Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-8ctnn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-27T07:34:50.918Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Stressful life events increase the risk of major depressive episodes: A population-based twin study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  03 August 2022

Ludvig D. Bjørndal*
Affiliation:
PROMENTA Research Center, Department of Psychology, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway
Kenneth S. Kendler
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Virginia Commonwealth University School of Medicine, Richmond, VA, USA Department of Human and Molecular Genetics, Virginia Commonwealth University School of Medicine, Richmond, VA, USA
Ted Reichborn-Kjennerud
Affiliation:
Department of Mental Disorders, Norwegian Institute of Public Health, Oslo, Norway Institute of Clinical Medicine, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway
Eivind Ystrom
Affiliation:
PROMENTA Research Center, Department of Psychology, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway Department of Mental Disorders, Norwegian Institute of Public Health, Oslo, Norway School of Pharmacy, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway
*
Author for correspondence: Ludvig D. Bjørndal, E-mail: l.d.bjorndal@psykologi.uio.no
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Previous studies have found that stressful life events (SLEs) are associated with an increased risk of adult depression. However, many studies are observational in nature and limited by methodological issues, such as potential confounding by genetic factors. Genetically informative research, such as the co-twin control design, can strengthen causal inference in observational studies. Discrete-time survival analysis has several benefits and multilevel survival analysis can incorporate frailty terms (random effects) to estimate the components of the biometric model. In the current study, we investigated associations between SLEs and depression risk in a population-based twin sample (N = 2299).

Methods

A co-twin control design was used to investigate the influence of the occurrence of SLEs on depression risk. The co-twin control design involves comparing patterns of associations in the full sample and within dizygotic (DZ) and monozygotic twins (MZ). Associations were modelled using discrete-time survival analysis with biometric frailty terms. Data from two time points were used in the analyses. Mean age at Wave 1 was 28 years and mean age at Wave 2 was 38 years.

Results

SLE occurrence was associated with increased depression risk. Co-twin control analyses indicated that this association was at least in part due to the causal influence of SLE exposure on depression risk for event occurrence across all SLEs and for violent SLEs. A minor proportion of the total genetic risk of depression reflected genetic effects related to SLEs.

Conclusions

The results support previous research in implicating SLEs as important risk factors with probable causal influence on depression risk.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Stressful life events (SLEs) are environmental factors that increase the risk of depression (Kessler, Reference Kessler1997; Tennant, Reference Tennant2002). Many different events can be classified as stressful, such as experiencing assault, job loss or sexual abuse. All of these SLEs have been implicated as risk factors for depression in genetically informative studies (Kendler et al., Reference Kendler, Kessler, Walters, MacLean, Neale, Heath and Eaves1995, Reference Kendler, Bulik, Silberg, Hettema, Myers and Prescott2000; Kendler & Aggen, Reference Kendler and Aggen2014; Kendler & Gardner, Reference Kendler and Gardner2001; Kendler, Karkowski, & Prescott, Reference Kendler, Karkowski and Prescott1999a). SLEs that occur during childhood, adolescence and adulthood are all associated with an increased risk of depression (Sjöholm, Lavebratt, & Forsell, Reference Sjöholm, Lavebratt and Forsell2009; Tennant, Reference Tennant2002).

Research on exposure to risk factors for illness is typically observational in nature due to ethical or practical constraints. Many observational studies are limited by methodological issues, such as potential confounding by genetic or environmental factors and reporting bias. Genetically informative samples can strengthen causal inference in observational research (Pingault et al., Reference Pingault, O'Reilly, Schoeler, Ploubidis, Rijsdijk and Dudbridge2018). The co-twin control design focuses on twins discordant for exposure to risk factors or outcomes and is one strategy for increasing control over potential confounders.

Findings from several genetically informative studies have indicated that the associations between SLEs and depression in part reflect the causal influence of exposure on depression risk (Kendler et al., Reference Kendler, Karkowski and Prescott1999a; Kendler & Aggen, Reference Kendler and Aggen2014; Kendler & Gardner, Reference Kendler and Gardner2010). However, it has been found that associations may in part reflect non-causal influence or confounding by genetic or familial factors (Brown et al., Reference Brown, Berenz, Aggen, Gardner, Knudsen, Reichborn-Kjennerud and Amstadter2014; Kendler et al., Reference Kendler, Karkowski and Prescott1999a; Silberg et al., Reference Silberg, Copeland, Linker, Moore, Roberson-Nay and York2016). These conflicting findings warrant further studies of the relationship between SLE exposure and depression risk with genetically informative samples. Attaining a better understanding of the nature of the influences of SLE exposure on depression risk can have implications for both efforts to prevent depression and for clinical practice.

In the current study, we investigated associations between SLEs and depression risk in a population-based sample of twins (N = 2299). Three primary research questions were examined: (a) Is the occurrence of any SLEs, relational violent SLEs (i.e. SLEs for which another person is responsible), non-relational other traumatic SLEs (i.e. SLEs for which another person is not necessarily responsible), and/or economic SLEs associated with increased risk of depression?; (b) can indicated causal influence of the different groups of SLEs on depression risk be detected in co-twin control analyses?; and (c) how much of the total genetic risk of depression can be explained by the genetic risk associated with SLEs?

Methods

Sample

Participants were recruited from the Norwegian Institute of Public Health Twin Panel (NIPHTP). The NIPHTP has been described at length elsewhere (Harris, Magnus, & Tambs, Reference Harris, Magnus and Tambs2002, Reference Harris, Magnus and Tambs2006; Nilsen et al., Reference Nilsen, Knudsen, Gervin, Brandt, Røysamb, Tambs and Harris2013). Zygosity was determined by a combination of questionnaire items and genotyping.

