Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-xbtfd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-10T15:44:23.017Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Potential resilience treatments for orangutans (Pongo spp.): Lessons from a scoping review of interventions in humans and other animals

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 December 2023

Lelia Bridgeland-Stephens*
Affiliation:
School of Biosciences, University of Birmingham, Birmingham, UK
Susannah KS Thorpe
Affiliation:
School of Biosciences, University of Birmingham, Birmingham, UK
Jackie Chappell
Affiliation:
School of Biosciences, University of Birmingham, Birmingham, UK
*
Corresponding author: Lelia Bridgeland-Stephens; Email: lxb1002@student.bham.ac.uk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Wild orangutans (Pongo spp.) rescued from human-wildlife conflict must be adequately rehabilitated before being returned to the wild. It is essential that released orangutans are able to cope with stressful challenges such as food scarcity, navigating unfamiliar environments, and regaining independence from human support. Although practical skills are taught to orangutans in rehabilitation centres, post-release survival rates are low. Psychological resilience, or the ability to ‘bounce back’ from stress, may be a key missing piece of the puzzle. However, there is very little knowledge about species-appropriate interventions which could help captive orangutans increase resilience to stress. This scoping review summarises and critically analyses existing human and non-human animal resilience literature and provides suggestions for the development of interventions for orangutans in rehabilitation. Three scientific databases were searched in 2021 and 2023, resulting in 63 human studies and 266 non-human animal studies. The first section brings together human resilience interventions, identifying common themes and assessing the applicability of human interventions to orangutans in rehabilitation. The second section groups animal interventions into categories of direct stress, separation stress, environmental conditions, social stress, and exercise. In each category, interventions are critically analysed to evaluate their potential for orangutans in rehabilitation. The results show that mild and manageable forms of intervention have the greatest potential benefit with the least amount of risk. The study concludes by emphasising the need for further investigation and experimentation, to develop appropriate interventions and measure their effect on the post-release survival rate of orangutans.

Type
Scoping Review
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of The Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Introduction

All three orangutan species (Pongo spp.) are critically endangered in the wild and face substantial population decline (Ancrenaz et al. Reference Ancrenaz, Gumal, Marshall, Meijaard, Wich and Husson2018; Singleton et al. Reference Singleton, Wich, Nowak, Usher and Utami-Atmoko2018). As the only great ape species endemic to the islands of Borneo and Sumatra, orangutans are threatened by a combination of factors, including habitat loss, degradation, and fragmentation (Wich et al. Reference Wich, Struebig, Refisch, Wilting, Kramer-Schadt and Meijaard2015, Reference Wich, Singleton, Nowak, Utami Atmoko, Nisam, Arif, Putra, Ardi, Fredriksson, Usher, Gaveau and Kühl2016). Many are deliberately killed as a by-product of habitat conversion, or as a result of human-wildlife conflict, which can lead to infants being captured and trafficked in the illegal wildlife trade (Ancrenaz et al. Reference Ancrenaz, Gumal, Marshall, Meijaard, Wich and Husson2018; Singleton et al. Reference Singleton, Wich, Nowak, Usher and Utami-Atmoko2018). Since pioneering efforts in the 1960s, a number of rehabilitation centres have been established across Borneo and Sumatra, with the aim of rescuing injured and/or trafficked orangutans and providing them with long-term rehabilitation (Russon Reference Russon, Wich, Utami Atmoko, Mitra Setia and van Schaik2008). The purpose of this is to prepare capable individuals for release back into the wild, and to provide lifelong care for those who are unable to be released, for example if they have a severe physical disability, or an inability to acquire the necessary skills for independent living. As many orangutans in rehabilitation are infants rescued from the illegal wildlife trade (Russon Reference Russon, Wich, Utami Atmoko, Mitra Setia and van Schaik2008), these individuals will spend a large proportion of their development in rehabilitation centres before they are old enough to be released back into the wild. Although other great ape species are released back into the wild, this process is more established for orangutans than the other great apes. This is partly because the Indonesian government mandates the release of all orangutans where possible (Sherman et al. Reference Sherman, Ancrenaz and Meijaard2020). Therefore, this review focuses on orangutan rehabilitation due to the more urgent need considering ongoing orangutan releases, and the fact that established rehabilitation and release practices for orangutans are already in place.

Wild orangutan infants stay with their mothers until they are around six to eight years old (van Noordwijk & van Schaik Reference van Noordwijk and van Schaik2005), learning all the essential skills they need to survive as an adult. Life in the wild for the world’s largest arboreal mammal (Cant Reference Cant1992) is extremely demanding. Released orangutans will need to face challenges such as the physical and cognitive requirements of travelling through the forest canopy and building appropriate nests (Thorpe et al. Reference Thorpe, Crompton and Alexander2007; van Casteren et al. Reference van Casteren, Sellers, Thorpe, Coward, Crompton, Myatt and Ennos2012; Halsey et al. Reference Halsey, Coward, Crompton and Thorpe2017), coping with unpredictable food scarcity (Knott Reference Knott1998), encountering unfamiliar or difficult-to-process food (Jaeggi et al. Reference Jaeggi, Dunkel, van Noordwijk, Wich, Sura and van Schaik2010), interacting with other orangutans, and managing potential conflict with humans and other dangerous animals (Meijaard et al. Reference Meijaard, Buchori, Hadiprakarsa, Utami-Atmoko, Nurcahyo, Tjiu, Prasetyo, Nardiyono, Ancrenaz, Abadi, Antoni, Armayadi, Dinato, Ella, Gumelar, Indrawan, Kussaritano, Priyono, Purwanto, Puspitasari, Putra, Rahmat, Ramadani, Sammy, Siswanto, Syamsuri, Andayani, Wu, Wells and Mengersen2011; Chappell & Thorpe Reference Chappell and Thorpe2022). In addition, when female orangutans breed successfully after being released, they must face all these challenges while providing their infants with extensive care until weaning, without having had the opportunity to experience species-typical maternal care from their own mothers.

As many orangutans in rehabilitation centres are orphaned at a young age, they are completely dependent on the support and care of human caretakers, and being able to learn from their peers, to help them acquire these life skills and prepare for independent living in the forest. Despite the extensive current efforts of rehabilitation centres to help orangutans develop each of these skills, there are continuing issues with survival after release into the wild (Russon Reference Russon, Wich, Utami Atmoko, Mitra Setia and van Schaik2008; Utami-Atmoko et al. Reference Utami-Atmoko, Traylor-Holzer, Rifqi, Siregar, Achmad, Priadjati, Husson, Hadisiswoyo, Saputra, Campbell-Smith, Kuncoro, Russon, Voigt, Santika, Nowak, Singleton, Sapari, Meididit, Chandradewi, Ripoll Capilla, Ermayanti and Lees2017; Basalamah et al. Reference Basalamah, Atmoko, Perwitasari-Farajallah, Qayim, Sihite, van Noordwijk, Willems and Van Schaik2018). One survey found that the reported survival rates of released orangutans range from 20 to 80%, with an estimated average 40% survival rate (Russon Reference Russon, Wich, Utami Atmoko, Mitra Setia and van Schaik2008). Therefore, even if orangutans appear to exhibit individual life skills while in rehabilitation, this does not necessarily translate into the capacity for independent survival. This indicates that there is a missing piece of the puzzle, and suggests that there may be a broader, less tangible, factor underpinning the successful adaption of orangutans to the wild.

In a survey of Orangutan Veterinary Advisory Group (OVAG) members (n = 43), 93% of respondents (40/43) agreed that an orangutan’s ‘drive to survive’ was an important factor in deciding whether to release orangutans back into the wild (unpublished OVAG questionnaire, L Bridgeland-Stephens 2020). Although OVAG was established with the primary purpose of providing support and advice to vets working with orangutans, its members and conference attendees (who were consulted in this survey) include a wide range of professionals working in orangutan rehabilitation, including centre managers, animal caretakers, and scientists. Participants described orangutans with the ‘drive to survive’ as having the motivation and skills to meet their own well-being needs, successfully adapt to the forest environment, and show durability in overcoming challenges. Anecdotally, participants describe finding that some individuals just give up, while others remain engaged with their new environment. Each of these factors is constructive in ensuring survival after orangutans have been released into the wild. This idea of individual variation in orangutans’ ‘drive to survive’ arguably overlaps with the concept of ‘resilience’, which can be defined as a successful adaptation or competence despite stress or trauma (Egeland et al. Reference Egeland, Carlson and Sroufe1993). In humans, a lack of resilience can be expressed in many ways, for example passive or helpless behaviour, a short attention span, or disregarding and even dismantling solutions to a problem which at first seem to be successful (Janoff-Bulman & Brickman Reference Janoff-Bulman, Brickman and Feather1982). Resilience influences the way in which events or problems are approached, and can be influenced by factors such as temperament, positive emotions, self-esteem, planning skills, and supportive environments (Fletcher & Sarkar Reference Fletcher and Sarkar2013). Traits indicative of resilience include resourcefulness, flexibility, high levels of activity, optimism (see S1 Appendix in Supplementary material), and curiosity (Block & Block Reference Block and Block1980).

