Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-8ctnn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-27T13:53:00.311Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Exploring awareness of age-related changes among over 50s in the UK: findings from the PROTECT study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 November 2021

Serena Sabatini*
Affiliation:
College of Medicine and Health, University of Exeter, Exeter, United Kingdom
Obioha C. Ukoumunne
Affiliation:
NIHR Applied Research Collaboration South West Peninsula (PenARC), University of Exeter, Exeter, United Kingdom
Clive Ballard
Affiliation:
College of Medicine and Health, University of Exeter, Exeter, United Kingdom
Rachel Collins
Affiliation:
College of Medicine and Health, University of Exeter, Exeter, United Kingdom
Anne Corbett
Affiliation:
College of Medicine and Health, University of Exeter, Exeter, United Kingdom
Helen Brooker
Affiliation:
College of Medicine and Health, University of Exeter, Exeter, United Kingdom Ecog Pro Ltd, Bristol, United Kingdom
Linda Clare
Affiliation:
College of Medicine and Health, University of Exeter, Exeter, United Kingdom NIHR Applied Research Collaboration South West Peninsula (PenARC), University of Exeter, Exeter, United Kingdom
*
Correspondence should be addressed to: Serena Sabatini, Centre for Research in Ageing and Cognitive Health (REACH), University of Exeter, South Cloisters, St Luke’s Campus, Exeter EX12LU, U.K. Phone: + 01392 726754. Email: s.sabatini2@exeter.ac.uk.

Abstract

Objectives:

Older people describe positive and negative age-related changes, but we do not know much about what contributes to make them aware of these changes. We used content analysis to categorize participants’ written comments and explored the extent to which the identified categories mapped onto theoretical conceptualizations of influences on awareness of age-related change (AARC).

Design:

Cross-sectional observational study.

Participants:

The study sample comprised 609 UK individuals aged 50 years or over (mean (SD) age = 67.9 (7.6) years), enrolled in the PROTECT study.

Measurements:

Between January and March 2019, participants provided demographic information, completed a questionnaire assessing awareness of age-related change (AARC-10 SF), and responded to an open-ended question asking them to comment on their responses.

Results:

While some of the emerging categories were in line with the existing conceptual framework of AARC (e.g. experiencing negative changes and attitudes toward aging), others were novel (e.g. engagement in purposeful activities or in activities that distract from age-related thoughts). Analysis revealed some of the thought processes involved in selecting responses to the questionnaire items, demonstrating different ways in which people make sense of specific items.

Conclusions:

Results support the ability of the AARC questionnaire to capture perceived age-related changes in cognitive functioning, physical and mental health, and engagement in social activities and in healthy and adaptive behaviors. However, findings also suggest ways of enriching the theoretical conceptualization of how AARC develops and offer insights into interpretation of responses to measures of AARC.

Type
Original Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
© International Psychogeriatric Association 2021

Introduction

The global proportion of older individuals is increasing, and this results in a growing number of people experiencing both positive and negative age-related changes. Examples of positive changes are greater knowledge, accumulated life experience, and increased leisure time (Carstensen et al., Reference Carstensen2011; Carstensen, Reference Carstensen and Jacobs1993; Steverink et al., Reference Steverink, Westerhof, Bode and Dittmann-Kohli2001; Timmer et al., Reference Timmer, Steverink and Dittmann-Kohli2002), whereas examples of negative changes are poorer mental, physical, and functional health and poorer cognitive performance (Barnett et al., Reference Barnett, Mercer, Norbury, Watt, Wyke and Guthrie2012; Kingston et al., Reference Kingston, Robinson, Booth, Knapp and Jagger2018; Levy, Reference Levy2017; Royall et al., Reference Royall, Lauterbach, Kaufer, Malloy, Coburn and Black2007; United Nations, 2019).

Maintenance of good health throughout later life is vital for quality of life and for the sustainability of the world health system and economy. The study of self-perceptions of aging (SPA) may suggest ways of promoting health in the second half of life. Indeed, those individuals with positive SPA are more likely to engage in healthy behaviors and to experience better health (Jaconelli et al., Reference Jaconelli, Terracciano, Sutin, Sarrazin, Raffard and Stephan2017; Kornadt et al., Reference Kornadt, Kessler, Wurm, Bowen, Gabrian and Klusmann2019; Montepare, Reference Montepare2019; Westerhof et al., Reference Westerhof2014) than those with negative SPA. However, the way in which SPA have been conceptualized and measured so far may have resulted in a simplified understanding of the relationship between SPA and health (Diehl et al., Reference Diehl2014).

Many existing measures of SPA are unidimensional (e.g. Attitude Toward Own Aging Scale; Lawton, Reference Lawton1975). In this way, respondents can report either negative or positive SPA, but not both. However, developmental gains and losses coexist throughout the lifespan (Baltes, Reference Baltes1987; Miche et al., Reference Miche, Wahl, Diehl, Oswald, Kaspar and Kolb2014). Moreover, many measures provide global evaluations of SPA (e.g. subjective age; Barrett, Reference Barrett2003) even though individuals can experience different perceptions of aging in relation to distinct life domains (e.g. social, cognitive, physical; Carstensen et al., Reference Carstensen2011; Sabatini et al., Reference SabatiniIn Press; Steverink et al., Reference Steverink, Westerhof, Bode and Dittmann-Kohli2001; Voss et al., Reference Voss, Kornadt, Hess, Fung and Rothermund2018). The concept of awareness of age-related change (AARC) aims to overcome these limitations (Diehl and Wahl, Reference Diehl and Wahl2010; Diehl et al., Reference Diehl2014) by capturing self-perceptions of positive (gains) and negative (losses) age-related changes across five life domains: health and physical functioning, cognition, interpersonal relationships, socio-cognitive and socio-emotional functioning, and lifestyle/life engagement.

Perceptions of AARC gains and losses are assessed via questionnaire (AARC-10 SF; Kaspar et al., Reference Kaspar, Gabrian, Brothers, Wahl and Diehl2019). Items included in the AARC questionnaire are the result of interviews and focus groups where US and German middle-aged and older adults discussed the changes they experienced while getting older. Examples of items assessing AARC gains and losses, respectively, are “With my increasing age, I realize that I have become wiser” and “With my increasing age, I realize that I have less energy.” The unique conceptual features of AARC may make it possible to advance our understanding of the mechanisms behind the association of SPA with health. However, so far, little is known about factors that influence and shape AARC.