Data came from a population-based study of psychiatric disorders among NIPHTP participants with two waves of data collection. From 1999 to 2004 (W1), psychiatric disorders were measured in a subsample of NIPHTP participants. In total, psychiatric interviews were conducted with 2801 twins. Mean age at W1 was 28.2 years (range 19–36). At W1, there were 449 MZ female twin pairs, 220 male MZ twin pairs, 263 female DZ twin pairs, 118 male DZ twin pairs, 341 DZ pairs of the opposite sex and 19 single twins.

Wave 2 (W2) of data collection was initiated in 2010 (Nilsen et al., Reference Nilsen, Knudsen, Gervin, Brandt, Røysamb, Tambs and Harris2013). Mean age at W2 was 37.8 years (range 30–44). Clinical diagnostic interviews were completed on 2284 of the twins interviewed during W1. At W2, there were 358 female MZ twin pairs, 154 male MZ twin pairs, 180 female DZ twin pairs, 76 male DZ twin pairs, 219 DZ twin pairs of the opposite sex, and 310 single twins. Data on occurrences of SLEs was collected using a questionnaire, which was based on a measure which has been used to record SLE occurrences in previous studies (Kendler et al., Reference Kendler, Kessler, Walters, MacLean, Neale, Heath and Eaves1995). In total, 2299 twins responded to the questionnaire at W2. We report additional characteristics of the sample in the online Supplementary Materials.

All participants gave their written informed consent to participate in the study. Approval was sought and received from the Regional Committee for Medical and Health Research Ethics (#2010/767).

Measurements

Psychiatric interviews were conducted using the Munich-Composite International Diagnostic Interview (M-CIDI; Wittchen and Pfister, Reference Wittchen and Pfister1997). M-CIDI assesses DSM-IV and ICD-10 diagnoses and has good validity and reliability (Reed et al., Reference Reed, Gander, Pfister, Steiger, Sonntag, Trenkwalder and Wittchen1998; Wittchen, Lachner, Wunderlich, & Pfister, Reference Wittchen, Lachner, Wunderlich and Pfister1998). Interviewers were primarily clinical psychology students in the final part of their degree or psychiatric nurses, all of whom attended standardised CIDI training. Interviews were largely conducted in person (8.3% of the interviews were conducted by telephone for practical reasons) in W1. All interviews were conducted via telephone in W2. Prevalence of DSM-IV Major Depressive Episode (MDE) is listed in Table 1.

Table 1. Depression diagnoses at Wave 1 and Wave 2

Subthreshold diagnoses were treated as diagnoses of depression for the primary analyses to maximise statistical power. Subthreshold diagnoses in M-CIDI are assigned when an individual meets all mandatory diagnostic criteria except for one (Wittchen et al., Reference Wittchen, Lachner, Wunderlich and Pfister1998). Analyses with only threshold diagnoses yielded similar results (see online Supplementary Appendix A). MDE diagnoses were recorded per year of each participant's life. The median age of MDE onset in Wave 1 was 21 years. The median age of MDE onset in Wave 2 was 29 years.

The SLE questionnaire included 18 SLEs. If a participant answered yes for any SLE, he or she was asked to enter the age (in years) when the event was experienced (or when it was first experienced if it had occurred multiple times). The frequencies of all 18 SLEs in the sample are listed in Table 2.

Table 2. Frequencies of stressful life events

Note. The table lists the frequency of SLEs in the full sample, SLEs that occurred before a depression diagnosis reported in W1, and SLEs that occurred before a depression diagnosis reported in W2. Median age for SLE occurrence (of time-censored SLEs) is reported in years.

The SLEs were aggregated in four groups for statistical analyses. All SLEs included all 18 SLEs listed in Table 2 and represented the overall effect of event exposure across all SLEs. Further grouping of SLEs was based on a distinction between relational violent SLEs (i.e. SLEs for which another person is responsible), non-relational violent SLEs, and economic or financial SLEs. The SLEs of the current study did not fit neatly into existing typologies such as the independent/dependent SLE categorisation (Kendler, Karkowski, & Prescott, Reference Kendler, Karkowski and Prescott1999b). Distinctions between relational and non-relational SLEs and economic SLEs have been used in some previous studies (Anders, Shallcross, & Frazier, Reference Anders, Shallcross and Frazier2012; Stone et al., Reference Stone, Diop, Declercq, Cabral, Fox and Wise2015; Ward, Kanu, & Robb, Reference Ward, Kanu and Robb2017).

Relational violent SLEs included five SLEs: threatened with a weapon, held captive or kidnapped; sexual abuse; rape; otherwise physically attacked or assaulted; and otherwise physically abused as a child. Non-relational other traumatic SLEs included three SLEs: life-threatening accident; actively participated in war/combat; and witnessed anyone be badly injured or killed. Economic SLEs included two SLEs: long-term financial difficulties and unemployment for more than six months.

The biometric model and the co-twin control design

Twin designs are based on the varying genetic relatedness between monozygotic (MZ) twins, who share 100% of their genetic material, and dizygotic (DZ) twins, who share 50% of their genetic material. The basic notion is that, if genetic influences are important for variation in a phenotype, MZ twins will be (on average) more similar than DZ twins for the given phenotype. The biometric model explains phenotypic variation as influenced by additive genetic factors (A), non-additive genetic effects (D), shared environmental factors (C) and non-shared environmental factors (E) (Eaves, Last, Young, & Martin, Reference Eaves, Last, Young and Martin1978; Fisher, Reference Fisher1918; Jinks & Fulker, Reference Jinks and Fulker1970).