There is a lack of existing knowledge regarding resilience in non-human great apes. However, it seems likely that resilience, and the ability to ‘bounce back’ from stress, would be important for orangutans in rehabilitation centres. Resilience may also underpin the ‘drive to survive’, highlighted by OVAG members as an important aspect of successful orangutan releases. All orangutans in rehabilitation will have experienced at least one major life stressor, in being captured and then travelling to the rehabilitation centre. In addition, prior to rehabilitation, many will have witnessed the death of their mother, experienced physical injuries from humans or other orangutans, poor living conditions in small cages, or emaciation from lack of food (Sherman et al. Reference Sherman, Ancrenaz and Meijaard2020). They are likely to continue to experience stressors during their time in rehabilitation, for example conflict with conspecifics, veterinary interventions such as surgeries or routine health checks, being near dominant males (Mitra Setia & van Schaik Reference Mitra Setia and van Schaik2007; Great Ape Taxon Advisory Group 2018; Kunz et al. Reference Kunz, Duvot, van Noordwijk, Willems, Townsend, Mardianah, Utami Atmoko, Vogel, Nugraha, Heistermann, Agil and Weingrill2021), and moving to unfamiliar environments (e.g. pre-release islands). As well as overcoming stressors during rehabilitation, it seems likely that psychological resilience would be particularly important when orangutans are released, so that they have the capacity to adapt to challenges related to life in the wild. Low levels of resilience could explain why some orangutans appear to simply ‘give up’, despite their skillset, because persistence is an important aspect of resilience (Grotberg Reference Grotberg1995; Brown Reference Brown2015). In this context, ‘give up’ means not striving to meet one’s own well-being needs, thereby increasing the likelihood of serious harm or death. Behaviours that indicate a striving to meet well-being needs include actively seeking food and avoiding predators, building appropriate nests, establishing a range, and travelling through the canopy rather than across the ground. Resilience may be an important missing piece of the puzzle, potentially underpinning the chances of a successful release by providing orangutans with a more flexible and generalised ability to ‘bounce back’ from previously unencountered challenges, rather than having to rely upon specialised, situation-specific skills.

In humans, resilience is considered to be a malleable, rather than fixed, characteristic (Fletcher & Sarkar Reference Fletcher and Sarkar2013). Similarly, although little is known about orangutan resilience, 70% of OVAG respondents (n = 30/43) felt that an orangutan’s ‘drive to survive’ could be influenced through the rehabilitation process (unpublished OVAG questionnaire, L Bridgeland-Stephens 2020). However, despite the potential importance of psychological resilience on the success of rehabilitation and release in orangutans, this factor has not yet been addressed scientifically. In order to begin filling this gap in scientific knowledge, this review will draw together human and animal research on resilience interventions, to establish an understanding of the foundations of resilience, and to identify possible resilience-promoting treatments for orangutans. ‘Resilience treatments’ are defined here as interventions designed to influence one or more attributes of resilience in an individual. In reviewing the existing literature, the word ‘treatment’ is taken to mean an experimental condition or intervention, including treatments expected to have a negative impact. It is important to understand the potential negative effects of specific interventions in order to avoid replicating them and thereby causing further harm. It should be noted that resilience is a latent variable and can only be measured or influenced indirectly. Therefore, interventions designed to promote resilience are likely to overlap and inter-relate with tools to measure resilience, as both are proxies of ‘true’ resilience.

The purpose of this review is to outline key recommendations for practical interventions that can be used to promote resilience in orangutans in rehabilitation centres. Although the findings of this review are more immediately useful for orangutans in rehabilitation centres, they also have relevance for orangutans in zoos and sanctuaries, as well as for other species of captive great apes. Due to the lack of research into great ape resilience interventions, this review relies upon existing research on humans and other species, largely rodent studies. Due to the close phylogenetic relationship and cognitive overlap among great ape species, and the resulting ‘psycho-socio-biological continuity’ (Brüne et al. Reference Brüne, Brüne-Cohrs, McGrew and Preuschoft2006), it is not unlikely that many of the main factors underpinning resilience in humans are shared by other great apes. There is a longstanding tradition of comparative research on aspects of psychology, for example theory of mind (Premack & Woodruff Reference Premack and Woodruff1978), attachment theory (Harlow Reference Harlow1958), learned helplessness (Overmier & Seligman Reference Overmier and Seligman1967), and psychopathology (e.g. Fabrega Reference Fabrega2002). In addition, although rodents are not closely related to great apes, rodent studies have long been used as a starting point for pre-clinical medical research due to similarities in anatomy, physiology, and genetics (Bryda Reference Bryda2013) (though for limitations of animal studies in human psychology, see Shapiro Reference Shapiro1998). In conjunction, research on human and non-human animals will help to build a picture of factors underpinning resilience and possible treatments. However, the findings of this scoping review should only be interpreted as starting points for cautious future research into great ape resilience treatments. Section one will address human resilience treatments. Although most of these methods are not applicable to great apes, it is important to understand the essential elements of resilience and identify common themes that underpin successful resilience treatments. Therefore, this section will draw out key concepts that have the potential to be translated into interventions for orangutans. Section two will summarise the most common resilience treatments in studies of non-human animals, hereafter referred to as animals. The animal resilience treatments described in section two all overlap with at least one theme described in the human resilience section. Each treatment type will be critically analysed to evaluate its relevance for orangutan rehabilitation centres, and practical suggestions will be made for integrating similar techniques into appropriate resilience treatments for orangutans.

Materials and methods

An electronic search was conducted on 7th February 2021 using three search engines: ProQuest Biological Science Collection, Scopus, and Web of Knowledge. Boolean operators were used for the following search terms: “psychological resilience” OR “resilience interventions” OR “resilience program” OR “stress inoculation” OR “stress immunity” OR “foster resilience”; and “psychological resilience” AND “animal”. The language filter was set to English, and the sources included were books, conference papers and proceedings, dissertations and theses, government & official publications, reports, scholarly journals, and working papers. There was no exclusion based on publication date.

The initial search returned 1,680 results, reduced to 1,241 articles of potential relevance after removing duplicates. The reference lists of the animal articles were also searched for relevant articles, as a greater diversity of treatment type was identified in the animal literature compared to the human literature. Only resources which had institutional access were used, excluding 20 articles from the final list. Irrelevant articles, and those which related to a very specific aspect of resilience, for example in a particular activity or relating to a particular illness or pain threshold, were removed in favour of articles with more general relevance. Studies on prenatal stress, and studies proposing medicinal aids to resilience, were excluded. Finally, only interventions which were targeted at building individual resilience (rather than family resilience, or group resilience) were included, even if the resilience interventions were provided in a group setting. The search and filtering process was carried out by the lead author and resulted in 63 human papers and 240 animal papers. An additional search of animal articles was carried out on 11th May 2023, using the same search terms and filtering criteria as before, to ensure that the literature was up-to-date. This returned 202 results, and, after removing duplicates (n = 1) and articles that did not meet the criteria (n = 175), resulted in a further 26 relevant animal studies, bringing the total number of animal studies to 266. Due to the breadth of fields drawn together in this review, a glossary for lesser-known terms is provided in S1 Appendix in the Supplementary material.

Section one: Human resilience treatments

The human resilience studies encompassed a broad range of age groups and spanned five continents (Asia, Australia, Europe, and North and South America). An overwhelming majority of the treatments used talking therapies, in a group or individual setting. The in-text descriptions of the content of each resilience programme were coded into different categories and then collated into broader recurring themes, listed in Table 1. Many of these themes match those described by OVAG members as important factors in an orangutan’s ‘drive to survive’; for example, flexibility, hardiness, self-efficacy, independence, social skills and interest in conspecifics, physical activity, problem-solving, and the ability to overcome stress (unpublished OVAG questionnaire, L Bridgeland-Stephens 2020).

Table 1. Common themes in human resilience interventions

‘Number of studies’ includes all studies which mention each theme; some studies include multiple themes (see S1 Appendix in Supplementary material for definitions of terms).

Interventions included training in one or more of these themes, with a mean of 6.5 different themes per treatment and a range of 1–15. Studies can be broadly divided into those based on a type of cognitive behaviour therapy called Stress Inoculation Therapy (see S1 Appendix; Supplementary material) (Meichenbaum Reference Meichenbaum1985), further detailed in Stress inoculation below, and those using alternative methods. More than 80 different types of questionnaire were used to measure aspects of resilience, shown in S2 Appendix (Supplementary material). These were either carried out by an experimenter/professional or self-administered.

Resilience interventions

Several interventions were found to significantly increase resilience (Steinhardt & Doblier Reference Steinhardt and Doblier2010; Peng et al. Reference Peng, Li, Zuo, Miao, Chen, Yu, Liu and Wang2014; Rogerson et al. Reference Rogerson, Meir, Crowley-McHattan, Mcewen and Pastoors2016; Pluess et al. Reference Pluess, Boniwell, Hefferon and Tunariu2017; van Agteren et al. Reference van Agteren, Iasiello and Lo2018; Joyce et al. Reference Joyce, Hons, Shand, Bryant, Lal and Harvey2018; Henshall et al. Reference Henshall, Davey and Jackson2020), particularly in individuals who initially had low levels of resilience (Peng et al. Reference Peng, Li, Zuo, Miao, Chen, Yu, Liu and Wang2014; van Agteren et al. Reference van Agteren, Iasiello and Lo2018). In applying a resilience training programme to female prisoners, which focused on mindfulness techniques, ‘positive psychology’ (see S1 Appendix; Supplementary material), and cognitive behavioural therapy, Lo et al. (Reference Lo, Iasiello, Carey and van Agteren2020) identified a greater beneficial effect of the programme for long-term prisoners than prisoners awaiting release. This is encouraging for orangutans unable to be released into the wild who need to cope with pressures connected with long-term life in captivity, for example relative space limitations and lack of environmental complexity. Although there are clear differences between human prisoners and animals in long-term captivity, particularly regarding the intentions and causes underlying each situation, there are arguably similarities in the physical limitations and psychological stress (Morgan & Tromborg Reference Morgan and Tromborg2007) resulting from long-term life in captivity. In a resilience training programme with similar methods to Lo et al. (Reference Lo, Iasiello, Carey and van Agteren2020), Smith et al. (Reference Smith, Shatté, Perlman, Siers and Lynch2018) found that the longer participants engaged with resilience training, the greater the benefit they experienced. This indicates that it is beneficial to integrate resilience-building interventions throughout the whole rehabilitation period, to maximise the potential benefits. Although mindfulness and positive psychology themselves are not applicable to orangutans (as with all forms of talking therapy), it seems likely that their effectiveness in building resilience is related to the individual mechanisms of action of these methods, e.g. increasing relaxation and optimism (Rygula et al. Reference Rygula, Golebiowska, Kregiel, Kubik and Popik2015; Roelofs et al. Reference Roelofs, Boleij, Nordquist and van der Staay2016). Therefore, their inclusion is still relevant in terms of identifying aspects of resilience treatments which can be effective.

