is 1.46. This is the case for all other combinations of variables: the odds of an abnormal score on the mental health indicator are worse for White than for Indian children and the odds ratio of 1.46 gives an estimate of the size of the effect of ethnic group after allowing for our other variables.

Discussion

Main findings

This study found that adolescents living in England and who were ethnically Indian had lower rates of all types of mental health problems and substance misuse than White adolescents. This adds to previous findings that Indian children are predominantly protected against externalising problems. Reference Goodman, Patel and Leon13 The rates also reflect the patterns of mental health problems among indigenous adolescents of a similar age in India. Reference Pillai, Patel, Cardozo, Goodman, Weiss and Andrew14 Although the proportion of problems and their gender associations were similar to the White population (higher rates of emotional problems for girls and higher rates of conduct problems for boys), as established by other studies, Reference Green, McGinnity, Meltzer, Ford and Goodman2,Reference Ravens-Sieberer, Wille, Erhart, Bettge, Wittchen and Rothenberger3,Reference Herpertz-Dahlmann, Wille, Holling, Vloet and Ravens-Sieberer15 the rates were significantly lower for all problems apart from eating disorders. The rate of possible eating disorders in our study was 8% and this is considerably less than that identified in a German study of 1895 young people aged between 11 and 17 years, which found that 29% of girls and 14% of boys scored beyond the normal threshold. Reference Herpertz-Dahlmann, Wille, Holling, Vloet and Ravens-Sieberer15 The higher use of substances in boys compared with girls, found in the Indian and White groups, reflects the same pattern as identified by previous research. Reference Green, McGinnity, Meltzer, Ford and Goodman2,Reference Ravens-Sieberer, Wille, Erhart, Bettge, Wittchen and Rothenberger3

Comparing Indian and White adolescents who perceived they belonged to only one cultural group, the former scored lower for some problems, and it may be that in these areas where there is high presence of Indians or other ethnic groups, White adolescents are the ‘minority’ group who experience more marginalisation resulting in their higher rates of problems. Georgiades et al Reference Georgiades, Boyle and Duku16 found that, despite greater socioeconomic disadvantage, young people living in recent immigrant families had lower levels of emotional/behavioural problems and higher levels of school performance. Living in a neighbourhood with higher concentration of immigrants was associated with lower levels of problems among immigrant children but the reverse was true for non-immigrant children. Living in high-immigrant areas as an immigrant may provide community support whereas in White groups it may indicate socioeconomic disadvantage. This in part may explain some of the differences found, although most of the adolescents surveyed are unlikely to be ‘immigrants’ in that most were UK born. The high rate of multiple culture identification in Indian adolescents who also score well on mental health instruments suggests that cultural identification might have a mediating role in mental health problem prevalence, but this question requires targeted hypothesis-driven research.

As in previous research, adolescents in two-parent families are at lower risk of mental health problems than those from lone-parent or reconstituted families. Reference Green, McGinnity, Meltzer, Ford and Goodman2 With the large differences in proportions of Indian and White adolescents in the different family situations, and the strong association between family situation and proportions in abnormal or borderline ranges of the mental health scales and proportions using drugs, alcohol and tobacco, the question is whether the advantage in family situation of the Indian adolescents explains their mental health advantage. However, this was not supported by the finding that when considering only those in two-parent or extended families the Indian adolescents have significantly lower proportions in the abnormal or borderline range of the SDQ total score (the SMFQ score is also lower but does not reach significance). The pattern is similar for single-parent families. Although divorce rates in the British Indian population may be increasing, Reference Das17 they are still the lowest at 10%. 5-7 It may be that Indian adolescents are less likely to have to adjust to reconstituted families, and may also receive greater support from extended family networks, thereby mitigating the impact of parental separation.

Religion as a protective and risk factor has also been studied, Reference Meltzer, Dogra, Vostanis and Ford18 but in our sample belonging to a specific religious group appeared not to be a significant factor either way, although we did not measure how religious adolescents were in practice. Poverty is another well-established risk factor for child mental health problems. In this study we used the index entitlement to free school meals, which seems to have only a small effect but still, within those groups who qualify for free school meals and those who do not, Indian adolescents remain at an advantage. It does not appear that the two ethnic groups have different levels of poverty so that does not help explain the difference in the rates of problems identified. The study took place in the city of Leicester and in Wembley, neither of which are overtly affluent areas. The issue of socioeconomic disadvantage may be mitigated if children do not feel worse off than their peers because the level of disadvantage is possibly equal across the group.

Implications

Overall, adolescents of Indian ethnic background appear to enjoy better mental health than their White counterparts. Our data show the difference but do not explain it. There may be factors relating to how children are parented or their social support networks that are influencing their mental health that may warrant further investigation. This study has also demonstrated that national data samples may be of less use for services that do not reflect the national picture and we would suggest that repeating such surveys in areas with high minority populations may be useful. More in-depth research about the role of potentially mediating factors such as religion, culture and ethnicity is important before drawing definitive conclusions.