The co-twin control design represents one approach to studying exposure to environmental factors that cannot be experimentally manipulated using twin data. The design compares associations in the full sample and for discordant twins. As MZ and DZ twins are matched for the early shared environment, and either partially (DZ) or fully (MZ) for genetic material, this increases control over potential confounding by these factors. The co-twin control design can be understood within a counterfactual model of causality (McAdams, Rijsdijk, Zavos, & Pingault, Reference McAdams, Rijsdijk, Zavos and Pingault2021).

Patterns of associations in the full sample and in within-pair estimates for MZ and DZ twins help discern if environmental exposure may have a causal influence on the outcome. If observed associations are equally strong in the full sample and for discordant DZ and MZ twins, this is indicative of causal influence, as within-pair estimates control for shared environmental and genetic factors (partially for DZ twins and fully for MZ twins). Weaker associations within twins compared with the full sample are indicative of confounding by early shared environment. If the associations weaken with genetic similarity (i.e. strongest in the full sample and weakest in MZ twins), this is indicative of genetic confounding, which is controlled for to a varying extent in within-pair estimates.

Discrete-time survival analysis

Associations were modelled using discrete-time survival analysis. Survival analysis has several benefits, including the opportunity to incorporate right-censored observations (i.e. when the time to event is larger than the period of data collection) in estimates of event probabilities and study how the risk of events of interest is distributed over time (Hox, Reference Hox2010). In the discrete-time survival model, a hypothetical coefficient β 1 can be interpreted as the change in the logit of the hazard, following a one unit increase in the corresponding covariate. This effect is independent of specific time intervals. Multilevel survival models can incorporate random effects (often called frailty; Austin, Reference Austin2017).

Statistical analyses

Analyses were conducted using discrete-time survival analysis with frailty terms to estimate the components of the biometric model. The dataset was converted to a ‘long format’, which is required for survival analysis (Hox, Reference Hox2010). SLEs that had occurred after the onset of a depression diagnosis were time-censored (i.e. given the value of 0), so that only SLEs that occurred before MDE diagnoses were included in analyses. Only first onset MDE diagnoses for each interview were used in the analyses (i.e. only the first diagnosis was included in the analysis if an individual reported multiple depressive diagnoses, but participants were allowed to report different depressive episodes in W1 and W2).

A parameterisation for between- and within-family effects proposed by Rabe-Hesketh, Skrondal, and Gjessing (Reference Rabe-Hesketh, Skrondal and Gjessing2008) was applied to estimate the biometric components, with some modifications. Random effects were added at three levels: A random slope at the level of the twin pairs, shared by all twin pairs; a random slope at the level of ‘zygosity’, shared only by MZ twins; and one random effect at the level of participants, capturing residual dependence between measures from the same individual. The variables estimated as random slopes were given the value of $\sqrt {0.5}$. This parameterisation ensures that the co-variance for MZ and DZ twins differs depending on the type of twin.

The described model estimates the AE components. The ACE model was estimated by including an additional random intercept capturing residual dependence between individuals within the same twin pair not captured by genetic effects. The CE model was estimated by including the additional intercept and dropping the Level 2 and Level 3 random effects. The ADE model was not estimated as previous studies have not found evidence of non-additive genetic effects for depression (Kendler & Prescott, Reference Kendler and Prescott1999; Kendler, Neale, Kessler, Heath, & Eaves, Reference Kendler, Neale, Kessler, Heath and Eaves1992).

Two constraints were used in the model. The first constrained the residual variance of the random slopes representing genetic effects at Level 2 and Level 3 to be equal. The second constrained the surplus within and the ‘sparsity’ between fixed effects of the DZ twins to be the inverse of each other (these can be constrained to be the inverse of each other as both represent the effects of 0.5A).

The random effects were added in a generalised linear mixed model (GLMM) with a binomial distribution and a logit link function. The variance of the random effects can be compared against the residual variance (or error) at the lowest-level, which is (π 2)/3 for the logistic distribution (Hox, Reference Hox2010). This allows for estimating the biometric components. The intraclass correlation for the models can then be calculated using the estimates of the random effects, i.e., the between estimate divided by the total variance, which is the sum of the between estimate plus the residual variance.

Age of onset was fitted as a fixed effect and inspected up to the third order polynomial, after which it did no longer contribute to model fit. Age of onset, sex and wave of measurement (W1 or W2) were included as fixed effects. The dependent variable was having received a depression diagnosis at any time point. Models were evaluated using AIC (Akaike, Reference Akaike1987). The ACE model did not converge to estimate a confidence interval for the intercept estimating the C component (AIC = 8715.203), likely because this variance parameter was very close to 0 or negative. The C component could be dropped without deterioration in model fit for the AE model (AIC = 8715.203). The CE model did not improve model fit (AIC = 8719.587). Therefore, the AE model served as the basis for subsequent models that incorporated the effects of SLEs.

Results

Model with no SLEs

Table 3 reports the results for the fixed and random effects in the AE model with no SLEs.

Table 3. Fixed and random effects of the AE model

The fixed effects of time t determine the baseline hazard rate, i.e., the hazard when all covariates are zero (Rabe-Hesketh & Skrondal, Reference Rabe-Hesketh and Skrondal2008). Risk of depression increased over time but was slightly reduced at later time points (i.e. older ages) in the sample. The hazard rate peaked around age 30 and depression risk was higher for females.

The heritability estimate for receiving a first subthreshold or threshold MDE diagnosis in any given year was 24%. By excluding the lowest-level error term, the heritability of lifetime risk of depression was estimated at 58%. This latter approach is asymptotic to the ‘measurement model’ of psychometrics, where random measurement error is excluded from effect estimates (Kendler, Neale, Kessler, Heath, & Eaves, Reference Kendler, Neale, Kessler, Heath and Eaves1993; van den Berg, Glas, & Boomsma, Reference van den Berg, Glas and Boomsma2007).