A range of studies that focused on talking therapies found benefits in measures of specific aspects of resilience, including the ability to cope, proactivity, self-esteem, confidence, and lower levels of stress, depression, anxiety, inflexibility, and negative or suppressed emotions (Steinhardt & Doblier Reference Steinhardt and Doblier2010; Peng et al. Reference Peng, Li, Zuo, Miao, Chen, Yu, Liu and Wang2014; Gallegos-Guajardo et al. Reference Gallegos-Guajardo, Ruvalcaba-Romero, Langley and Villegas-Guinea2015; Pluess et al. Reference Pluess, Boniwell, Hefferon and Tunariu2017; van Agteren et al. Reference van Agteren, Iasiello and Lo2018; Joyce et al. Reference Joyce, Hons, Shand, Bryant, Lal and Harvey2018; Foster et al. Reference Foster, Cuzzillo and Furness2018; Henshall et al. Reference Henshall, Davey and Jackson2020; Kozina Reference Kozina2020; Lo et al. Reference Lo, Iasiello, Carey and van Agteren2020; Akeman et al. Reference Akeman, Aupperle, Clausen, Cosgrove, McDermott, Cromer, Paulus, Carolina, Kirlic, Clausen, Cosgrove, Mcdermott, Cromer, Paulus, Yeh and Aupperle2020). Pluess et al. (Reference Pluess, Boniwell, Hefferon and Tunariu2017) identified long-lasting beneficial effects in schoolchildren who had participated in a resilience treatment programme, when tested six and twelve months after the treatment. Neither Chandler and Roberts (Reference Chandler and Roberts2015) nor Delaney et al. (Reference Delaney, Barrere, Robertson, Zahourek, Diaz and Lachapelle2016) found a statistically significant difference between resilience treatment and control groups, although both studies had a relatively low number of participants (n = 28 and 40, respectively) and benefits were self-reported by participants in the form of written reflections or in-depth interviews. Reported benefits included building personal strengths, creating supportive connections with others (Chandler & Roberts Reference Chandler and Roberts2015), learning stress-relieving techniques, and sharing experiences of stress with others (Delaney et al. Reference Delaney, Barrere, Robertson, Zahourek, Diaz and Lachapelle2016).

These results suggest that it is possible for humans to build skills and attributes that contribute to resilience and protect against stress. However, the treatments above are all talking therapies, and non-linguistic methods are required for orangutans. In addition, resilience therapies in humans may have an underlying benefit by providing a degree of social support, same-species contact, and the feeling of ‘being understood’, and these benefits may not translate in a straightforward way for orangutans. However, the most common themes in human resilience treatments, shown in Table 1, offer a starting point for the design of orangutan interventions which incorporate one or more of these resilience themes.

Stress inoculation

Stress inoculation therapy

Stress Inoculation Therapy (SIT) (Meichenbaum Reference Meichenbaum1985) involves a form of cognitive behavioural therapy, and is based on the theory that behaviour and feelings are determined by individual perceptions, and can therefore be cognitively reframed. SIT is intended to break the ‘stress cycle’ of negative reactions and ineffective responses to stressful events by teaching a range of coping skills and behaviours. The three-stage process involves patients reconceptualising their responses to specific stressors, before learning coping skills and applying these to imaginary (role-played) and real-life stressors (Meichenbaum & Cameron Reference Meichenbaum, Cameron, Meichenbaum and Jaremko1983).

SIT was used by a treatment in 27 of the papers reviewed, and several randomised controlled trials identified beneficial effects (Law et al. Reference Law, Logan and Baron1994; Szabo & Marian Reference Szabo and Marian2012; Varker & Devilly Reference Varker and Devilly2012; Hourani et al. Reference Hourani, Tueller, Kizakevich, Lewis, Strange, Weimer, Bryant, Bishop, Hubal and Spira2016; Navaee & Kaykha Reference Navaee and Kaykha2019), with some positive effects still present two years after the intervention (Hourani et al. Reference Hourani, Tueller, Kizakevich, Lewis, Strange, Weimer, Bryant, Bishop, Hubal and Spira2016). However, in the context of orangutan rehabilitation, the cognitive reframing element of SIT would be extremely difficult to achieve in a non-linguistic form and may be counterproductive in blunting responses to stressors, for example by promoting the inhibition of appropriate reactions to stressful events during the ‘skills acquisition’ phase of the treatment (Meichenbaum & Cameron Reference Meichenbaum, Cameron, Meichenbaum and Jaremko1983). It is important for caretakers to be able to identify stressors for orangutans by reading their behaviour, to avoid further harm. This is already a challenge, as non-human primates are adept at masking overt expressions of pain and illness (Pelsker & Mayer Reference Pelsker and Mayer2008). SIT has also received criticism for moving the locus of control away from the patient (towards the therapist), and for framing certain responses to stress as ‘irrational’ (Hurley et al. Reference Hurley, Barrett and Reet2006). As will be discussed later in Controllability and predictability of stress, a sense of agency and control over the environment is thought to have a beneficial effect for a range of taxonomic groups. Although SIT is not appropriate here, certain coping skills involved in the treatment may be beneficial, for example problem-solving skills.

Inoculation from major lifetime stressors

Alongside daily stressors related to life in captivity, all orangutans in rehabilitation will have experienced at least one major life stressor, i.e. capture and travelling to the rehabilitation centre. However, depending on the prior history of each orangutan, they are likely to have experienced additional major stressors leading up to their rescue, such as witnessing the death of their mother, as well as stressors during their time in rehabilitation, for example veterinary interventions. Although some studies identified a positive linear relationship between cumulative lifetime adversity and the odds of being diagnosed with a stress-related disorder (Gerber et al. Reference Gerber, Frankfurt, Contractor, Oudshoorn, Dranger and Brown2018; Fernandez et al. Reference Fernandez, Choi, Marshall, Vicente, Saldivia, Kohn, Koenen, Arheart and Buka2020), there is more robust evidence for a U-shaped relationship. In a large study involving a stratified random-digit-dial telephone survey with 2,398 members of the public, Seery et al. (Reference Seery, Holman and Silver2010) found that individuals with some experience of lifetime adversity had lower ratings for distress, functional impairment, and PTSD symptoms, higher ratings for life satisfaction, and were least affected by recent adversity than those with no experience of prior adversity, or those with high exposure to adversity (categorised as mean + 1 SD; ̴ 87th percentile). These results are supported by Seery et al. (Reference Seery, Leo, Lupien, Kondrak and Almonte2013), who reported a similar U-shaped relationship between lifetime adversity and pain-induced catastrophising and negative affect, with moderate numbers of prior major life stressors (two to seven) associated with the highest resilience to stress. If a U-shaped relationship between life stressors and resilience exists for orangutans, it is very important that an ‘upper limit’ of life stressors is established, so it can be avoided. However, because orangutans’ life histories prior to arrival at the rehabilitation centres are often unknown, this will be difficult to achieve. The concept of stress inoculation and the effects of prior adversity will be discussed further in the Animal intervention section of this paper.

Section two: Animal resilience treatments

In this section, stress and resilience treatments for animals are broadly separated into five categories: direct stress procedures designed to elicit pain/fear/discomfort; separation from the group/mother; environmental deprivation/enrichment; social stress, including exposure to strangers or dominant individuals; and exercise regimes. These are different to the human resilience categories shown in Table 1 as the five categories described here relate to forms of treatment, rather than themes of resilience within different types of talking therapies. There were one to four treatments per study, with a mean of 1.36 treatments and the great majority of papers using a single experimental treatment, alongside a control (where applicable) (201/266). Many studies, particularly those involving rodents, used well-established behavioural tests to measure aspects of resilience and stress indicators, summarised in S3 Appendix (Supplementary material). A comprehensive overview of animal resilience treatments is shown in Tables 2(a)–(e). Since exactly the same event, with exactly the same effect on the animal, could either be an intended stress (i.e. stress-related resilience treatments) or an unintended ‘accidental’ stress (e.g. unavoidable/unpredictable stressors), treatments have been grouped by the form of stressor, rather than by whether the stress constituted a deliberate treatment or not, or the direction of the effect (i.e. whether positive or negative). The effect of each stressor on resilience must first be established before decisions are made about whether these can be turned into intentional treatments. It is also important to be able to compare the difference in effect of similar kinds of stress side-by-side. For example, short separations from the group/mother are generally considered helpful, whereas prolonged separations are generally deemed unhelpful to resilience. Therefore, Tables 2(a)–(e) and the remainder of this section groups treatments by their form of stress, in descending order of the number of relevant papers.

Key to Tables 2(a)–(e)

Generic stressors/stress schedules

It is important to understand the effects of direct stress procedures designed to elicit pain, fear, or discomfort, not only to investigate how prior stress influences resilience, but also whether milder forms of stress can have an inoculating effect. There is strong evidence that acute (single stress session) and chronic (over a period of at least ten days) stressors (see S1 Appendix; Supplementary material) cause a range of long-lasting negative effects in rodents, including increased anxiety and depression, heightened fear expression during re-exposure to stressors, and reduced cognitive and behavioural flexibility (see Table 2[a]). However, the evidence for the effects of restraint stress, or short periods of stress (3–7 days) of various kinds, such as electric shock, predator odour, forced swim, and/or elevated platform stress (see S1 Appendix; Supplementary material), on rodents is less clear-cut. There is contradictory evidence, with some studies showing positive outcomes, and others showing negative outcomes. As with separation stress, companionship with a conspecific may protect against the negative effects of direct stress. For example, one study of the effect of chronic unpredictable stress on male mice found that 30-min periods spent with a female mouse in between stressors ameliorated some depressive effects of the stress (Li et al. Reference Li, Zhang, Cao, Li, Xu, Song and hui2022). Another study found that stimulation of rats by humans, i.e. stroking them at a rate of 5 cm s–1 for ten minutes prior to chronic unpredictable mild stress, helped to protect against increased anxiety and depression in subsequent behavioural tests (Walker et al. Reference Walker, Cavieres, Peñaloza-Sancho, El-Deredy, McGlone and Dagnino-Subiabre2022).