Based on existing literature on SPA, Diehl and Wahl (Reference Diehl and Wahl2010) proposed an heuristic framework placing the AARC construct into a broader conceptual framework including variables that may act as antecedents of AARC, variables involved in the mental process of AARC, and outcomes of AARC. Hypothesized distal antecedents of AARC comprise sociodemographic (e.g. chronological age and socioeconomic status), health-related factors (e.g. health history and health status), and psychological factors (e.g. personality traits), whereas proximal antecedents include personal goals, life limitations, age stereotypes, and current life events. Hypothesized factors involved in the mental process of AARC include personal meaning-making and self-regulation of behavior. Finally, hypothesized outcomes of AARC are psychological health, physical health, and life engagement.

The relationships of AARC with some of the variables included in the Diehl and Wahl (Reference Diehl and Wahl2010) conceptual framework have been widely researched, whereas other potential relationships remain unexplored. Examples of known antecedents of AARC are sociodemographic variables and attitudes toward own aging (ATOA; Brothers et al., Reference Brothers, Gabrian, Wahl and Diehl2016; Reference Brothers, Miche, Wahl and Diehl2017; Reference Brothers, Kornadt, Nehrkorn-Bailey, Wahl and Diehl2020; English et al., Reference English, Bellingtier and Neupert2019; Sabatini et al., Reference Sabatini2020b), whereas examples of unexplored factors are social engagement and lifestyle. An example of a variable related to the mental process of AARC is engagement in self-regulatory behaviors (Dutt et al., Reference Dutt, Gabrian and Wahl2016), whereas an example of an unexplored factor is the interpretation of changes as being a consequence of aging or of other factors such as illnesses (Rothermund et al., Reference Rothermund2021). In summary, currently there is no evidence supporting some of the hypothesized predictors of AARC. Moreover, the heuristic framework of AARC, outlined by Diehl and Wahl (Reference Diehl and Wahl2010), does not presume to be comprehensive and additional variables related to AARC are yet to be identified.

In the current study, we therefore aim to increase knowledge about what contributes to making people aware of age-related changes as these develop in the second half of life. This aim will be achieved by qualitatively examining the open-ended comments that individuals aged 50 years and over made when reporting on aspects of AARC. More specifically, we will evaluate the degree to which the comments (grouped into categories) that participants make while answering the AARC questionnaire map onto the variables (i.e. distal antecedents, proximal antecedents, variables involved in the mental process of AARC, and developmental outcomes) hypothesized to be related to AARC in the Diehl and Wahl (Reference Diehl and Wahl2010) conceptual framework of AARC, and/or to new variables that could be added to the Diehl and Wahl (Reference Diehl and Wahl2010) conceptual framework of AARC. Hence, study results may help to better understand and refine the current conceptual framework of AARC.

To achieve our study aim, we will also consider participants’ open-ended comments (identified categories) alongside the answers they give to the questionnaire items (their levels of AARC gains and losses). This will make it possible to explore the relative salience of different variables/antecedents (e.g. daily life events) on AARC, and whether different types of thoughts described by participants are differentially related to levels of AARC gains and losses. For instance, individuals who mention having experienced a recent health issue may report higher AARC losses and/or lower AARC gains compared to individuals who do not report a recent health issue. Exploring which factors individuals take into consideration when reporting their AARC may extend our understanding of why AARC is associated with health outcomes.

The current study will gather new evidence on AARC in several ways. First, although the study by Miche et al. (Reference Miche, Wahl, Diehl, Oswald, Kaspar and Kolb2014) provided useful insights into factors that trigger perceptions of AARC, this is the only available study on the topic, and it is limited in that it focused exclusively on reports of daily events. This study, therefore, aims to understand whether the formation of age-related thoughts is influenced by broader psychosocial factors as this has never been investigated with qualitative analysis. Second, in their study, Miche et al. (Reference Miche, Wahl, Diehl, Oswald, Kaspar and Kolb2014) included only participants aged 70 years and over. However, perceptions of age-related change become salient from middle-age (Kornadt et al., Reference Kornadt, Hess, Voss and Rothermund2018) and, due to the good health that generally characterizes middle-aged individuals (Balcombe and Sinclair, Reference Balcombe and Sinclair2001), events related to AARC in this age group may be different. The current study therefore explores a wider range of factors associated with AARC across the second half of life to reach a more comprehensive understanding of how AARC is formed.

Third, this study will explore for the first time the thoughts that UK residents report in relation to AARC. Indeed, although the short form of the AARC questionnaire has recently been validated within the UK population (Sabatini et al., Reference Sabatini2020b), qualitative accounts in relation to the AARC questionnaire have never been explored among UK individuals. This is important given that factors related to AARC seem to vary across cultures (Sabatini et al., Reference Sabatini2020a). Fourth, exploration of participants’ thoughts in relation to the AARC questionnaire may provide additional information on the use of the AARC questionnaire in the UK and, therefore, complement the psychometric validation of the AARC questionnaire in this country. For instance, this study may identify issues (such as interpretation of the meaning of items) that UK individuals report in relation to the items included in the AARC questionnaire.

Methods

Study design and participants

Analyses were conducted on cross-sectional data collected as part of the PROTECT study (https://www.protectstudy.org.uk). Participants were 609 UK individuals aged 50 years or over (M age = 67.9 years; SD = 7.6). The majority of participants in the current study were women, White, married, and nonworking (see Table 1). Further information on study design and participants is provided in Supplementary Text 1.

Table 1. Demographic characteristics of PROTECT participants who answered and who did not answer the AARC open-ended question

Total PROTECT sample including participants who took part in the PROTECT study between 1 January 2019 and 31 March 2019. Participants who did not answer the AARC open-ended question includes all the participants who were part of the PROTECT study between 1 January 2019 and 31 March 2019 but did not answer the AARC open-ended question. Total study sample in this paper comprises participants who in 2019 answered the AARC open-ended question.

Measures

Demographic variables included age, sex, marital status (married and not married), education level (secondary, post-secondary, vocational qualification, undergraduate degree, postgraduate degree, and doctorate), and employment (employed and not employed). AARC was assessed with the AARC-10 SF (Kaspar et al., Reference Kaspar, Gabrian, Brothers, Wahl and Diehl2019) which includes 10 items, 5 measuring gains, and 5 measuring losses across the 5 AARC life domains. Respondents evaluate how much items apply to them on a five-point scale (1 – “Not at all”; 2 – “a little bit”; 3 – “moderately”; 4 – “quite a bit”; 5 – “very much”). Totals scores for each of the gains and losses scales are obtained by summing items belonging to the respective scales and range from a possible 5 to 25; higher scores indicate higher levels of AARC. In the UK validation of the AARC-10 SF, the Cronbach’s alpha (α) measure of internal consistency was 0.77 for the gains subscale and 0.80 for the losses subscale (Sabatini et al., Reference Sabatini2020b). The following open-ended question was used to ask participants to add their thoughts regarding AARC: “If you wish, please add any additional information or comments for this section in the box below.” This question was asked only once after the completion of the AARC-10 SF.