Stable and unstable environmental effects on depression risk were disentangled by dividing the estimated σ E and the lowest-level variance by the total variance. In any given year in the current sample, depression risk was explained by 24% genetic influences, 17% stable (time-invariant) environmental influences, and 59% unstable (time-variant) environmental influences including measurement error.

Co-twin control analyses

Table 4 reports the results of co-twin control analyses of associations between grouped SLEs and depression risk.

Table 4. Results of co-twin control analyses displaying hORs

Note. hOR refers to hazard odds ratio (Petras et al., Reference Petras, Masyn, Buckley, Ialongo and Kellam2011). Associations were statistically significant if corresponding p values were below a threshold of α = 0.05

The occurrence of one SLE (across all 18 SLEs) was associated with increased depression risk in the full sample. The associations were similar in size for DZ twins and MZ twins.

For violent SLEs, the occurrence of one SLE was associated with increased depression risk in the full sample and for MZ twins. The within-pair association for DZ twins was slightly attenuated compared with the within-pair estimate for MZ twins and was not statistically significant. For other traumatic SLEs, the occurrence of one SLE was not associated with an increased risk of depression in the full sample, nor for MZ twins or DZ twins. For economic SLEs, the occurrence of one event was associated with increased depression risk in the full sample, and stronger associations were observed for MZ twins and for DZ twins. The time-censoring means that the SLEs predicted future depression risk – no backward associations could occur.

Table 5 displays the residual random effects estimated in the models incorporating the various SLEs. Heritability was estimated before and after controlling for SLEs. The reduction in σA after including SLEs reflects how much of the total genetic risk of depression which was explained by genetic effects linked to SLEs, in the present sample. After including all of the SLEs, the heritability of depression went from 58% to 50%.

Table 5. Residual random effects for the models with grouped SLEs

Note. Table 5 displays residual random effects in the AE models incorporating the effects of SLEs and the reduction in additive genetic effects after including SLEs in the models (in percentages). These were compared to the random effects estimated in the AE model with no SLEs, listed in Table 3.

Discussion

Overall, SLE occurrence was associated with an increased risk of depression for all 18 SLEs, violent SLEs and economic SLEs in the full sample. For all SLEs and violent SLEs, co-twin control analyses indicated that these associations at least in part reflect the causal influence of SLE exposure on depression risk. The occurrence of other traumatic SLEs was not associated with increased depression risk. A small proportion of the genetic risk of depression reflected genetic effects linked to SLEs.

The results of our co-twin analyses of the occurrence of all SLEs and violent SLEs are in accordance with previous genetically informative studies that have found that the association between dependent SLEs and depression risk is likely substantially causal in nature (Kendler et al., Reference Kendler, Karkowski and Prescott1999a; Kendler & Gardner, Reference Kendler and Gardner2010). Almost all events in the current study were personal SLEs (i.e., SLEs that occur to individuals, in contrast to SLEs occurring to people within an individual's social network): indicated causal influence of exposure to personal SLEs on depression risk has also been found previously (Kendler et al., Reference Kendler, Karkowski and Prescott1999a). We find that occurrences of violent SLEs increase depression risk when controlling for shared genetic and environmental factors, which aligns with previous studies which have assessed associations between various forms of trauma and depression risk (Brown et al., Reference Brown, Berenz, Aggen, Gardner, Knudsen, Reichborn-Kjennerud and Amstadter2014; McCutcheon et al., Reference McCutcheon, Heath, Nelson, Bucholz, Madden and Martin2009).

Causal influence on the risk of depression has been suggested for a number of specific SLEs included in the current study, such as assault (Kendler et al., Reference Kendler, Karkowski and Prescott1999a, Reference Kendler, Kessler, Walters, MacLean, Neale, Heath and Eaves2010), sexual abuse (Kendler et al., Reference Kendler, Bulik, Silberg, Hettema, Myers and Prescott2000; Kendler & Aggen, Reference Kendler and Aggen2014; Nelson et al., Reference Nelson, Heath, Madden, Cooper, Dinwiddie, Bucholz and Martin2002), and romantic problems and job loss (Kendler & Gardner, Reference Kendler and Gardner2001). Our results are in agreement with the findings of with these previous studies, although we analysed aggregated SLEs. Evidence of a causal effect of maltreatment in childhood on the risk of major depression was also found in a recent genome-wide association study (GWAS) meta-analysis using Mendelian Randomisation (Warrier et al., Reference Warrier, Kwong, Luo, Dalvie, Croft, Sallis and Cecil2021). Some SLEs, like sexual abuse, affect women and men with different frequencies (Barth, Bermetz, Heim, Trelle, & Tonia, Reference Barth, Bermetz, Heim, Trelle and Tonia2013).

No associations between the occurrence of other traumatic SLEs (i.e. life-threatening accident, participated in the war, witnessed anyone be badly injured or killed) and risk of depression were observed in the current study. This is not in accordance with results from previous genetically informative studies, which have found increased depression risk associated with combat exposure (Koenen et al., Reference Koenen, Lyons, Goldberg, Simpson, Williams, Toomey and Tsuang2003) and somatic illness and injury (Kendler et al., Reference Kendler, Karkowski and Prescott1999a). While it is not clear why there is an absence of associations between other traumatic SLEs and depression risk in the current study, we note that this aggregate included fewer SLEs compared with violent and all SLEs and was substantially more heterogeneous compared with the other aggregated SLEs.