Table 2(a). Summary of generic stressor/stress treatment effects by taxonomic group, and sex

(Grey-shaded cells highlight primate studies; S = sex (F = female, M = male, B = both/mixed, ? = not stated)

However, some taxonomic groups may experience benefits from stress treatments. For example, one week of unpredictable stress in Japanese quail (Coturnix japonica) led to increased spatial learning and behavioural flexibility when tested the next day (Calandreau et al. Reference Calandreau, Bertin, Boissy, Arnould, Constantin, Desmedt, Guémené, Nowak and Leterrier2011). One sheep (Ovis aries) study found that three days of 6-h restraint sessions led to more optimistic judgements than unrestrained controls in a positive/negative bias test (Doyle et al. Reference Doyle, Fisher, Hinch, Boissy and Lee2010). However, as the subjects were not later retested, the duration of these benefits is unclear. Behavioural flexibility, cognitive skills, and optimism are all thought to be aspects of resilience (Masten et al. Reference Masten, Best and Garmezy1990; Mandleco & Peery Reference Mandleco and Peery2000), and therefore may help individuals in overcoming future stress (see Human resilience).

Some rodent studies indicate that certain characteristics are conducive to resilience against stress, for example low anxiety and exploration, low emotionality, and positive affect (see S1 Appendix; Supplementary material) (Ducottet & Belzung Reference Ducottet and Belzung2004; Mällo et al. Reference Mällo, Matrov, Kõiv and Harro2009; Castro et al. Reference Castro, Diessler, Varea, Márquez, Larsen, Cordero and Sandi2012). It is interesting that Castro et al. (Reference Castro, Diessler, Varea, Márquez, Larsen, Cordero and Sandi2012) found that a combination of low anxiety and low exploration led to resilience against stress in rats, as this contradicts the fact that curiosity, physical activity, and self-motivation are all thought to be important to resilience. In addition, once released into the wild, orangutans will need to exhibit enough targeted exploratory behaviours appropriate to specific situations to be able to locate food and establish a home range without unnecessary exposure to risk. It is possible that the importance of low anxiety and exploration is specific to resilience in rats, or the laboratory environment, and so further research needs to be carried out here to understand the relationship between individual characteristics and levels of resilience. Liu et al. (Reference Liu, Atrooz, Salvi and Salim2017) identified a resilient group of subjects who showed earlier signs of anxiety and depression following acute stress, but had no behavioural deficits later in life. In another rat study, rough-and-tumble play during chronic unpredictable stress protected against any negative effects (Burgdorf et al. Reference Burgdorf, Kroes and Moskal2017). These protective characteristics should be further explored, to see whether they are relevant in other contexts.

Controllability and predictability of stress

Uncontrollable stress can lead to ‘learned helplessness’ (see S1 Appendix; Supplementary material), a term which describes the failure to respond to avoidable shocks, due to prior experience of inescapable traumatic shock (Overmier & Seligman Reference Overmier and Seligman1967). This should be taken seriously in the context of orangutan rehabilitation, where space is limited and orangutans have very little control or ability to make decisions to change their situation. For example, they are restricted in what and when they eat, who they have social contact with, and where they can go while in the rehabilitation centre. Maier (Reference Maier2001) found that inescapable stress followed by repeated exposure to the specific context in which stress took place prolonged depression and learned helplessness in rats indefinitely. Captive orangutans may be repeatedly exposed to stressful contexts such as relatively small enclosures or visits from the vet. Repeatedly ‘refreshing’ the stress associated with captivity and small environments may have long-lasting detrimental effects. It would be beneficial to identify coping strategies currently used by orangutans in rehabilitation to manage long-term stressors.

There is strong evidence that there are positive effects of being able to control electric shock stress in male rodents, in protecting against stress and depression when exposed to a variety of future stressors and enhancing social exploration and the ability to learn how to discriminate between positively and negatively reinforced odours (see Table 2[a]). Similar benefits are seen in sheep. Greiveldinger et al. (Reference Greiveldinger, Veissier and Boissy2009) found that teaching lambs how to control an airblast to the muzzle during feeding led to lower emotionality and vigilance compared with subjects who did not have the opportunity to control the stressor. Predictability can also reduce the impact of stress. Lambs that experienced a predictable sudden event – either due to regularity of appearance, or because it was associated with a cue – had a reduced startle response and less disruption to feeding than those that experienced an unpredictable event (Greiveldinger et al. Reference Greiveldinger, Veissier and Boissy2007). Although neither predictability nor controllability of the stressor had a noticeable effect on chicken (Gallus domesticus) behaviour, loss of both had the effect of increasing their frustration, measured by higher levels of vocalisations (Zimmerman & Koene Reference Zimmerman and Koene1998). When learned helplessness has occurred, two studies on dogs and rats, respectively, found that it can be reversed by force, i.e. by a human physically dragging the animal away from the source of electric shock (Seligman et al. Reference Seligman, Maier and Geer1968, Reference Seligman, Rosellini and Kozak1975). Although the ethical problems with these methods are substantial, it does indicate that recovery from learned helplessness is theoretically possible.

Direct stress summary

This section clearly demonstrates the potential harms from both acute and chronic forms of stress. However, some forms of milder, less extended, and more controllable forms of stress may have potential benefits. It is natural for orangutans to experience stressful situations in the wild, and it is therefore important for rehabilitant orangutans to be given the opportunity to develop resilience by successfully overcoming a range of different challenges prior to release. Puzzle-boxes, or treatments such as a controllable airblast during feeding, may be worth considering, as they provide a mildly frustrating – but ultimately rewarding – challenge that can be resolved through persistence and problem-solving. The importance of effort-based reward is something that has been addressed in human studies. For example, Dweck (Reference Dweck1975) found a way to ‘treat’ learned helplessness in human children, by teaching them to take responsibility by attributing failures to a lack of effort, rather than ability. Appropriate difficulty levels of cognitive enrichment will be discussed further in Environmental conditions below. There may also be specific personality traits that help to protect against stress, such as low anxiety, low emotionality, and positive affect, which may help orangutans adapt to a new environment. However, although generally low levels of anxiety may be beneficial, it is important that orangutans are still responsive to genuine threats, so that these can be avoided. Therefore, interventions that induce an anxious response to specific threats, for example snakes and humans, can still be utilised, even if overall low anxiety levels are desirable for resilience in orangutans.

Separation

This section is particularly relevant for three reasons. The first is that nearly all orangutans in rehabilitation have been separated from their mothers at an early age, which is likely to cause considerable distress and persisting trauma considering the long-lasting mother/infant bond in orangutans (van Noordwijk & van Schaik Reference van Noordwijk and van Schaik2005). The second reason is that human caretakers will not have the same intensity and duration of contact that the orangutans would normally have with their mother. On the other hand, too much contact with humans may increase dependence on human care (unpublished OVAG questionnaire, L Bridgeland-Stephens 2020) and lead to orangutans seeking out humans after they have been released into the wild, which could pose a considerable risk to the orangutans. The balance between ‘tough’ and ‘motherly’ approaches to caretaking is discussed in more detail by Palmer (Reference Palmer2020). Therefore, it is important to understand the effects of the presence and/or absence of a caretaker, so that an optimal amount and appropriate form of contact between humans and orangutans in a rehabilitation setting can be determined. Lastly, understanding the effects of separation has implications when considering the logistics of housing orangutans in groups, and the potential effects of isolation if this is not possible.

Brief separations

Brief daily separation treatments (15–60 min) have a range of beneficial effects, and this is supported by a strong body of evidence. Over the past two decades, a series of stress inoculation studies were carried out on squirrel monkeys (Samiri sciureus) raised in a normal, species-typical way in a laboratory environment. These involved moving subjects from their natal group to an enclosure adjacent to an unfamiliar group for one hour each week, for a period of ten weeks during adolescence, with the purpose of inoculating the monkeys against future stress. This treatment resulted in benefits such as reduced anxiety, increased time spent exploring and interacting with novel objects, and enhanced inhibitory control (Parker et al. Reference Parker, Buckmaster, Schatzberg and Lyons2004, Reference Parker, Buckmaster, Justus, Schatzberg and Lyons2005, Reference Parker, Rainwater, Buckmaster, Schatzberg, Lindley and Lyons2007, Reference Parker, Buckmaster, Lindley, Schatzberg and Lyons2012, Reference Parker, Buckmaster, Hyde, Schatzberg and Lyons2019). However, it should be noted that these studies were all carried out by the same research team, and some research subjects were used for multiple studies, so caution should be taken in generalising the results to other populations. Many rodent studies found positive effects in adulthood of 15 min of daily keeper handling (involving separation from the litter) during infancy or adolescence, including protection against stress, a reduction in anxiety, depression, and fear, and increased playfulness and exploration (see Table 2[b]). These brief separation treatments may be transferable to the context of orangutan rehabilitation. Exposing young infant orangutans to mild separation stress by removing them from their social group and placing them next to unfamiliar orangutans may have stress-inoculating benefits. However, careful management decisions would need to be made on an individual basis regarding the appropriateness of this method. In rehabilitation centres, detailed knowledge of prior traumatic events experienced by individual orangutans is usually not available. As the studies detailed above are carried out on normally reared laboratory animals with known histories, there is less risk of unexpected trauma surfacing during the separations. This potential interaction between prior stress and the effect of separation is related more generally to the nature of the relationship between life stressors and resilience, discussed in the section above on human resilience.