Data analyses

Content analysis was applied to participants’ answers to the AARC open-ended question (Grbich, Reference Grbich2012; Pope et al., Reference Pope, Ziebland and Mays2000). The first author manually analyzed the data using the following steps: (1) data familiarization through reading and rereading participants’ responses (Polit and Beck, Reference Polit and Beck2004) and (2) definition of the categories and manual assignment of participants’ comments to categories. When appropriate, participants’ statements were assigned to more than one category (Richards, Reference Richards2015); (3) visual presentation of categories in a diagram. Further information on qualitative analyses is provided in Supplementary Text 1.

As quantitative data can contribute to the interpretation of meaning in qualitative accounts (Sandelowski and Barroso, Reference Sandelowski and Barroso2003), we calculated the number of participants whose comments were assigned to each of the identified categories. We used the t-test to compare the mean AARC gains and losses scores between those individuals whose comments were assigned to a specific category and those individuals who were not. This enabled us to understand whether the experience of certain life events and/or psychosocial factors makes individuals more likely to perceive higher or lower levels of AARC gains and losses. Hedges’s g was used to quantify effect size (Lakens, Reference Lakens2013). Effect sizes of 0.2, 0.5, and 0.8 are considered small, medium, and large effects, respectively.

Results

Descriptive analysis

Descriptive statistics are reported in Table 1 for all participants who took part in PROTECT in 2019, for the subgroup of participants that took part in PROTECT in 2019 but did not answer the AARC open-ended question, and for the subsample who answered the AARC open-ended question. In this last subsample, participants on average perceived “moderate levels” of AARC gains and “a little bit” of losses. Table 1 reports analyses of differences among the subsamples who did and did not answer the open-ended question. Compared to the sample of participants who did not complete the AARC open-ended question, the subsample who completed it included more individuals who were women, better-educated, and not in paid employment.

Comments associated with AARC judgments

The mean length of comments was 1.5 sentences per participant, although length varied considerably and ranged from a short sentence of 3 words to 10 sentences. From analysis of participants’ comments about their AARC, 10 categories capturing comments associated with AARC were generated and grouped into 3 higher-order categories (Figure 1 illustrates how we hypothesized the categories may be related). The first higher-order category covers “subjective experiences while aging” and includes five categories: “experiencing negative changes,” “experiencing positive changes,” “attitudes toward aging,” “feeling unchanged,” and “purpose-driven experiences.” The second higher-order category captures “factors impacting evaluation of awareness of changes” and includes three categories: “fluctuating levels of awareness of changes,” “disregarding the impact of increased age,” and “difficulty when answering the AARC questionnaire.” The third higher-order category describes the “process of awareness of changes” and includes two categories: “healthy lifestyle and self-regulation” and “social comparison.”

Figure 1. The thematic map illustrates the hypothesis of how the 10 categories identified through content analyses may be interrelated. Categories are organized into predictors of AARC (listed under the heading “subjective experiences while aging”), factors explaining lack of accuracy in AARC (listed under the heading “factors impacting evaluation of awareness of changes”), and factors involved in the mental process of AARC.

Subjective experiences while aging

Examples of participants’ comments for the identified categories are presented in Table 2. The higher-order category “Subjective experiences while aging” describes factors that can increase or decrease awareness of changes. Some participants (33.3% of the current study sample (N = 609)) experienced negative changes across various life domains such as physical and cognitive domains. Changes in the physical domain included the presence of a chronic health condition, a recent injury or illness, falls, physical pain, decreased energy, and changes in physical appearance. Changes in the cognitive domain related to loss of memory or concentration. However, some participants also experienced positive changes (18.2% of the current study sample) that, similar to awareness of negative changes, happened in various domains including mental health (e.g. being happier), physical health, cognitive abilities (e.g. increased knowledge), life engagement (e.g. more time to pursue hobbies), and even characterized their personality (e.g. becoming less impulsive).

Table 2. Examples of participants’ comments per each of the identified categories

For some participants, awareness of changes was influenced by their attitudes toward aging and life in general (6.7% of the current study sample). Some people experienced older age as a positive period of life and perceived older people as competent, whereas others viewed older age as a negative stage of life and believed that older people are fragile and in need of help. For some participants, their attitudes toward life changed while aging; for instance, individuals become more aware of the finitude of life or shift their focus from the future to the present.

Other participants instead felt unchanged or at least felt that some aspects of their lives, attitudes, or behaviors had not changed with their increased age (28% of the current study sample). Finally, another factor that some participants (12.8% of the current study sample) described as being related to their awareness of changes is their level of engagement in a variety of activities including work, hobbies (e.g. traveling), or caring for someone living with an illness or for grandchildren. Being able to conduct specific activities made some participants feel competent or conferred a sense of purpose. For other individuals having several commitments left them little time to reflect on the age-related changes they were experiencing, resulting in lower levels of awareness of changes.

Factors impacting evaluation of awareness of changes

In the higher-order category “Factors impacting evaluation of awareness of changes” participants remarked how several factors can influence the levels of changes they perceive. First, a small proportion of participants (1.2% of the current study sample) reported that their AARC fluctuates depending on several factors such as the situation they are in. Many individuals (31.3% of the current study sample), despite acknowledging the presence of relevant, specific changes in their life, clarified that such changes are not due to their increased age. Instead, participants identified various factors that in their opinion cause the experienced changes, including being a caregiver, a recent bereavement, financial restrictions, societal changes, a physical condition, the menopause, and a mental health issue. Finally, some individuals (6.2% of the current study sample) had difficulty in answering the AARC questionnaire due to being forced to choose an answer where they would have preferred a non-applicable option, finding items to be vague or not understanding the specific meaning. Overall, the above-described factors impacted on the scores that a relevant proportion of participants obtained on the AARC questionnaire. This may result in decreased accuracy of individuals’ scores on the AARC-10 SF.

Process of awareness of changes

The higher-order category “Process of awareness of changes” describes mental processes that can result in intention to react or adapt to changes and mental processes that individuals use to make sense of the changes they experience. A small proportion of people reacted or adapted to AARC losses by abandoning physically demanding activities and hobbies, and engaging in new activities and/or in healthy behaviors (7.5% of the current study sample). Few individuals (1.5%) used mental strategies such as comparing themselves with other people that were in a less advantageous position than them or emphasizing the positive opinion that other individuals had about them. Frequencies for each category for the study sample are presented in Table 3.