For economic SLEs, strong associations were observed between event occurrence and increased risk of depression in the full sample. This is in line with previous genetically informative studies that have found increased depression risk associated with financial problems and job loss (Kendler et al., Reference Kendler, Karkowski and Prescott1999a; Reference Kendler, Kessler, Walters, MacLean, Neale, Heath and Eaves2010; Kendler & Gardner, Reference Kendler and Gardner2001; Reference Kendler and Gardner2014).

However, observed associations between economic SLEs and risk of depression were stronger in within-pair estimates for MZ and DZ twins compared with the full sample. It is not clear why these results were found for economic SLEs. Two possible explanations are proposed. Firstly, it may be the case that this association was suppressed in the full sample due to confounding. This could have resulted if there was an inverse effect of the confounder(s) on exposure and outcome, which masked or suppressed an effect in the full sample (e.g. if familial factors associated with less economic problems were associated with increased depression risk, this could suppress the full sample association).

A second possible explanation is that the within-pair associations were inflated due to non-shared confounders in twin pairs. Stronger associations in within-pair estimates than the full sample can result when confounding variable(s) within twin pairs have lower correlation than the correlation of exposure to a risk factor within twin pairs (Frisell, Öberg, Kuja-Halkola, & Sjölander, Reference Frisell, Öberg, Kuja-Halkola and Sjölander2012). As discordant twins are selectively chosen to be dissimilar, this implies that they differ in non-shared causes of exposure to risk factors (i.e. a twin pair discordant for exposure is likely less similar than a twin pair concordant for exposure). If a confounder increases the probability of exposure to economic SLEs and depression, an unexposed twin would be less likely to possess the confounder and become depressed, as the confounder increases the risk of depression. This can potentially inflate within-pair estimates.

Several previous genetically informative studies have implicated economic SLEs as risk factors for depression, but not specifically reported on within-pair estimates of the associations (Kendler et al., Reference Kendler, Karkowski and Prescott1999a, Reference Kendler, Kessler, Walters, MacLean, Neale, Heath and Eaves2010; Kendler & Halberstadt, Reference Kendler and Halberstadt2013). Although the inflated within-pair estimates in our study are difficult to interpret, findings from some previous genetically informative studies indicate that this association may be at least in part causal. Using a similar design as in the current study, Kendler and Gardner (Reference Kendler and Gardner2001) found that MZ twins with a history of depression reported higher rates of job loss compared with non-depressed co-twins. Kendler and Gardner (Reference Kendler and Gardner2014) found that the association between financial and occupational difficulties and depression risk were stronger in male twins discordant for depression compared with female twins.

Limitations

The current study has some potential limitations. Although the co-twin control design increases control over genetic and shared environmental confounders, it is unable to control for non-shared confounders that only affect one twin, which can bias estimated effects (Frisell et al., Reference Frisell, Öberg, Kuja-Halkola and Sjölander2012; Kendler & Gardner, Reference Kendler and Gardner2010). We focused on the main effects of SLEs. Previous studies have, however, found genotype-environment interactions for SLEs and depression (e.g. Choi et al. Reference Choi, Chen, Ursano, Sun, Jain, Kessler and Smoller2020; Mandelli & Serretti, Reference Mandelli and Serretti2013). The current study was retrospective in nature. Therefore, the possible influence of recall bias (i.e. that current depression or mood states affects correct reporting of previously experienced SLEs or depressive episodes) cannot be excluded. Participants were censored after the first onset of a MDE, and so we did not study the effects of SLEs on recurrent depression. While we adjusted for sex in the statistical models, we did not investigate if there were different causes of life events and depression by gender. Previous studies have indicated that men and women share most, but not all, genetic factors that influence the risk of depression (Kendler, Ohlsson, Lichtenstein, Sundquist, & Sundquist, Reference Kendler, Ohlsson, Lichtenstein, Sundquist and Sundquist2018; Sullivan, Neale, & Kendler, Reference Sullivan, Neale and Kendler2000). Similarly, one previous study found a substantial genetic correlation between men and women for genetic factors influencing the propensity to experience personal SLEs (Bolinskey, Neale, Jacobson, Prescott, & Kendler, Reference Bolinskey, Neale, Jacobson, Prescott and Kendler2004). A number of limitations apply to our measurement of SLEs. Several SLE categories were relatively broad, e.g., ‘sexual abuse’, and may therefore capture events with different relationships to depression risk. For instance, it has been found that the risk of psychiatric disorders is higher for childhood sexual abuse that involves intercourse compared with nongenital sexual abuse (Kendler et al., Reference Kendler, Bulik, Silberg, Hettema, Myers and Prescott2000). Most studies have found that SLEs primarily increase depression risk over a shorter time period (e.g. months; Kendler, Karkowski, & Prescott, Reference Kendler, Karkowski and Prescott1998, Reference Kendler, Karkowski and Prescott1999a). Although SLEs were time-censored so that only SLEs occurring before depression were included in analyses, SLE occurrence and MDE onset were recorded to the nearest year. Thus, the effects could have been different if the timing of SLE occurrence was recorded closer to the MDE onset. In addition, our analyses could have included SLEs with long timespans between SLE occurrence and MDE onset (as long as the SLE occurred before the diagnosis), and the relationship between SLE exposure and depression onset with long time intervals is less clear. Additional descriptive analyses (see online Supplementary Appendix B) where we investigated the temporal relationship between SLE occurrence and MDE onset align with the previous literature, suggesting that most SLEs occur the same year as MDE onset. Additionally, previous studies have shown that high long-term contextual threat increases MDE onset following SLE occurrence substantially (Kendler et al., Reference Kendler, Karkowski and Prescott1998), but we did not assess long-term contextual threat in the current study. We assessed SLEs using a questionnaire and not, for instance, using trained interviewers. This could potentially result in higher inter-respondent variation in interpretation of the 18 SLEs (e.g. for sexual abuse), which could result in either under-reporting or over-reporting.