Table 2(b). Summary of maternal/group separation treatment effects by taxonomic group, and sex (96/266 papers reviewed)

(Grey-shaded cells highlight primate studies; S = sex (F = female, M = male, B = both/mixed, ? = not stated)

Longer separations

There is fairly consistent evidence in rodents that longer periods of separation (2–4 h daily) have a range of negative effects (see Table 2[b]), and this is also supported by a primate study (Pryce et al. Reference Pryce, Dettling, Spengler, Spaete and Feldon2004). Wild orangutan infants are in near-continuous bodily contact with their mother for the first two years of life, only gaining full locomotory independence at 5–6, and sharing her nest until weaning at 6–8 years old (van Noordwijk & van Schaik Reference van Noordwijk and van Schaik2005; van Adrichem et al. Reference van Adrichem, Utami, Wich, van Hooff and Sterck2006). Even years after gaining independence, orangutans have been observed to occasionally ‘visit’ their mother at eleven years old (van Noordwijk et al. Reference van Noordwijk, Sauren, Abham, Morrogh-Bernard, Atmoko, van Schaik, Wich, Atmoko, Setia and van Schaik2009). However, early maternal separation is inevitable for nearly all orangutans rescued from the illegal wildlife trade, as the process of capture usually requires the death of the infants’ mother (Nijman Reference Nijman2005). This means that infant orangutans rely mostly on a combination of human caretakers and peer-rearing. A study of laboratory chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) found that individuals who had been separated from their mothers earlier, and isolated for longer, were less social, less dominant, and more affected by stressful experiences (Reimers et al. Reference Reimers, Schwarzenberger and Preuschoft2007). Compared with maternally raised individuals, same-species surrogacy leads to a reduction in locomotion and time spent exploring in a number of other studies (Sackett Reference Sackett1972; Röder et al. Reference Röder, Timmermans and Vossen1989; Corcoran et al. Reference Corcoran, Pierre, Haddad, Bice, Suomi, Grant, Friedman and Bennett2012). In the context of orangutan rehabilitation, early maternal separation will unavoidably have a detrimental impact. However, social support may help to ameliorate stress; in one marmoset study (Callithrix kuhli), anxiety experienced upon moving to a new enclosure was reduced by the presence of their breeding partner during the transition (Smith et al. Reference Smith, McGreer-Whitworth and French1998).

Prolonged isolation

Orangutans in the illegal wildlife trade may spend prolonged periods of time, even years, with no conspecific contact. However, it may be possible to reverse certain behavioural effects of isolation. Some 1970s primate studies measured the consequences of extreme forms of separation in rhesus macaques. Although disturbing in their methods, two studies have demonstrated successful reversals of the traumatic effects of prolonged isolation. Suomi and Harlow (Reference Suomi and Harlow1972) managed to reverse the effects of six months of isolation in four male rhesus monkeys, by socially exposing them to younger, normally reared female monkeys. After six months of ‘social rehabilitation’, isolated monkeys demonstrated similar social behaviour to controls and an absence of stereotypies. Similar results were achieved in another study of the same species after an entire year of total isolation (Novak & Harlow Reference Novak and Harlow1975). Particularly traumatised orangutans may therefore benefit from peer support in carefully arranged group housing, so that individuals who are coping more effectively can experience a positive effect by association (see Social stress for potential risks). Another technique which has been borrowed from the rehabilitation of ex-laboratory primates is increasing the ratio of caretakers to orangutans in order to monitor progress more carefully, although this may result in excess humanisation (Palmer Reference Palmer2020).

Separation stress summary

There are many potential harms from extended periods of maternal/group separation. However, briefer separations could be used as a mechanism to create a positive, stress-inoculating effect, as seen in the squirrel monkey studies by Parker and colleagues. Maintaining contact with a social peer during separations may help to reduce associated stress. These kinds of ‘gentle’, sporadic separations should be explored as a potentially appropriate treatment for orangutans in rehabilitation, applying a principle of caution and initially trialling with very short periods of time.

Environmental conditions

Despite best efforts, captive environments can never equal the spatial and temporal complexity of life in the forest, and the level of enrichment provided by wild environments. However, there is great potential here, as environmental conditions can be relatively easily controlled in a rehabilitation setting. The treatments in this section either involve environmental enrichment or environmental stress/deprivation. Understanding the negative effects of environmental deprivation is important for orangutan rehabilitation, as most individuals will experience poor conditions before arriving at the rehabilitation centre. Even during rehabilitation, captive environments are relatively deprived of enrichment compared to the complex and dynamic forest environment, and so it is important that opportunities for enrichment are maximised within these constraints.

Environmental enrichment

Environmental enrichment can have a variety of benefits in rodents, including protecting against the negative effects of future stress on anxiety, depression, fear, and emotional reactivity, as well as increasing activity levels, learning ability, and spatial memory (see Table 2[c]). Cognitive challenges can also have beneficial effects, with effort-based reward training in rats leading to improved persistence and problem-solving (Bardi et al. Reference Bardi, Rhone, Franssen, Hampton, Shea, Hyer, Huber and Lambert2012; Lambert et al. Reference Lambert, Hyer, Rzucidlo, Bergeron, Landis and Bardi2014). This makes sense in terms of having the opportunity to exercise agency (see S1 Appendix; Supplementary material), and overcoming a certain degree of challenge having the effect of eliciting a positive emotional state (Clark Reference Clark2011). There is also strong evidence that environmental enrichment improves the well-being of pigs (Sus scrofa domesticus), including promoting long-term memory, mobility, and diversity of behaviour, and reducing emotionality (De Jong et al. Reference De Jong, Prelle, van De Burgwal, Lambooij, Korte, Blokhuis and Koolhaas2000; Wemelsfelder et al. Reference Wemelsfelder, Haskell, Mendl, Calvert and Lawrence2000; Puppe et al. Reference Puppe, Ernst, Schön and Manteuffel2007). However, the form of enrichment used may be important. In a study comparing the effects of natural and artificial (e.g. plastic/manufactured) novel object enrichment on rats, Lambert et al. (Reference Lambert, Hyer, Bardi, Rzucidlo, Scott, Terhune-Cotter, Hazelgrove, Silva and Kinsley2016) observed that rats interacted nearly three times more with natural enrichment items than artificial enrichment items matched according to their functionality and purpose (i.e. climbing, shelter, or manipulation), as well as having a greater reduction in anxiety-like behaviours. Although natural enrichment may be preferable, the practicalities of this with larger animals can be complicated, as enrichment and enclosure features must also be durable and not easily destroyed. Chappell and Thorpe (Reference Chappell and Thorpe2022) argue that non-natural enclosure modifications can simulate the mechanics of a natural environment and provide similar physical and cognitive challenges to those provided by wild environments.

Table 2(c). Summary of environmental conditions treatment effects by taxonomic group, and sex (41/266 papers reviewed)

(Grey-shaded cells highlight primate studies; S = sex (F = female, M = male, B = both/mixed, ? = not stated)

However, moving from an enriched to a barren environment can be more harmful than only experiencing barren environments (Bolhuis et al. Reference Bolhuis, Schouten, Schrama and Wiegant2006). In studies of pigs, moving from an enriched to a barren environment has been found to reduce activity levels (Bolhuis et al. Reference Bolhuis, Schouten, Schrama and Wiegant2006), and increase pessimism in a cognitive bias test used to measure attitudes towards an ambiguous cue (Douglas et al. Reference Douglas, Bateson, Walsh, Bédué and Edwards2012) Similarly, starlings (Sturnus vulgaris) who had moved from enriched to barren environments had increased pessimism in a cognitive bias test (Bateson & Matheson Reference Bateson and Matheson2007). This has implications for orangutan rehabilitation, as orangutans are likely to experience a fluctuation of environmental conditions, from wild environments to extremely poor captive conditions (for illegally traded individuals), to improved conditions at the rehabilitation centre with varying levels of enrichment, depending on the stage of rehabilitation. It is not clear from the literature how a series of multiple changes in condition would affect individuals, and this makes it very difficult to be sure how changing environmental conditions are likely to impact orangutans in rehabilitation. However, it seems likely that an increasing trajectory of environmental complexity would be beneficial, with orangutans in rehabilitation experiencing gradual increases in enrichment and autonomy and, as far as possible, avoiding a reversal in conditions.

Environmental stress/deprivation

Most evidence suggests that environmental deprivation, for example wire floors and restricted bedding during infancy, has a range of negative effects in rodents, for example increased anxiety, impaired spatial memory and novel object recognition, and reduced exploration (see Table 2[c]). However, the studies on environmental stress in birds and fish had less straightforward results. In zebra finches (Taeniopygia guttata), a low quality diet led to faster spatial learning and exploration compared to individuals on a high quality diet (Krause et al. Reference Krause, Honarmand, Wetzel and Naguib2009; Kriengwatana et al. Reference Kriengwatana, Farrell, Aitken, Garcia and MacDougall-Shackleton2015), although spatial memory was impaired (Kriengwatana et al. Reference Kriengwatana, Farrell, Aitken, Garcia and MacDougall-Shackleton2015). In the Panamanian bishop (Brachyrhaphis episcopi), high-predation environments were associated with increased activity and exploration (Archard & Braithwaite Reference Archard and Braithwaite2011). Exposure to environmental stressors such as food availability, or risk of predation, may therefore have some potential benefits in fish and birds, such as increased motivation and exploration. However, a study of foraging demand on bonnet macaques (Macaca radiata) found – perhaps surprisingly – that infants whose mothers had to periodically forage in woodchip were less likely to explore a novel environment than those for whom food was easily available (Andrews & Rosenblum Reference Andrews and Rosenblum1993). This indicates that there may be taxonomic and context-specific differences in how various types of environmental stress are experienced, and that human-controlled environmental deprivation may have different effects to more naturally arising situations such as competition for food.