Table 3. Number (%) of the study sample who provided responses to the open-ended AARC question that fell into specific categories

Relationships among categories

The 10 categories are organized in a diagram showing how they are interrelated (Figure 1). The diagram shows how different subjective experiences of aging can lead to varying degrees of awareness of changes, attempts to explain how awareness of changes in some cases may not be interpreted as a consequence of increased age, and how awareness of changes may trigger specific mental processes. The left side of the diagram illustrates five categories (experiencing positive changes, experiencing negative changes, ATOA, feeling unchanged, and purpose-driven experiences) that may enhance or decrease awareness of changes happening in individuals’ lives. The center of the diagram illustrates variables that, irrespective of the changes that individuals are experiencing, may impact on individuals’ evaluation of awareness of changes, and hence on scores on the AARC questionnaire. Indeed, some participants did not attribute changes to their increased age, experienced fluctuations in levels of awareness of changes, and had difficulty in understanding the AARC questionnaire. The right side of the diagram illustrates how being aware of changes may trigger mental processes related to the implementation of self-regulatory strategies, attention to healthy lifestyles, and social comparison. These mental processes could potentially be related to motivation to engage in interventions teaching individuals to adapt to age-related changes.

Comparing levels of AARC gains and losses among individuals whose comments were and were not assigned to a specific category

Results from the t-tests showing whether the extent of gains or losses for those individuals who had comments assigned to the given category was different to the extent of gains or losses reported by individuals whose comments were not assigned to the same category are reported in Tables 4a and 4b. Those participants who felt unchanged (small effect size) and/or found it difficult to answer the AARC questionnaire (large effect size) perceived fewer AARC gains than those participants whose comments were not assigned to these categories. Participants who reported purpose-driven experiences (small effect size) and attention to a healthy lifestyle and adaptation to age-related changes (small effect size) perceived more AARC gains than those participants whose comments were not assigned to these categories. Regarding the remaining seven categories, participants who commented on a specific category did not differ significantly in levels of AARC gains from those participants whose comments were not assigned to the same category.

Table 4a. For each of the 10 categories, the table summarizes comparisons of the mean AARC gain score between individuals whose comments were assigned to a category and individuals whose comments were not assigned to the category

Hedges’s g = effect size. 0.2, 0.5 and 0.8 are considered small, medium and large effects, respectively. Degrees of Freedom (df) = 607.

Table 4b. For each of the 10 categories, the table summarizes comparisons of the mean AARC gain score between individuals whose comments were assigned to a category and individuals whose comments were not assigned to the category

Hedges’s g = effect size. 0.2, 0.5, and 0.8 are considered small, medium, and large effects, respectively. Degrees of freedom (df) = 607.

Those participants who reported feeling unchanged (medium effect size) or described purpose-driven experiences (small effect size) perceived fewer AARC losses than those participants whose comments were not assigned to these categories. Participants who reported experiencing negative changes (small effect size), fluctuating levels of awareness of changes (large effect size), and disregarding the impact of their age on the changes they are experiencing (small effect size), perceived more AARC losses than participants whose comments were not assigned to these categories. Regarding the remaining five categories, participants whose comments were assigned to a specific category did not differ significantly in levels of AARC losses from those participants whose comments were not assigned to the same category.

Discussion

This study is the first to examine and categorize the open-ended comments made by UK individuals aged over 50 years in relation to their responses on the AARC questionnaire. From analysis of participants’ comments about their AARC, we generated 10 categories, further grouped into 3 higher-order categories, suggesting that participants’ levels of AARC gains and losses were shaped by a wide range of life events and psychosocial factors. However, in some cases, scores on the AARC questionnaire could be influenced by bias related to completion of the AARC items. Some of the factors (categories) identified in relation to perceptions of AARC were in line with the variables outlined in the Diehl and Wahl (Reference Diehl and Wahl2010) conceptual framework of AARC, whereas others were novel and had not been previously considered as potential contributors to AARC, for example, engagement in purpose-driven activities or in activities that distract from age-related thoughts. Among the identified sources of inaccuracy related to the AARC questionnaire, perhaps the most important is that a sizeable proportion of participants scored high on AARC losses even though they did not interpret the experienced changes as being a consequence of aging. In the following paragraphs, the identified higher-order categories with their respective subcategories are discussed in relation to the Diehl and Wahl (Reference Diehl and Wahl2010) conceptual framework of AARC. The higher-order category subjective experiences while aging will be discussed first, followed by mental process of awareness of changes and factors impacting evaluation of awareness of changes. Finally, we will consider how study results provide possible interpretations for the associations of AARC with health outcomes.

Most of the categories that we identified (experiencing negative changes, experiencing positive changes, attitudes toward aging, and purpose-driven experiences) within the higher-order category subjective experiences while aging corroborate the hypothesized roles of health status, experience of limitations, current life events, age stereotyping, and personal goals as factors influencing the formation of AARC (Diehl and Wahl, Reference Diehl and Wahl2010; Sabatini et al., Reference Sabatini2021). However, our results suggest a more complex picture. Whereas so far theoretical and empirical literature considered abandoned goals and age-related roles as factors increasing awareness of age-related losses, we found that those individuals engaged in pursuing life goals (e.g. volunteering) perceived more gains and fewer losses than those not engaged in pursuing life goals. Hence, future interventions could enhance engagement in life goals as a way of promoting more positive experiences of aging. In line with Terror Management Theory (Burke et al., Reference Burke, Martens and Faucher2010), it may be that undertaking roles and hobbies typically assumed by older people increases awareness of mortality, negative emotions, and negative self-perceptions (including higher AARC losses) only when individuals are not engaged in activities that convey a high sense of purpose and self-efficacy. Terror Management Theory (Burke et al., Reference Burke, Martens and Faucher2010; Greenberg et al., Reference Greenberg, Pyszczynski, Solomon and Baumeister1986) indeed assumes that increasing age and death awareness give rise to anxiety only among those individuals who do not have a sense of purpose and/or high levels of self-efficacy.