Implications

The findings of the current study have several implications. The results support previous research in implicating SLEs as important risk factors with probable causal influence on depression risk. Some of the SLEs included in our study (e.g. job loss) are indirectly influenced by policy and our findings underline the importance of preventing SLEs, when possible. The influence of SLEs on depression risk also suggests that trauma could be a useful focus in psychological treatments.

When controlling for SLE occurrence (across all 18 SLEs), the estimated heritability of depression was reduced by 8%, indicating that a minor proportion of the genetic risk of depression reflected genetic effects linked to SLEs. This estimate aligns with that of one previous study, which found that roughly 10–15% of the impact of genetic risk of depression was mediated through life experiences (Kendler & Karkowski-Shuman, Reference Kendler and Karkowski-Shuman1997). Minor genetic effects linked to SLEs suggest that it is unlikely that a large proportion of identified genetic variants will be linked to SLEs in GWASes. Our study also illustrates the utility of quantitative genetic designs in estimating genetic factors with direct effects and genetic factors acting through a heritable exposure, as we find that a minor proportion of genetic effects on depression risk reflect genetic effects related to SLE exposure. The heritability estimate for one-year depression risk was slightly lower than the estimate reported by Kendler and Gardner (Reference Kendler and Gardner2017), but close to heritability estimates for depressive symptoms in the past year (Matthews et al., Reference Matthews, Danese, Wertz, Odgers, Ambler, Moffitt and Arseneault2016) and in the past two weeks (Czajkowski, Røysamb, Reichborn-Kjennerud, & Tambs, Reference Czajkowski, Røysamb, Reichborn-Kjennerud and Tambs2010).

Conclusion

In the current study, we investigated patterns of associations between exposure to SLEs and depression risk in a large population-based twin sample using a co-twin control design. Diagnoses of depression were based on repeated clinical interviews across two waves of measurements. Event occurrence across all 18 SLEs, of violent SLEs and of economic SLEs was associated with an increased risk of depression. For the former two, co-twin control analyses indicated that the associations at least in part reflect the causal influence of SLE exposure on depression risk. The pattern of associations for economic SLEs could have resulted from suppression of the full sample association or inflated within-estimates due to non-shared confounding. No associations between other traumatic SLEs and depression risk were observed. A minor proportion of the total genetic risk of depression reflected genetic effects related to SLEs.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291722002227

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank the participants from the Norwegian Institute of Public Health Twin Panel.

Financial support

We acknowledge funding from the Research Council of Norway (226 985; 240 061), the Norwegian Foundation for Health and Rehabilitation, the Norwegian Council for Mental Health, and the European Commission under the programme ‘Quality of Life and Management of the Living Resources’ of the Fifth Framework Programme (QLG2-CT-2002-01254). EY is supported by the Norwegian Research Council (262 177; 288 083) and the European Research Council (818 425; 101045526).

Conflict of interest

None.