Limitations

A number of limitations need to be acknowledged. The study relies on self-report questionnaires, which are less accurate than diagnostic interviews. However, the components comprising the questionnaire have all been well validated and the same process was used to collect data in both groups. Those adolescents with mental health problems may have chosen to absent themselves from school when they were expected to complete the questionnaires but again this would apply to both ethnic groups. Although the measures used are well validated, they have their own limitations given that they are all self-report measures. The SDQ, for example has only moderate sensitivity of around 15% for all disorders. Reference Goodman, Renfrew and Mullick19 Also, the SDQ has been validated for use in British samples, most of which have been White. Reference Green, McGinnity, Meltzer, Ford and Goodman2 In the Office for National Statistics survey on child mental health in which the SDQ was used, there were no comments or reports (Indian sample size 361) that the cut-offs for this ethnic group needed to be different. We are not aware of any studies in ‘Indian ethnicity’ children that have used the SDQ and then a gold-standard psychiatric interview, so the SDQ has not been calibrated for Indian children and it is possible that a different cut-off for risk of disorder would be needed. An additional limitation is that there were not multiple informants and SDQs, when completed by parents and teachers, are generally better predictors than SDQs completed just by the adolescent. Reference Goodman, Ford, Simmons, Gatward and Meltzer20 There is also potential inconsistency in how the questionnaires were administered across different schools, although we mitigated this by providing clear guidance and research staff to help with the process if needed. There was no corroborative information from parents, or more detailed measurement of identity factors and other potential mediating variables, such as parenting attitudes and practice. There were no major differences between those schools that participated and those that did not, although those that did may have had staff with more awareness of mental health issues.

Future research

In addition to addressing these issues, future research could replicate this study with other ethnic groups. This would establish the patterns of mental health problems across different communities, to enhance the understanding of the social mechanisms that might account for the variations that exist between ethnic groups, but are accounted for by more factors than ethnicity per se. It would also inform reliable health services recording and service planning based on evidence of children's and young people's mental health needs.

Funding

The National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) Clinical Research Networks funded the project.

Acknowledgements

We are grateful to all the young people who took part in this study. We also thank their parents and teachers for their kind cooperation, and Dr David Clarke, Associate Director of the Leicestershire Partnership NHS Trust, for his ongoing support.

Footnotes

Declaration of interest

None.

References

1 World Health Organization. Atlas on Child and Adolescent Mental Health Resources – Global Concerns: Implications for the Future. World Health Organization, 2005.Google Scholar
2 Green, H, McGinnity, A, Meltzer, H, Ford, T, Goodman, R. Mental Health of Children and Young People in Great Britain, 2004. Office for National Statistics, 2005.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
3 Ravens-Sieberer, U, Wille, N, Erhart, M, Bettge, S, Wittchen, H, Rothenberger, A, et al Prevalence of mental health problems among children and adolescents in Germany: results of the BELLA study within the National Health Interview and Examination survey. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2008; 17 (suppl 1): 2233.Google Scholar
4 Dogra, N, Singh, SP, Svirydzenka, N, Vostanis, P. Mental health problems in children and young people from minority ethnic groups: the need for targeted research. Br J Psychiatry 2012; 200; 265–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
5 Leicester City Council. Census 2001. Office for National Statistics, 2003 (http://www.leicester.gov.uk/your-council-services/council-and-democracy/city-statistics/census2001).Google Scholar
6 Brent Council. Brent Borough Census 2001 (https://intelligence.brent.gov.uk/Pages/default.aspx).Google Scholar
8 Goodman, R (2001). Psychometric properties of the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ). J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2001; 40: 1337–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
9 Angold, A, Costello, EJ, Messer, SC, Pickles, A, Winder, F, Silver, D. The development of a short questionnaire for use in epidemiological studies of depression in children and adolescents. Int J Methods Psychiatr Res 1995; 5: 237–49.Google Scholar
10 Morgan, JF, Reid, F, Lacey, JH. The SCOFF questionnaire: assessment of a new screening tool for eating disorders. BMJ 1999; 319: 1467–68.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
11 Dugard, P, Todman, J, Staines, H. Approaching Multivariate Analysis, 2nd edn. Routledge, 2010.Google Scholar
12 Howell, DC. Statistical Methods for Psychology, 7th edn. Wadsworth, 2010.Google Scholar
13 Goodman, A, Patel, V, Leon, D. Why do British Indian children have an apparent mental health advantage? J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2010; 51: 1171–83.Google Scholar
14 Pillai, A, Patel, V, Cardozo, P, Goodman, R, Weiss, HA, Andrew, G. Non-traditional lifestyles and prevalence of mental disorders in adolescents in Goa, India. Br J Psychiatry 2008; 192: 4551.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
15 Herpertz-Dahlmann, B, Wille, N, Holling, H, Vloet, T, Ravens-Sieberer, U, BELLA study group. Disordered eating behaviour and attitudes, associated psychopathology and health related quality of life: results of the BELLA study. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2008; 17 (suppl 1): 8291.Google Scholar
16 Georgiades, K, Boyle, MH, Duku, E. Contextual influences on children's mental health and school performance: the moderating effects of family immigrant status. Child Dev 2007; 78: 1572–91.Google Scholar
17 Das, C. Resilience, risk and protective factors for British-Indian Children of Divorce. J Soc Sci 2010; 25: 97108.Google Scholar
18 Meltzer, H, Dogra, N, Vostanis, P, Ford, T. Religiosity and the mental health of adolescents in Great Britain. Ment Health Relig Cult 2011; 14: 703–13.Google Scholar
19 Goodman, R, Renfrew, D, Mullick, M. Predicting type of psychiatric disorder from Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaires (SDQ) scores in child mental health clinics in London and Dhaka. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2000; 9: 129–34.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
20 Goodman, R, Ford, T, Simmons, H, Gatward, R, Meltzer, H. Using the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) to screen for child psychiatric disorders in a community sample. Br J Psychiatry 2000; 177: 534–9.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1 Rates of abnormal and borderline scores and substance use in Indian and White subgroupsa

Figure 1

Table 2 Comparison of Indian and White boys

Figure 2

Table 3 Comparison of Indian and White girls

Supplementary material: PDF

Dogra et al. supplementary material

Supplementary Table S1-S2

Download Dogra et al. supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 1 MB
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.