Alternatively, it may be that the engagement in certain roles such as caring for grandchildren acts as a full-time occupation that reduces individuals’ time to reflect on and worry about negative changes (Bordone and Arpino, Reference Bordone and Arpino2015). Finally, some participants reported “feeling unchanged”; these individuals perceived fewer AARC gains and losses compared to those who did not feel unchanged. This empirically supports the hypothesis that those individuals who are less self-reflective may maintain a more consistent self-concept and hence report fewer AARC gains and losses, regardless of the amount of objective changes experienced (Diehl and Wahl, Reference Diehl and Wahl2010; Kaufman, Reference Kaufman1986). Future studies could explore whether levels of introspection mediate the effects of antecedents on subjective perceptions of AARC.

The second higher-order category mental process of awareness of changes corroborates Diehl and Wahls’ (Reference Diehl and Wahl2010) hypothesis that perceptions of AARC give rise to a series of psychological processes involving self-regulation and meaning-making. In line with the Two-Process Framework (Brandtstädter and Rothermund, Reference Brandtstädter and Rothermund2002), for some individuals AARC was a precondition for developing the intention to engage in an “healthy lifestyle and/or in self-regulation” (e.g. abandonment of old hobbies for less physically demanding ones). Moreover, these individuals scored higher on AARC gains compared to those who did not engage in such behaviors, confirming that perceived gains can be maintained through adaptation even when individuals experience negative changes (Baltes, Reference Baltes1987; Brandtstädter and Rothermund, Reference Brandtstädter and Rothermund2002; Wilton-Harding and Windsor, Reference Wilton-Harding and Windsor2021). For a small proportion of participants, AARC triggered favorable “social comparison” (Festinger, Reference Festinger1954), meaning that they compared themselves with someone experiencing more negative changes than them. Age-Stereotype Internalization and Dissociation Theory posits that becoming aware of age-related changes can give rise to concerns about meeting those negative age stereotypes that individuals have internalized early in life. Consequently, to counteract negative thoughts and worries, individuals often distance themselves from their peers through favorable social comparison (Weiss and Kornadt, Reference Weiss and Kornadt2018).

The last higher-order category factors impacting evaluation of awareness of changes incudes three categories covering potential sources of bias related to the use of the AARC questionnaire. Almost one-third of participants disregarded the impact of their increased age on the changes experienced, yet on average they perceived more AARC losses than participants whose comments did not fit into this category. This means that several individuals reported high AARC losses despite attributing their changes to other factors. As the concept of AARC (Diehl and Wahl, Reference Diehl and Wahl2010) implies that people are both aware of changes and also attribute such changes to their increased age, scores on the AARC questionnaire on some occasions may not accurately capture individuals’ AARC. Recent evidence confirms that the tendency to causally attribute changes to increased age differs among individuals (Rothermund et al., Reference Rothermund2021). As attributing changes to increased age enhances awareness of limited time to live (Burke et al., Reference Burke, Martens and Faucher2010), the tendency of some individuals with high levels of AARC losses to attribute changes to causes other than their age may be an emotional reaction to decrease the negative thoughts associated with awareness of mortality (Hobfoll, Reference Hobfoll2002; Leventhal et al., Reference Leventhal, Phillips and Burns2016; Rosenstock, Reference Rosenstock1974).

Participants’ comments suggest that difficulty when answering the AARC questionnaire could be reduced by including a “non-applicable” answer option, specifying the comparative time frame in the item stem, and for UK use, substitute some American English words with more common alternatives in British English. Finally, some individuals experienced fluctuating levels of awareness of changes depending on several aspects of their mental and physical health. Micro-longitudinal research studies also document day-to-day fluctuation in levels of AARC gains and losses within the same person (Neupert and Bellingtier, Reference Neupert and Bellingtier2017; Zhu and Neupert, Reference Zhu and Neupert2020). Daily fluctuation in AARC is related to fluctuation in other variables such as levels of positive and negative affect (Neupert and Bellingtier, Reference Neupert and Bellingtier2017) and control beliefs (Zhang and Neupert, Reference Zhang and Neupert2020). As daily fluctuation in AARC influences daily cognitive performance (O’Brien et al., Reference O’Brien, Torres and Neupert2020; Zhu and Neupert, Reference Zhu and Neupert2020), the fluctuating nature of AARC gains and losses may limit the predictive accuracy of the AARC questionnaire. It would, therefore, be important in future research to control for variables (e.g. mood) that account for variability in AARC.

Study results may help to explain some of the existing associations of AARC with health outcomes. First, in line with gerontological literature (Carstensen et al., Reference Carstensen2011; McGrath et al., Reference McGrath, Al Snih, Markides, Hall and Peterson2019; Miche et al., Reference Miche, Wahl, Diehl, Oswald, Kaspar and Kolb2014; Rubio et al., Reference Rubio, Dumitrache, Cordon-Pozo and Rubio-Herrera2016), participants’ comments show that with increasing age negative experiences mainly relate to physical health (e.g. recent illnesses), whereas positive experiences often relate to socio-emotional aspects (e.g. increased confidence and experience). Moreover, those participants who commented on experiencing negative changes scored higher on levels of AARC losses compared to those who did not commented on this category. This pattern of results may explain why more AARC losses are associated with worse mental and physical health, whereas more AARC gains are associated with higher mental health but not with physical health (Brothers et al., Reference Brothers, Gabrian, Wahl and Diehl2019; Sabatini et al., Reference Sabatini2020a; Reference Sabatini2020c).

Second, those participants who engaged in purpose-driven experiences (e.g. hobbies) perceived more AARC gains and fewer AARC losses than those participants not mentioning any engagement in activities. As high engagement in activities, high perceived gains, and low perceived losses are all related to better mental health, levels of engagement in activities may explain why individuals with high gains and low losses maintain good mental health over time (Chippendale, Reference Chippendale2013; Dutt and Wahl, Reference Dutt and Wahl2018; Grossman and Gruenewald, Reference Grossman and Gruenewald2017; Lindsay-Smith et al., Reference Lindsay-Smith, O’Sullivan, Eime, Harvey and Van Uffelen2018; Reference Lindsay-Smith, Eime, O’Sullivan, Harvey and van Uffelen2019; Simone and Haas, Reference Simone and Haas2013; Wilton-Harding and Windsor, Reference Wilton-Harding and Windsor2021). Third, the finding that some individuals attribute changes to increased age whereas others attribute them to illnesses raises the question of whether attributing changes to different factors leads to different behavioral reactions. A recent study showed that those individuals who attribute changes to aging perceive lower levels of control over the experienced changes (Rothermund et al., Reference Rothermund2021). As a consequence, it may be that these individuals are more likely to opt for passive coping strategies (e.g. acceptance of changes). Future studies on SPA could explore whether the attribution of changes to increased age (AARC) act as a moderator of the association of SPA with engagement in healthy and adaptive behaviors.