References

Akaike, H. (1987). Factor analysis and AIC. Psychometrika, 52(3), 317332. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02294359.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anders, S. L., Shallcross, S. L., & Frazier, P. A. (2012). Beyond criterion A1: The effects of relational and non-relational traumatic events. Journal of Trauma & Dissociation, 13(2), 134151. https://doi.org/10.1080/15299732.2012.642744.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Austin, P. C. (2017). A tutorial on multilevel survival analysis: Methods, models and applications. International Statistical Review, 85(2), 185203. https://doi.org/10.1111/insr.12214.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barth, J., Bermetz, L., Heim, E., Trelle, S., & Tonia, T. (2013). The current prevalence of child sexual abuse worldwide: A systematic review and meta-analysis. International Journal of Public Health, 58(3), 469483. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00038-012-0426-1.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bolinskey, P. K., Neale, M. C., Jacobson, K. C., Prescott, C. A., & Kendler, K. S. (2004). Sources of individual differences in stressful life event exposure in male and female twins. Twin Research, 7(1), 3338. https://doi.org/10.1375/13690520460741426.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, R. C., Berenz, E. C., Aggen, S. H., Gardner, C. O., Knudsen, G. P., Reichborn-Kjennerud, T., … Amstadter, A. B. (2014). Trauma exposure and axis I psychopathology: A co-twin control analysis in Norwegian young adults. Psychological Trauma: Theory, Research, Practice, and Policy, 6(6), 652660. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0034326.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Choi, K. W., Chen, C.-Y., Ursano, R. J., Sun, X., Jain, S., Kessler, R. C., … Smoller, J. W. (2020). Prospective study of polygenic risk, protective factors, and incident depression following combat deployment in US army soldiers. Psychological Medicine, 50(5), 737745. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291719000527.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Czajkowski, N., Røysamb, E., Reichborn-Kjennerud, T., & Tambs, K. (2010). A population-based family study of symptoms of anxiety and depression: The HUNT study. Journal of Affective Disorders, 125(1), 355360. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2010.01.006.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eaves, L. J., Last, K. A., Young, P. A., & Martin, N. G. (1978). Model-fitting approaches to the analysis of human behaviour. Heredity, 41(3), 249320. https://doi.org/10.1038/hdy.1978.101.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fisher, R. A. (1918). The correlation between relatives on the supposition of Mendelian inheritance. Earth and Environmental Science Transactions of The Royal Society of Edinburgh, 52(2), 399433. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0080456800012163.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frisell, T., Öberg, S., Kuja-Halkola, R., & Sjölander, A. (2012). Sibling comparison designs: Bias from non-shared confounders and measurement error. Epidemiology (Cambridge, Mass.), 23(5), 713720. https://doi.org/10.1097/EDE.0b013e31825fa230.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harris, J. R., Magnus, P., & Tambs, K. (2002). The Norwegian institute of public health twin panel: A description of the sample and program of research. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 5(5), 415423. https://doi.org/10.1375/twin.5.5.415.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harris, J. R., Magnus, P., & Tambs, K. (2006). The Norwegian institute of public health twin program of research: An update. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 9(6), 858864. https://doi.org/10.1375/twin.9.6.858.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hox, J. J. (2010). Multilevel analysis: Techniques and applications (2nd ed.). New York: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jinks, J. L., & Fulker, D. W. (1970). Comparison of the biometrical genetical, MAVA, and classical approaches to the analysis of the human behavior. Psychological Bulletin, 73(5), 311349. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0029135.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kendler, K. S., & Aggen, S. H. (2014). Clarifying the causal relationship in women between childhood sexual abuse and lifetime major depression. Psychological Medicine, 44(6), 12131221. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291713001797.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kendler, K. S., Bulik, C. M., Silberg, J., Hettema, J. M., Myers, J., & Prescott, C. A. (2000). Childhood sexual abuse and adult psychiatric and substance use disorders in women: An epidemiological and cotwin control analysis. Archives of General Psychiatry, 57(10), 953959. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.57.10.953.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kendler, K. S., & Gardner, C. O. (2001). Monozygotic twins discordant for major depression: A preliminary exploration of the role of environmental experiences in the aetiology and course of illness. Psychological Medicine, 31(3), 411423. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291701003622.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kendler, K. S., & Gardner, C. O. (2010). Dependent stressful life events and prior depressive episodes in the prediction of major depression: The problem of causal inference in psychiatric epidemiology. Archives of General Psychiatry, 67(11), 11201127. https://doi.org/10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.136.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kendler, K. S., & Gardner, C. O. (2014). Sex differences in the pathways to major depression: A study of opposite-sex twin pairs. American Journal of Psychiatry, 171(4), 426435. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2013.13101375.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kendler, K. S., & Gardner, C. O. (2017). Genetic and environmental influences on last-year major depression in adulthood: A highly heritable stable liability but strong environmental effects on 1-year prevalence. Psychological Medicine, 47(10), 18161824. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291717000277.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kendler, K. S., & Halberstadt, L. J. (2013). The road not taken: Life experiences in monozygotic twin pairs discordant for major depression. Molecular Psychiatry, 18(9), 975984. https://doi.org/10.1038/mp.2012.55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kendler, K. S., Karkowski, L. M., & Prescott, C. A. (1998). Stressful life events and major depression: Risk period, long-term contextual threat, and diagnostic specificity. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 186(11), 661669. https://doi.org/10.1097/00005053-199811000-00001.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kendler, K. S., Karkowski, L. M., & Prescott, C. A. (1999a). Causal relationship between stressful life events and the onset of major depression. American Journal of Psychiatry, 156(6), 837841. https://doi.org/10.1176/ajp.156.6.837.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kendler, K. S., Karkowski, L. M., & Prescott, C. A. (1999b). The assessment of dependence in the study of stressful life events: Validation using a twin design. Psychological Medicine, 29(6), 14551460. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291798008198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kendler, K. S., & Karkowski-Shuman, L. (1997). Stressful life events and genetic liability to major depression: Genetic control of exposure to the environment? Psychological Medicine, 27(3), 539547. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291797004716.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kendler, K. S., Kessler, R. C., Walters, E. E., MacLean, C. J., Neale, M. C., Heath, A. C., & Eaves, L. J. (1995). Stressful life events, genetic liability, and onset of an episode of major depression in women. American Journal of Psychiatry, 152(6), 833842. https://doi.org/10.1176/foc.8.3.foc459.Google ScholarPubMed