Environmental conditions summary

The damaging effects of early life stress are extremely relevant to the context of orangutan rehabilitation, as many infant orangutans are rescued from the illegal wildlife trade and found in deprived conditions. In situations where an orangutan is rescued from a human environment, it is reasonable to assume that some detrimental effects of early life stress, e.g. increased anxiety and reduced exploration, may carry through into adulthood, emerging over time. Environmental enrichment can protect against stress, reduce anxiety, depression, and fear, and promote physical activity and spatial memory. However, moving animals from enriched to unenriched environments may have unintended negative effects. This shift in environmental conditions can occur in several circumstances. For example, despite existing enrichment provision in rehabilitation centres, e.g. hammocks and feeding enrichment (Damerius et al. Reference Damerius, Graber, Willems and van Schaik2017), orangutans who have been living in the wild before arriving at a rehabilitation centre will experience a large shift in conditions, from a complex forest environment to the relative deprivation of captivity. However, even within rehabilitation centres there is limited space, and orangutans may need to be temporarily housed in smaller enclosures than those to which they are accustomed. For example, infant orangutans too old for ‘forest school’, i.e. learning skills under human supervision in an area of open forest during the day, may need to be housed for some years in an enclosure before they are old enough to be released into the wild (R Jaya, personal communication 2022). Similarly, orangutans who spend time on a ‘pre-release island’ to experience semi-independent living may be temporarily returned to their enclosure before being released into the wild. In these situations, the regression back to an enclosed environment may have considerably negative effects, including increasing pessimism, which is discussed in the following section. This could be heightened if enclosed environments have negative associations, for example in orangutans who have experienced life in the illegal wildlife trade or who have other prior experience of enclosed environments. Regression to a smaller environment is also likely to reduce levels of physical activity, which in turn is likely to have a negative impact on resilience.

In humans, optimism and pessimism are considered to influence the capacity for resilience; for example, affecting self-perception, environmental perception, and how information is processed and actioned (Forgeard & Seligman Reference Forgeard and Seligman2012). Hobfoll (Reference Hobfoll2002) argues that optimism, self-esteem, and a sense of agency, overlap and tend to be correlated. Optimists can actively approach challenges in a constructive way, perceiving the possibility to act in order to alter outcomes (Forgeard & Seligman Reference Forgeard and Seligman2012). Therefore, an optimistic outlook may help orangutans maintain a sense of control over their surroundings. However, it could also be argued that temporary realistic pessimism may be more appropriate in the face of danger, to prepare for worst-case scenarios and lower expectations of success to prevent disappointment (Forgeard & Seligman Reference Forgeard and Seligman2012). Therefore, a careful balance should be maintained here; the beneficial effects of enrichment can be used in parallel with some constructive environmental challenges that encourage persistence and problem-solving. This requires an understanding of appropriate difficulty levels, so that the challenge presents some degree of frustration, but ultimately elicits a positive emotional state rather than apathy, boredom, or a negative emotional state (Clark Reference Clark2011). In addition, long-term planning is important to ensure that the positive effects are long-lasting and maintained up until the point orangutans are released. It would also be helpful to establish an understanding of current enrichment practices in rehabilitation centres, to measure the effectiveness of different methods and explore the relationship between environmental enrichment and optimism/pessimism in orangutans.

Social stress

As orangutans are semi-solitary animals who live in dispersed societies, only forming temporary aggregations (Galdikas Reference Galdikas1985; Malone et al. Reference Malone, Fuentes and White2012; Roth et al. Reference Roth, Rianti, Fredriksson, Wich and Nowak2020), proximity with large numbers of other orangutans in rehabilitation centres may cause stress, particularly where direct conflict between conspecifics occurs. However, the sociability of orangutans may be partially dependent upon resource availability. For example, Schuppli et al. (Reference Schuppli, Forss, Meulman, Atmoko and van Noordwijk2017) found that Sumatran orangutans (Pongo abelii) in an area with higher food availability were more sociable and, in turn, more exploratory, than Bornean orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus warmbii) in an area with lower food availability. This indicates that there is some degree of behavioural flexibility, and/or species differences, in the sociability of orangutans. Therefore, appropriate levels of sociability in orangutan rehabilitation centres may vary depending on a number of factors, including localised differences and individual tendencies.

While in captivity, and after being released into the wild, it is important for orangutans to be able to communicate appropriately and navigate social situations in an effective way. Among treatments reviewed, the most common type of social stress included brief (often 15 min) exposure to a larger, more dominant, strain of rat, with the interaction ending in ‘social defeat’ (see S1 Appendix; Supplementary material), i.e. forced subordination, for the focal animal. Another treatment, called ‘chronic social stress’ or ‘social instability stress’, involved rotating individuals around unfamiliar conspecifics over consecutive days. Both forms of stress were found to have a negative effect in rodents (see Table 2[d]). As with other forms of stress, the negative effects of social defeat stress in male rodents can be ameliorated with regular female companionship throughout the period of stress (Shi et al. Reference Shi, Hu, Ren and Dai2023). There is also evidence for social stress being contagious, and observing a conspecific experiencing social defeat can lead to depressive and anxious behaviours, and memory deficits in rats (Patki et al. Reference Patki, Solanki and Salim2014), although the close social structure in rats may heighten the negative effect of this treatment. However, if applicable to orangutans, this aspect of social stress would be particularly relevant in the context of great ape rehabilitation, as individuals are likely to be exposed to other stressed individuals, potentially exacerbating their own stress, learning to copy stress-related behaviours such as stereotypies, and/or complicating or prolonging the rehabilitation process. Conversely, indirect exposure to a dominant individual can have a stress-inoculating effect in rodents. This treatment does not involve social defeat, but rather confinement to a small part of an enclosure which houses a dominant individual. This can lead to a range of positive effects, including a more active coping strategy, increased exploration and social interaction, and reduced anxiety (Brockhurst et al. Reference Brockhurst, Cheleuitte-Nieves, Buckmaster, Schatzberg and Lyons2015; Lyons et al. Reference Lyons, Buckmaster and Schatzberg2018; Ayash et al. Reference Ayash, Schmitt, Lyons and Müller2020).

Table 2(d). Summary of social stress treatment effects by taxonomic group, and sex (26/266 papers reviewed)

(Grey-shaded cells highlight primate studies; S = sex (F = female, M = male, B = both/mixed, ? = not stated)

In summary, social stress can cause a range of negative effects and can also be contagious, as exposure to stressed rats can cause similar effects to direct stress. However, milder and less direct forms of social stress may have an ‘inoculating’ effect, helping to promote a more active coping strategy, reduce depression and anxiety, and increase exploration and social interaction. Therefore, exposing individuals to mildly challenging social situations may be beneficial, as long as less dominant individuals are not being subjected to prolonged or repeated aggression from more dominant conspecifics. Orangutans are already likely to encounter some degree of social challenge during their time in rehabilitation, for example being moved to an enclosure with unfamiliar conspecifics as infants. However, any social stress is either likely to be one-off (e.g. the initial experience of encountering unfamiliar orangutans) or chronic (e.g. ongoing competition within the enclosure group), rather than the brief, regular, and time-constrained social stressors described in this section. As young orangutans and adult female orangutans in the wild are more likely to associate with related females, particularly their mothers (Ashbury et al. Reference Ashbury, Willems, Utami Atmoko, Saputra, van Schaik and van Noordwijk2020), it may be stressful to be in prolonged proximity to unrelated orangutans, especially males (Kunz et al. Reference Kunz, Duvot, van Noordwijk, Willems, Townsend, Mardianah, Utami Atmoko, Vogel, Nugraha, Heistermann, Agil and Weingrill2021). Therefore, it would be beneficial to test the effects of a more controlled programme of mild social stress. In addition, the potential dynamics of stress contagion, balanced with the therapeutic effects of conspecifics (see Prolonged isolation), should be explored further, to identify potential benefits while minimising further harm.

Exercise

Building locomotion skills is an important aspect of orangutan rehabilitation, as orangutans must be cognitively and physically capable of traversing the complex forest environment (Thorpe et al. Reference Thorpe, Crompton and Alexander2007; Tecwyn Reference Tecwyn2013; Halsey et al. Reference Halsey, Coward, Crompton and Thorpe2017). However, locomotion can also have beneficial psychological side-effects as a form of exercise. There is strong evidence that voluntary wheel-running in rodents can protect against stress, and additional evidence that forced exercise can be beneficial in some situations (see Table 2[e]). Zhang et al. (Reference Zhang, He, Qu, Li, Wang, Yuan, Hou, Zhu, Cai, Zhang, Guo, An, Jia and Tai2021) found that mice with high baseline levels of physical activity, measured by voluntary wheel-running, that were subjected to social defeat stress were more sociable in a social interaction test than subjects with low levels of physical activity. Voluntary exercise is preferable for several reasons, including ethical ones, and Leasure and Jones (Reference Leasure and Jones2008) found that forced exercise increased anxiety. This suggests that a lack of agency is constraining the potential benefits of exercise in this context. However, these negative results contrast with Greenwood et al. (Reference Greenwood, Foley, Day, Campisi, Hammack, Campeau, Maier and Fleshner2003), who found that both forced and voluntary exercise in rats improved response to fear conditioning and escape in a shuttle-box test. The authors explain this as being due to the experimental apparatus: instead of a treadmill, a wheel was designed to simulate a natural stop-start running pattern and distance that closely resembled voluntary wheel-running. As well as replicating a more natural style of movement for rats, this apparatus also replicates the ‘stop/start’ structure of high intensity interval training in humans. Interval training, where exercise involves short bursts of intense activity alternated with periods of recovery, is thought to help generate fitness and improve cardiac health in humans more quickly than prolonged ‘endurance’ periods of moderate exercise (Wisløff et al. Reference Wisløff, Ellingsen and Kemi2009; Gillen & Gibala Reference Gillen and Gibala2014), as well as having benefits for psychological well-being (Martland et al. Reference Martland, Korman, Firth, Vancampfort, Thompson and Stubbs2022). For orangutans, species-appropriate exercise may involve guided exercise which simulates natural physical activities such as travelling through the canopy, gap-crossing, or bending and breaking branches for nest-building. However, providing them with any opportunity or reason to move will encourage physical activity.