Strengths and limitations

This study has two limitations. First, as analyses are based on a self-selected sample, study participants may have had strong positive or negative feelings when answering the questionnaire (Ogletree and Katz, Reference Ogletree and Katz2020). Second, the sample included a majority of participants who were women, White, and well-educated. The study, however, also has strengths including a large sample and the conduct of both qualitative and quantitative analyses. Participants reported their spontaneous thoughts, and hence participants’ comments are not biased by the research questions or hypotheses.

Conclusion

A wide range of factors impact on perceptions of AARC. Some of these factors corroborate the variables outlined in the Diehl and Wahl (Reference Diehl and Wahl2010) conceptual framework of AARC. Others such as engagement in purpose-driven activities or in activities that distract from age-related thoughts are novel and could be added to the conceptual framework of AARC. Overall, results support the ability of the AARC questionnaire to capture subjective experiences of age-related changes in physical, mental, and cognitive health, engagement in social activities and in healthy and adaptive behaviors. However, as we identified some issues related to use of the AARC questionnaire, accuracy in assessing AARC could be increased through some minor adjustments to the questionnaire. Finally, study results raise new questions for future research; for instance, the AARC may act as a moderator of the associations of more general SPA with engagement in healthy and adaptive behaviors. It also remains to be explored whether engagement in activities mediates the associations of higher AARC gains and lower AARC losses with better health.

Conflict of interest

None.

Funding

This work was supported by the University of Exeter College of Life and Environmental Sciences (School of Psychology), University of Exeter College of Medicine and Health, and the National Health and Medical Research Council Centre for Research Excellence in Cognitive Health (#1100579 to Kaarin Anstey).

Description of authors’ roles

SS served as principal investigator of the research, designed the study, conducted data analyses, and took the lead in writing the manuscript.

LC contributed to the design of the study, analysis of data, and writing the manuscript.

OU contributed to analyses of data and provided feedback on the draft of the manuscript.

AC, HB, and CB contributed to data collection and design of the PROTECT study and provided feedback on the draft of the manuscript.

RC provided feedback on the draft of the manuscript.

Acknowledgements

The authors are grateful to the University of Exeter for funding a PhD scholarship for Serena Sabatini to carry out this work. This paper represents independent research funded by the National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) Biomedical Research Centre at South London and Maudsley NHS Foundation Trust and King’s College London. This research was also supported by the National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) Applied Research Collaboration (ARC) South-West Peninsula. The views expressed are those of the author(s) and not necessarily those of the NHS, the NIHR or the Department of Health. The authors are grateful to Jessica Rundle for contributing to the analysis as second rater.

Availability of data and materials

This study was conducted using secondary data collected as part of the UK version of the PROTECT ongoing study. PROTECT data are available to investigators outside the PROTECT team after request and approval by the PROTECT Steering Committee. Data for the AARC questionnaire will be available from May 2022.

Supplementary material

For supplementary material accompanying this paper visit https://doi.org/10.1017/S104161022100123X