Kendler, K. S., Kessler, R. C., Walters, E. E., MacLean, C. J., Neale, M. C., Heath, A. C., & Eaves, L. J. (2010). Stressful life events, genetic liability, and onset of an episode of major depression in women. FOCUS, 8(3), 459470. https://doi.org/10.1176/foc.8.3.foc459.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kendler, K. S., Neale, M. C., Kessler, R. C., Heath, A. C., & Eaves, L. J. (1992). A population-based twin study of major depression in women: The impact of varying definitions of illness. Archives of General Psychiatry, 49(4), 257266. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.1992.01820040009001.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kendler, K. S., Neale, M. C., Kessler, R. C., Heath, A. C., & Eaves, L. J. (1993). The lifetime history of major depression in women: Reliability of diagnosis and heritability. Archives of General Psychiatry, 50(11), 863870. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.1993.01820230054003.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kendler, K. S., Ohlsson, H., Lichtenstein, P., Sundquist, J., & Sundquist, K. (2018). The genetic epidemiology of treated major depression in Sweden. American Journal of Psychiatry, 175(11), 11371144. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2018.17111251.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kendler, K. S., & Prescott, C. A. (1999). A population-based twin study of lifetime major depression in men and women. Archives of General Psychiatry, 56(1), 3944. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.56.1.39.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kessler, R. C. (1997). The effects of stressful life events on depression. Annual Review of Psychology, 48, 191214. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.48.1.191.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koenen, K. C., Lyons, M. J., Goldberg, J., Simpson, J., Williams, W. M., Toomey, R., … Tsuang, M. T. (2003). Co-twin control study of relationships among combat exposure, combat-related PTSD, and other mental disorders. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 16(5), 433438. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1025786925483.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mandelli, L., & Serretti, A. (2013). Gene-environment interaction studies in depression and suicidal behavior: An update. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 37(10), 23752397. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.07.011.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Matthews, T., Danese, A., Wertz, J., Odgers, C. L., Ambler, A., Moffitt, T. E., & Arseneault, L. (2016). Social isolation, loneliness and depression in young adulthood: A behavioural genetic analysis. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 51(3), 339348. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00127-016-1178-7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McAdams, T. A., Rijsdijk, F. V., Zavos, H. M. S., & Pingault, J.-B. (2021). Twins and causal inference: Leveraging nature's experiment. Cold Spring Harbor Perspectives in Medicine, 11, 121. https://doi.org/10.1101/cshperspect.a039552.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCutcheon, V. V., Heath, A. C., Nelson, E. C., Bucholz, K. K., Madden, P. A. F., & Martin, N. G. (2009). Accumulation of trauma over time and risk for depression in a twin sample. Psychological Medicine, 39(3), 431441. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291708003759.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, E. C., Heath, A. C., Madden, P. A. F., Cooper, M. L., Dinwiddie, S. H., Bucholz, K. K., … Martin, N. G. (2002). Association between self-reported childhood sexual abuse and adverse psychosocial outcomes: Results from a twin study. Archives of General Psychiatry, 59(2), 139145. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.59.2.139.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nilsen, T. S., Knudsen, G. P., Gervin, K., Brandt, I., Røysamb, E., Tambs, K., … Harris, J. R. (2013). The Norwegian twin registry from a public health perspective: A research update. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 16(1), 285295. https://doi.org/10.1017/thg.2012.117.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petras, H., Masyn, K. E., Buckley, J. A., Ialongo, N. S., & Kellam, S. (2011). Who is most atrisk for school removal? A multilevel discrete-time survival analysis of individual- and context-level influences. Journal of Educational Psychology, 103(1), 223237. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021545.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pingault, J.-B., O'Reilly, P. F., Schoeler, T., Ploubidis, G. B., Rijsdijk, F., & Dudbridge, F. (2018). Using genetic data to strengthen causal inference in observational research. Nature Reviews Genetics, 19(9), 566580. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41576-018-0020-3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rabe-Hesketh, S., & Skrondal, A. (2008). Multilevel and longitudinal modeling using stata (2nd ed., 331372). College Station, Tex: Stata Press.Google Scholar
Rabe-Hesketh, S., Skrondal, A., & Gjessing, H. K. (2008). Biometrical modeling of twin and family data using standard mixed model software. Biometrics, 64(1), 280288. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1541-0420.2007.00803.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reed, V., Gander, F., Pfister, H., Steiger, A., Sonntag, H., Trenkwalder, C., … Wittchen, H.-U. (1998). To what degree does the Composite International Diagnostic Interview (CIDI) correctly identify DSM-IV disorders? Testing validity issues in a clinical sample. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research, 7(3), 142155. https://doi.org/10.1002/mpr.44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Silberg, J. L., Copeland, W., Linker, J., Moore, A. A., Roberson-Nay, R., & York, T. P. (2016). Psychiatric outcomes of bullying victimization: A study of discordant monozygotic twins. Psychological Medicine, 46(9), 18751883. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291716000362.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sjöholm, L., Lavebratt, C., & Forsell, Y. (2009). A multifactorial developmental model for the etiology of major depression in a population-based sample. Journal of Affective Disorders, 113(1–2), 6676. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2008.04.028.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stone, S. L., Diop, H., Declercq, E., Cabral, H. J., Fox, M. P., & Wise, L. A. (2015). Stressful events during pregnancy and postpartum depressive symptoms. Journal of Women's Health, 24(5), 384393. https://doi.org/10.1089/jwh.2014.4857.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sullivan, P. F., Neale, M. C., & Kendler, K. S. (2000). Genetic epidemiology of major depression: Review and meta-analysis. American Journal of Psychiatry, 157(10), 15521562. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.157.10.1552.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tennant, C. (2002). Life events, stress and depression: A review of recent findings. Australian & New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 36(2), 173182. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1440-1614.2002.01007.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van den Berg, S. M., Glas, C. A. W., & Boomsma, D. I. (2007). Variance decomposition using an IRT measurement model. Behavior Genetics, 37(4), 604616. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10519-007-9156-1.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ward, T. S., Kanu, F. A., & Robb, S. W. (2017). Prevalence of stressful life events during pregnancy and its association with postpartum depressive symptoms. Archives of Women's Mental Health, 20(1), 161171. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00737-016-0689-2.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Warrier, V., Kwong, A., Luo, M., Dalvie, S., Croft, J., Sallis, H., … Cecil, C. (2021). Gene-environment correlations and causal effects of childhood maltreatment on physical and mental health: A genetically informed approach. The Lancet, 8(5), 373386. https://doi.org/10.17863/CAM.62469.Google ScholarPubMed
Wittchen, H.-U., Lachner, G., Wunderlich, U., & Pfister, H. (1998). Test-retest reliability of the computerized DSM-IV version of the Munich-Composite International Diagnostic Interview (M-CIDI). Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 33(11), 568578.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wittchen, H.-U., & Pfister, H. (1997). DIA-X Interview (M-CIDI). Frankfurt: Swets & Zeitlinger.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Depression diagnoses at Wave 1 and Wave 2

Figure 1

Table 2. Frequencies of stressful life events

Figure 2

Table 3. Fixed and random effects of the AE model

Figure 3

Table 4. Results of co-twin control analyses displaying hORs

Figure 4

Table 5. Residual random effects for the models with grouped SLEs

Supplementary material: File

Bjørndal et al. supplementary material

Appendices A-D

Download Bjørndal et al. supplementary material(File)
File 66.4 KB