Table 2(e). Summary of exercise treatment effects by taxonomic group, and sex (21/266 papers reviewed)

(Grey-shaded cells highlight primate studies; S = sex (F = female, M = male, B = both/mixed, ? = not stated)

The results in Table 2(e) indicate that species-specific, voluntary exercise may have numerous benefits, particularly in protecting against the effects of future stress and recovering from past stress. ‘Forest school’, where infant orangutans are taken into the forest by human caretakers to learn a variety of different skills, will already involve a certain amount of physical activity (Preuschoft et al. Reference Preuschoft, Yassir, Putri, Aoliya, Yuliani, Badriyah, Corbi, Sugianto, Sitepu and Kalcher‐Sommersguter2021). This is arguably the best opportunity for exercise prior to pre-release islands or living in the wild, in combination with large, complex enclosures that provide plenty of opportunities for locomotion. Although many orangutans will learn by trial and error, some less active individuals may require more encouragement to engage in physical activity. Social learning could be utilised here, as well as potential solutions such as using ropes to pull food higher into the canopy to encourage climbing. In certain circumstances, human caretakers have themselves learned tree-climbing skills to encourage orangutans to climb to higher levels in the canopy (Epstein & Reed Reference Epstein and Reed2019). However, there is a wide variety of forest school capacity and enclosure sizes among different rehabilitation centres, and not all centres have access to pre-release islands. Therefore, careful consideration should be given to the provision of in-cage physical enrichment and roof feeding (Chappell & Thorpe Reference Chappell and Thorpe2022), and, in the longer term, building larger enclosures.

Discussion

In order to effectively integrate resilience interventions into orangutan rehabilitation programmes, it is essential to strike the right balance, by exposing orangutans to opportunities where they can build resilience and prepare for challenges in the wild, while protecting them against further harm. In this review, resilience themes have been drawn from human and animal interventions, and these interventions have been assessed in terms of their potential to be adapted for orangutan rehabilitation centres.

Study limitations

Although this scoping review has spanned multiple disciplines, from animal biology to human psychology, the results have been limited by the search terms used. As these terms centred around ‘resilience’ and ‘stress’, the search may have missed related topics which do not use either of these terms. In addition, as the results show such a diversity of different aspects and expressions of resilience, each of the themes described above could have their own scoping review in terms of relevance for orangutan rehabilitation.

Due to the diversity of species included in this review, from mice to humans, caution must be applied in extrapolating these findings to orangutans. For example, social stress in close-knit, hierarchical taxonomic groups such as mice and rats may be expressed in different ways to orangutans, who are semi-solitary in the wild, living in dispersed societies and only forming temporary aggregations (Galdikas Reference Galdikas1985; Malone et al. Reference Malone, Fuentes and White2012; Roth et al. Reference Roth, Rianti, Fredriksson, Wich and Nowak2020). In addition, most of the animal research presented here is carried out in a laboratory environment, with human-manipulated genetics, where life histories of the research subjects are already known. These conditions are very different to those in orangutan rehabilitation centres, where life histories are rarely known, and orangutans are likely to have had complex life experiences which intrinsically affect resilience. Despite these limitations, the fact that the animal resilience treatments all relate to at least one human resilience theme indicates that there are aspects of resilience which bridge these taxa, despite clear species differences.

In terms of the findings on human resilience interventions, some of these themes may be easier to translate to orangutan rehabilitation centres than others. For example, interventions such as problem-solving, social interactions, and physical activity are relatively straightforward to use as a measure of resilience and/or apply as an intervention in orangutans. In addition, the animal resilience literature indicates that these aspects can be influenced through practical interventions. However, other attributes relevant to human resilience, such as emotional intelligence, self-awareness, and a sense of agency, are more difficult to identify and measure, let alone ‘teach’, through non-linguistic means. However, there is plenty of scope here for investigating treatments which utilise one or more of the human and/or animal themes to foster or measure resilience in the context of orangutan rehabilitation.

Main findings

A recurring theme in this review is that mild and manageable interventions are the safest form of resilience treatment. Benefits were found from interventions that involved short-term and/or manageable forms of stress, including brief separations, mildly frustrating cognitive enrichment, and opportunities to indirectly navigate difficult social interactions. Many of these can be integrated into existing management schedules as part of enrichment provision at rehabilitation centres.

However, there are potential conflicts between different types of stress-related interventions. For example, although mild stress inoculation appears to be beneficial, the possible U-shaped relationship between lifetime stressors and resilience should be considered. There may be a stress ‘threshold’ for orangutans that would be nearly impossible to identify, considering the frequently unknown life histories of orangutans in rehabilitation. Research into this topic might be easier to conduct in a zoo environment, where there are records of entire life histories and major stressors, such as moving to a new zoo. Although orangutans will be exposed to stressful experiences in the wild, they will usually have some opportunity to escape or avoid such situations. As there is limited ability for captive orangutans to control or avoid harms, it is important to limit their exposure as much as possible to potentially traumatic events which they do not have the opportunity to escape. In humans, potentially traumatic events are those which are perceived by the individual as having a real or potential threat to the life or bodily integrity of the self or others (American Psychiatric Association 2013; National Child Traumatic Stress Network [NCTSN] 2023). In the rehabilitation setting, examples of potentially traumatic events may include orangutans witnessing conspecifics being darted for a veterinary procedure, physical threats or attacks from conspecifics in an enclosed space or being confined in a box while being transported to a pre-release island. Peer support may also help orangutans to support one another during stressful experiences. However, more research needs to be carried out on stress contagion and the potential positive and negative effects of housing more stressed individuals with less stressed individuals. This type of arrangement may be beneficial for the more stressed individual but could lead to the negative effects of stress contagion for their conspecific.

Bearing in mind the potential risks of introducing stressful experiences, any investigation of these kinds of interventions should begin with the mildest form, building from this if found to be appropriate and effective. Cognitively demanding challenges through enrichment may be an appropriate way of providing opportunities to overcome controllable frustration and foster independence without causing harm. There are many different forms of enrichment that can be provided to primates, including food-based, occupational (including cognitive), structural, sensory, and social (Bloomsmith et al. Reference Bloomsmith, Brent and Schapiro1991), and each of these categories has the potential to address a number of different factors contributing to resilience. Clark (Reference Clark2011) highlights the importance of maintaining interest and long-term engagement in enrichment, ensuring that the level of complexity is appropriate for the individual, and rotating or modifying enrichment at regular intervals. Physical enrichment is also important, as voluntary exercise can have numerous benefits and protective effects against future stress. Regarding opportunities for exercise, further research is needed into the extent to which orangutans of different age groups have access to forest school, and the frequency/duration of forest school sessions.

In addition to adding enrichment to an enclosure, the design of the enclosure itself is something that can be enriched and modified to encourage cognitive and physical activity, for example by using the Enclosure Design Tool to promote wild-type behaviours (Thorpe et al. Reference Thorpe, Neufuss, Myatt, Tarrega, Wamba, Sulistyo, Benítez López, Chappell, Unwin, White and Landiouw2022). Enrichment provision spans a large proportion of the human resilience themes identified in this review, including optimism/positive affect, exercise/physical activity, cognitive skills, agency/independence, problem-solving, planning, persistence, hardiness, goal orientation, and flexibility. However, the findings of this review indicate that long-term planning is essential to ensure that later exposure to less-enriched conditions does not reverse the benefits. A negative change in environmental conditions may have a severely detrimental effect on resilience: for example, during the change from forest school to enclosed environments, or moving an orangutan back into an enclosure after living on a pre-release island before release into the wild. It would be useful to monitor the effectiveness of existing enrichment methods and explore the relationship between enrichment and optimism/pessimism in orangutans.

Some studies identified a role of resilient personality traits, for example low anxiety, low emotionality, and positive affect. This makes sense in terms of the wider literature on animal personality, where research indicates that personality traits can influence cognitive styles (the way information is acquired and processed), strategies for balancing risk and reward, and well-being (Capitanio Reference Capitanio2011; Sih & Del Giudice Reference Sih and Del Giudice2012; Cole & Quinn Reference Cole and Quinn2014; Zandberg et al. Reference Zandberg, Quinn, Naguib and van Oers2017). One study on chimpanzees found that personality traits accounted for around 50% variance in well-being, with ‘extraversion’, ‘agreeableness’ and ‘low neuroticism’ being particularly relevant (King & Landau Reference King and Landau2003). The importance of personality is also seen in a study of 300 captive gorillas (Gorilla gorilla gorilla), which found that extraversion was associated with longer lifespans (Weiss et al. Reference Weiss, Gartner, Gold and Stoinski2013). Therefore, it is likely that individual orangutans will have different resilience ‘baselines’ and may respond to resilience interventions in different ways depending upon their personality traits. Further research is needed into expected resilience levels in orangutans, and individual variation in coping styles, in order to be able to measure the effectiveness of interventions.