References

Balcombe, N. R. and Sinclair, A. (2001). Ageing: definitions, mechanisms and the magnitude of the problem. Best Practice & Research Clinical Gastroenterology, 15, 835849. DOI 10.1053/bega.2001.0244.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baltes, P. B. (1987). Theoretical propositions of life-span developmental psychology: on the dynamics between growth and decline. Developmental Psychology, 23, 611626. DOI 10.1037/0012-1649.23.5.611.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barnett, K., Mercer, S. W., Norbury, M., Watt, G., Wyke, S. and Guthrie, B. (2012). Epidemiology of multimorbidity and implications for health care, research, and medical education: a cross-sectional study. The Lancet, 380, 3743. DOI 10.1016/S0140-6736(12)60240-2.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barrett, A. E. (2003). Socioeconomic status and age identity: the role of dimensions of health in the subjective construction of age. The Journals of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 58, 101109. DOI 10.1093/geronb/58.2.S101.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bordone, V. and Arpino, B. (2015). Do grandchildren influence how old you feel? Journal of Aging and Health, 28, 10551072. DOI 10.1177/0898264315618920.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brandtstädter, J. and Rothermund, K. (2002). The life-course dynamics of goal pursuit and goal adjustment: a two-process framework. Developmental Review, 22, 117150. DOI 10.1006/drev.2001.0539.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brothers, A. F., Gabrian, M., Wahl, H.-W. and Diehl, M. K. (2016). Future time perspective and awareness of age-related change: examining their role in predicting psychological well-being. Psychology and Aging, 31, 605617. DOI 10.1037/pag0000101.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brothers, A. F., Gabrian, M., Wahl, H.-W. and Diehl, M. K. (2019). A new multidimensional questionnaire to assess awareness of age-related change (AARC). The Gerontologist, 59, e141e151. DOI 10.1093/geront/gny006.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brothers, A. F., Kornadt, A. E., Nehrkorn-Bailey, A., Wahl, H.-W. and Diehl, M. K. (2020). The effects of age stereotypes on physical and mental health are mediated by self-perceptions of aging. The Journals of Gerontology, 76, 845857. DOI 10.1093/geronb/gbaa176.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brothers, A. F., Miche, M., Wahl, H.-W. and Diehl, M. K. (2017). Examination of associations among three distinct subjective aging constructs and their relevance for predicting developmental correlates. The Journals of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 72, 547560. DOI 10.1093/geronb/gbv085.Google ScholarPubMed
Burke, B. L., Martens, A. and Faucher, E. H. (2010). Two decades of terror management theory: a meta-analysis of mortality salience research. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 14, 155195. DOI 10.1177/1088868309352321.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carstensen, L. L. 1993. Motivation for social contact across the life span: A theory of socioemotional selectivity. In: Jacobs, J. E. (Ed.), Current Theory and Research in Motivation (pp. 209254). Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Carstensen, L. L. et al. (2011). Emotional experience improves with age: evidence based on over 10 years of experience sampling. Psychology and Aging, 26, 2133. DOI 10.1037/a0021285.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chippendale, T. (2013). Factors associated with depressive symptoms among elders in senior residences: the importance of feeling valued by others. Clinical Gerontologist, 36, 162169. DOI 10.1080/07317115.2012.749321.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohen, J. (1960). A coefficient of agreement for nominal scales. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 20, 3746.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Diehl, M. K. and Wahl, H.-W. (2010). Awareness of age-related change: examination of a (mostly) unexplored concept. The Journals of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 65B, S340S350. DOI 10.1093/geronb/gbp110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Diehl, M. K. et al. (2014). Awareness of aging: theoretical considerations on an emerging concept. Developmental Review, 34, 93113. DOI 10.1016/j.dr.2014.01.001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dutt, A. J., Gabrian, M. and Wahl, H.-W. (2016). Developmental regulation and awareness of age-related change: a (mostly) unexplored connection. The Journals of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 73, 934943. DOI 10.1093/geronb/gbw084.Google Scholar
Dutt, A. J. and Wahl, H.-W. (2018). Future time perspective and general self-efficacy mediate the association between awareness of age-related losses and depressive symptoms. European Journal of Ageing, 16, 227236. DOI 10.1007/s10433-018-0482-3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elliott, R., Fischer, C. T. and Rennie, D. L. (1999). Evolving guidelines for publication of qualitative research studies in psychology and related fields. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 38, 215229. DOI 10.1348/014466599162782.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
English, A. N., Bellingtier, J. A. and Neupert, S. D. (2019). It’s “the Joneses”: the influence of objective and subjective socioeconomic status on subjective perceptions of aging. European Journal of Ageing, 16, 121128. DOI 10.1007/s10433-018-0475-2.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Festinger, L. (1954). A theory of social comparison processes. Human Relations, 7, 117140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grbich, C. (2012). Qualitative Data Analysis: An Introduction. Los Angeles, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Greenberg, J., Pyszczynski, T. and Solomon, S. 1986. The causes and consequences of a need for self-esteem: A terror management theory. In: Baumeister, R. F. (Ed.), Public Self and Private Self (pp. 189192). New York, NY: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grossman, M. R. and Gruenewald, T. L. (2017). Caregiving and perceived generativity: a positive and protective aspect of providing care? Clinical Gerontologist, 40, 435447. DOI 10.1080/07317115.2017.1317686.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hobfoll, S. E. (2002). Social and psychological resources and adaptation. Review of General Psychology, 6, 307324. DOI 10.1037/1089-2680.6.4.307.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hsieh, C.-M. (2005). Age and relative importance of major life domains. Journal of Aging Studies, 19, 503512. DOI 10.1016/j.jaging.2005.07.001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jaconelli, A., Terracciano, A., Sutin, A. R., Sarrazin, P., Raffard, S. and Stephan, Y. (2017). Subjective age and dementia. Clinical Gerontologist, 40, 106113. DOI 10.1080/07317115.2016.1187695.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaspar, R., Gabrian, M., Brothers, A. F., Wahl, H.-W. and Diehl, M. K. (2019). Measuring awareness of age-related change: development of a 10-Item short form for use in large-scale surveys. The Gerontologist, 59, e130e140. DOI 10.1093/geront/gnx213.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaufman, S. R. (1986). The Ageless Self: Sources of Meaning in Late Life. Madison, WI: University of Wisconsin Press.Google Scholar
Kingston, A., Robinson, L., Booth, H., Knapp, M. and Jagger, C. (2018). Projections of multi-morbidity in the older population in England to 2035: estimates from the population ageing and care simulation (PACSim) model. Age and Ageing, 47, 374380. DOI 10.1093/ageing/afx201.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kornadt, A. E., Hess, T. M., Voss, P. and Rothermund, K. (2018). Subjective age across the life span: a differentiated, longitudinal approach. The Journals of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 73, 767777. DOI 10.1093/geronb/gbw072.Google ScholarPubMed
Kornadt, A. E., Kessler, E.-M., Wurm, S., Bowen, C. E., Gabrian, M. and Klusmann, V. (2019). Views on ageing: a lifespan perspective. European Journal of Ageing, 17, 387401. DOI 10.1007/s10433-019-00535-9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lakens, D. (2013). Calculating and reporting effect sizes to facilitate cumulative science: a practical primer for t-tests and ANOVAs. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 863. DOI 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00863.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lawton, M. P. (1975). The Philadelphia geriatric center morale scale: a revision. Journal of Gerontology, 30, 8589.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leventhal, H., Phillips, L. A. and Burns, E. (2016). The Common-Sense Model of Self-Regulation (CSM): a dynamic framework for understanding illness self-management. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 39, 935946. DOI 10.1007/s10865-016-9782-2.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levy, B. R. (2017). Age-stereotype paradox: opportunity for social change. The Gerontologist, 57, S118S126. DOI 10.1093/geront/gnx059.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lindsay-Smith, G., Eime, R., O’Sullivan, G., Harvey, J. and van Uffelen, J. G. Z. (2019). A mixed-methods case study exploring the impact of participation in community activity groups for older adults on physical activity, health and wellbeing. BMC Geriatrics, 19, 243. DOI 10.1186/s12877-019-1245-5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lindsay-Smith, G., O’Sullivan, G., Eime, R., Harvey, J. and Van Uffelen, J. G. Z. (2018). A mixed methods case study exploring the impact of membership of a multi-activity, multicentre community group on social wellbeing of older adults. BMC Geriatrics, 18, 226. DOI 10.1186/s12877-018-0913-1.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGrath, R., Al Snih, S., Markides, K., Hall, O. and Peterson, M. (2019). The burden of health conditions for middle-aged and older adults in the United States: disability-adjusted life years. BMC Geriatrics, 19, 100. DOI 10.1186/s12877-019-1110-6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miche, M., Wahl, H.-W., Diehl, M. K., Oswald, F., Kaspar, R. and Kolb, M. (2014). Natural occurrence of subjective aging experiences in community-dwelling older adults. The Journals of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 69, 174187. DOI 10.1093/geronb/gbs164.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Montepare, J. M. (2019). An exploration of subjective age, actual age, age awareness, and engagement in everyday behaviors. European Journal of Ageing, 17, 299307. DOI 10.1007/s10433-019-00534-w.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Neuman, W. L. (2011). Social Research Methods: Qualitative and Quantitative Approaches. Whitewater, WI: Pearson.Google Scholar
Neupert, S. D. and Bellingtier, J. A. (2017). Aging attitudes and daily awareness of age-related change interact to predict negative affect. The Gerontologist, 57, S187S192. DOI 10.1093/geront/gnx055.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O’Brien, E. L., Torres, G. E. and Neupert, S. D. (2020). Cognitive interference in the context of daily stressors, daily awareness of age-related change, and general aging attitudes. The Journals of Gerontology, Psychological Sciences: Series B, 76(5), 920929. DOI 10.1093/geronb/gbaa155.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ogletree, A. and Katz, B. (2020). How do older adults recruited using MTurk differ from those in a national probability sample? The International Journal of Aging and Human Development, 93(2), 700721. DOI 10.1177/0091415020940197.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Polit, D. F. and Beck, C. T. (2004). Nursing Research: Principles and Methods. Philadelphia, PA: Lippincott Williams and Wilkins.Google Scholar
Pope, C., Ziebland, S. and Mays, N. (2000). Analysing qualitative data. BMJ, 320, 114126. DOI 10.1136/bmj.320.7227.114.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Richards, L. (2015). Handling Qualitative Data: A Practical Guide. London: Sage.Google Scholar
Rosenstock, I. M. (1974). Historical origins of the health belief model. Health Education Monographs, 2, 328335. DOI 10.1177/109019817400200403.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rothermund, K. et al. (2021). Age-related attributions of experienced changes in life: origins and implications. The Journals of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 76, 881893. DOI 10.1093/geronb/gbaa160.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Royall, D. R., Lauterbach, E. C., Kaufer, D., Malloy, P., Coburn, K. L. and Black, K. J. (2007). The cognitive correlates of functional status: a review from the Committee on Research of the American Neuropsychiatric Association. The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 19, 249265. DOI 10.1176/jnp.2007.19.3.249.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rubio, L., Dumitrache, C., Cordon-Pozo, E. and Rubio-Herrera, R. (2016). Coping: impact of gender and stressful life events in middle and in old age. Clinical Gerontologist, 39, 468488. DOI 10.1080/07317115.2015.1132290.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sabatini, S. et al. (2020a). Associations of awareness of age-related change with emotional and physical well-being: a systematic review and meta-analysis. The Gerontologist, 60, e477e490. DOI 10.1093/geront/gnz101.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sabatini, S. et al. (2020b). International relevance of two measures of awareness of age-related change (AARC). BMC Geriatrics, 20, 359. DOI 10.1186/s12877-020-01767-6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sabatini, S. et al. (2020c). The cross-sectional relationship between pain and awareness of age-related changes. British Journal of Pain, 15(3), 335344. DOI 10.1177/2049463720961798.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sabatini, S. et al. (2021). Cross-sectional association between objective cognitive performance and perceived age-related gains and losses in cognition. International Psychogeriatrics, 33, 727741. DOI 10.1017/S1041610221000375.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sabatini, S. et al. (In Press). What does feeling younger or older than one’s chronological age mean to men and women? Qualitative and quantitative findings from the PROTECT study. Psychology and Health.Google Scholar
Sandelowski, M. and Barroso, J. (2003). Classifying the findings in qualitative studies. Qualitative Health Research, 13, 905923. DOI 10.1177/1049732303253488.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simone, P. M. and Haas, A. L. (2013). Frailty, leisure activity and functional status in older adults: relationship with subjective well being. Clinical Gerontologist, 36, 275293. DOI 10.1080/07317115.2013.788114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Steverink, N., Westerhof, G. J., Bode, C. and Dittmann-Kohli, F. (2001). The personal experience of aging, individual resources, and subjective well-being. The Journals of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 56, 364373. DOI 10.1093/geronb/56.6.P364.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Teal, R. et al. (2015). Applying cognitive interviewing to inform measurement of partnership readiness: a new approach to strengthening community-academic research. Progress in Community Health Partnerships: Research, Education, and Action, 9, 513519. DOI 10.1353/cpr.2015.0083.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Timmer, E., Steverink, N. and Dittmann-Kohli, F. (2002). Cognitive representations of future gains, maintenance, and losses in the second half of life. The International Journal of Aging and Human Development, 55, 321339. DOI 10.1177/009141500205500401.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
United Nations. (2019). Levels of trends in population ageing. https://www.unsecretariat.net/sections/issues-depth/ageing/index.html.Google Scholar
Voss, P., Kornadt, A. E., Hess, T. M., Fung, H. H. and Rothermund, K. (2018). A world of difference? Domain-specific views on aging in China, the US, and Germany. Psychology and Aging, 33, 595606. DOI 10.1037/pag0000237.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weiss, D. and Kornadt, A. E. (2018). Age-stereotype internalization and dissociation: contradictory processes or two sides of the same coin? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 27, 477483. DOI 10.1177/0963721418777743.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Westerhof, G. J. et al. (2014). The influence of subjective aging on health and longevity: a meta-analysis of longitudinal data. Psychology and Aging, 29, 793802. DOI 10.1037/a0038016.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whittemore, R., Chase, S. K. and Mandle, C. L. (2001). Validity in qualitative research. Qualitative Health Research, 11, 522537. DOI 10.1177/104973201129119299.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilton-Harding, B. and Windsor, T. D. (2021). Awareness of age-related change, future time perspective, and implications for goal adjustment in older adulthood. Aging & Mental Health, 19. DOI 10.1080/13607863.2021.1893269.Google ScholarPubMed
Zhang, S. and Neupert, S. D. (2020). Within-and between-person relationships among health, awareness of aging, and control beliefs: a micro-longitudinal study. The Journals of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 39(2), 282298. DOI 10.1093/geronb/gbaa180.Google Scholar
Zhu, X. and Neupert, S. D. (2020). Dynamic awareness of age-related losses predict concurrent and subsequent changes in daily inductive reasoning performance. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 39, 282298. DOI 10.1111/bjdp.12344.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Demographic characteristics of PROTECT participants who answered and who did not answer the AARC open-ended question

Figure 1

Figure 1. The thematic map illustrates the hypothesis of how the 10 categories identified through content analyses may be interrelated. Categories are organized into predictors of AARC (listed under the heading “subjective experiences while aging”), factors explaining lack of accuracy in AARC (listed under the heading “factors impacting evaluation of awareness of changes”), and factors involved in the mental process of AARC.

Figure 2

Table 2. Examples of participants’ comments per each of the identified categories

Figure 3

Table 3. Number (%) of the study sample who provided responses to the open-ended AARC question that fell into specific categories

Figure 4

Table 4a. For each of the 10 categories, the table summarizes comparisons of the mean AARC gain score between individuals whose comments were assigned to a category and individuals whose comments were not assigned to the category

Figure 5

Table 4b. For each of the 10 categories, the table summarizes comparisons of the mean AARC gain score between individuals whose comments were assigned to a category and individuals whose comments were not assigned to the category

Supplementary material: File

Sabatini et al. supplementary material

Sabatini et al. supplementary material 1

Download Sabatini et al. supplementary material(File)
File 17.2 KB
Supplementary material: File

Sabatini et al. supplementary material

Sabatini et al. supplementary material 2

Download Sabatini et al. supplementary material(File)
File 71.4 KB