Animal welfare implications

Since so little is known about great ape resilience, it is difficult to know where to start with more ‘risky’ interventions like stress inoculation, which might have the potential to cause further harm. It might be argued that the risks outweigh any potential benefits to orangutans. In zoo environments, for example, where the main consideration for caretakers is the well-being of the animal, deliberately causing stress or frustration might be seen as unacceptable. However, there is a strong justification for investigating these types of resilience treatments, as long as a principle of caution is applied throughout the process. For orangutans that are likely to be returned to the wild, the justification is clear: these individuals will face unavoidable stress and new challenges, probably daily, and preparing them for this reality is essential for their ability not only to cope, but to thrive in their new environment. Although rehabilitation centres strive to teach essential life skills to the orangutans in their care, some challenges will be experienced for the first time after orangutans are released into the wild. For example, orangutans in rehabilitation receive a balanced, regular diet to maximise their well-being in captivity (Schmidt Reference Schmidt2004). However, due to the temporal availability of food in the wild due to tree masting (mass fruiting) events, wild orangutans experience a dramatic fluctuation of weight loss and gain (Knott Reference Knott1998). Having prior experience of manageable stress and frustration will help orangutans develop the persistence and hardiness to endure this kind of difficult experience, and the flexibility and problem-solving skills to overcome them. However, orangutans and other great apes in lifelong captivity, in environments like zoos, can also benefit from the experience of overcoming challenging situations. As well as general stressors associated with zoos (Hosey Reference Hosey2000; Birke Reference Birke2002; Skynner et al. Reference Skynner, Amory and Hosey2004), great apes are also likely to experience major challenges throughout their life, including moving to a new zoo, welcoming a new group member into their enclosure, construction/maintenance work, and medical treatment. Therefore, resilience is important for captive great apes to be able to bounce back from these stressors and maximise their well-being.

It is apparent from these findings that there is substantial scope for further research into understanding great ape resilience and the potential benefits of resilience interventions. More work needs to be done to develop and test individual interventions in each of the areas mentioned above, tailoring each intervention to maximise their effectiveness in captive environments, and trialling different combinations of interventions. Ultimately, the purpose of these investigations would be to prepare for the implementation of a long-term study, to trial and measure the effect of a resilience intervention programme on the survival rate of orangutans released back into the forest. This could act as an essential resource for rehabilitation centres in helping to prepare orangutans as much as possible during their time in rehabilitation and inform decisions as to when each individual is ready to face independent life in the wild.

Conclusion

This review has identified key overarching themes within the human and animal resilience literature and has critically analysed the applicability of different treatments within the context of orangutan rehabilitation. Several starting points have been suggested, with the caveat that further research is necessary into each of these potential treatments and a principle of caution should be applied. In general, the interventions which offer a mild, but manageable, challenge appear to be the most effective and appropriate in the context of orangutan rehabilitation. Due to the lack of existing knowledge about non-human great ape resilience, it is essential that there is a foundation from which effective interventions can be developed. Therefore, this review aims to be a starting point for future research into this essential field, with implications not only for the survival of orangutans released into the wild, but also for the well-being of great apes in all captive environments.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at http://doi.org/10.1017/awf.2023.97.

Acknowledgements

The authors thank members of the Orangutan Veterinary Advisory Group for their engagement in this research. This research was funded by the Central England NERC Training Alliance (CENTA) Doctoral Training Partnership as a PhD studentship awarded to the lead author.

Competing interest

None. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

Footnotes

Author contributions: Conceptualisation: LBS, SKST, JC; Data curation: LBS; Formal analysis: LBS, SKST, JC; Funding acquisition: SKST, JC; Investigation: LBS; Methodology: LBS, JC; Project administration: LBS, SKST, JC; Supervision: SKST, JC; Software: LBS, JC; Visualisation: LBS, SKST, JC; Writing – original draft: LBS; Writing – review & editing: LBS, SKST, JC.

References

Abraham, A and Gruss, M 2010 Stress inoculation facilitates active avoidance learning of the semi-precocial rodent Octodon degus. Behavioural Brain Research 213: 293303. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2010.05.018CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Adamec, RE and Shallow, T 1993 Lasting effects on rodent anxiety of a single exposure to a cat. Physiology and Behavior 54: 101109. https://doi.org/10.1016/0031-9384(93)90050-PCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aisa, B, Tordera, R, Lasheras, B, Del Río, J and Ramírez, MJ 2007 Cognitive impairment associated to HPA axis hyperactivity after maternal separation in rats. Psychoneuroendocrinology 32: 256266. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2006.12.013CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aisa, B, Tordera, R, Lasheras, B, Del Río, J and Ramírez, MJ 2008 Effects of maternal separation on hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal responses, cognition and vulnerability to stress in adult female rats. Neuroscience 154: 12181226. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2008.05.011CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Akeman, E, Aupperle, RL, Clausen, AN, Cosgrove, KT, McDermott, TJ, Cromer, LD, Paulus, MP, Carolina, N, Kirlic, N, Clausen, AN, Cosgrove, KT, Mcdermott, TJ, Cromer, LD, Paulus, MP, Yeh, H and Aupperle, RL 2020 A pragmatic clinical trial examining the impact of a resilience program on college student mental health. Depression and Anxiety 37: 202213. https://doi.org/10.1002/da.22969CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Amat, J, Aleksejev, RM, Paul, E, Watkins, LR and Maier, SF 2010 Behavioral control over shock blocks behavioral and neurochemical effects of later social defeat. Neuroscience 165: 10311038. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2009.11.005.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
American Psychiatric Association 2013 Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-5. American Psychiatric Association: Arlington, VA, USA.Google Scholar
Ancrenaz, M, Gumal, M, Marshall, AJ, Meijaard, E, Wich, SA and Husson, S 2018 Pongo pygmaeus, Bornean orangutan. The IUCN Red List of Threatened Species. IUCN: Cambridge, UK.Google Scholar
Andrews, MW and Rosenblum, LA 1993 Assessment of attachment in differentially reared infant monkeys (Macaca radiata): response to separation and a novel environment. Journal of Comparative Psychology 107: 8490. https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7036.107.1.84CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Archard, GA and Braithwaite, VA 2011 Increased exposure to predators increases both exploration and activity level in Brachyrhaphis episcopi. Journal of Fish Biology 78: 593601. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1095-8649.2010.02880.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ashbury, AM, Willems, EP, Utami Atmoko, SS, Saputra, F, van Schaik, CP and van Noordwijk, MA 2020 Home range establishment and the mechanisms of philopatry among female Bornean orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus wurmbii) at Tuanan. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 74: 121. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-020-2818-1CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Avital, A, Ram, E, Maayan, R, Weizman, A and Richter-Levin, G 2006 Effects of early-life stress on behavior and neurosteroid levels in the rat hypothalamus and entorhinal cortex. Brain Research Bulletin 68: 419424. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainresbull.2005.09.015CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ayash, S, Schmitt, U, Lyons, DM and Müller, MB 2020 Stress inoculation in mice induces global resilience. Translational Psychiatry 10: 200. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41398-020-00889-0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baratta, MV, Leslie, NR, Fallon, IP, Dolzani, SD, Chun, LE, Tamalunas, AM, Watkins, LR and Maier, SF 2018 Behavioural and neural sequelae of stressor exposure are not modulated by controllability in females. European Journal of Neuroscience 47: 959967. https://doi.org/10.1111/ejn.13833CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bardi, M, Rhone, AP, Franssen, CL, Hampton, JE, Shea, EA, Hyer, MM, Huber, J and Lambert, KG 2012 Behavioral training and predisposed coping strategies interact to influence resilience in male Long-Evans rats: Implications for depression. Stress 15: 306317. https://doi.org/10.3109/10253890.2011.623739CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Basalamah, F, Atmoko, SSU, Perwitasari-Farajallah, D, Qayim, I, Sihite, J, van Noordwijk, M, Willems, E and Van Schaik, CP 2018 Monitoring orangutan reintroduction: Results of activity budgets, diets, vertical use and associations during the first year post-release in Kehje Sewen Forest, east Kalimantan, Indonesia. Biodiversitas 19: 609620. https://doi.org/10.13057/biodiv/d190242CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bateson, M and Matheson, SM 2007 Performance on a categorisation task suggests that removal of environmental enrichment induces ‘pessimism’ in captive European starlings (Sturnus vulgaris). Animal Welfare 16: 3336. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0962728600031705CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bath, KG, Nitenson, AS, Lichtman, E, Lopez, C, Chen, W, Gallo, M, Goodwill, H and Manzano-Nieves, G 2017 Early life stress leads to developmental and sex selective effects on performance in a novel object placement task. Neurobiology of Stress 7: 5767. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ynstr.2017.04.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baudin, A, Blot, K, Verney, C, Estevez, L, Santamaria, J, Gressens, P, Giros, B, Otani, S, Daugé, V and Naudon, L 2012 Maternal deprivation induces deficits in temporal memory and cognitive flexibility and exaggerates synaptic plasticity in the rat medial prefrontal cortex. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 98: 207214. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2012.08.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baugher, BJ and Sachs, BD 2022 Early life maternal separation induces sex-specific antidepressant-like responses but has minimal effects on adult stress susceptibility in mice. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience 16: 941884. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2022.941884CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bazak, N, Kozlovsky, N, Kaplan, Z, Matar, M, Golan, H, Zohar, J, Richter-Levin, G and Cohen, H 2009 Pre-pubertal stress exposure affects adult behavioral response in association with changes in circulating corticosterone and brain-derived neurotrophic factor. Psychoneuroendocrinology 34: 844858. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2008.12.018CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beck, KD and Luine, VN 2002 Sex differences in behavioral and neurochemical profiles after chronic stress: Role of housing conditions. Physiology and Behavior 75: 661673. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0031-9384(02)00670-4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benaroya-Milshtein, N, Hollander, N, Apter, A, Kukulansky, T, Raz, N, Wilf, A, Yaniv, I and Pick, CG 2004 Environmental enrichment in mice decreases anxiety, attenuates stress responses and enhances natural killer cell activity. European Journal of Neuroscience 20: 13411347. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-9568.2004.03587.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bian, Y, Yang, L, Wang, Z, Wang, Q, Zeng, L and Xu, G 2015 Repeated three-hour maternal separation induces depression-like behavior and affects the expression of hippocampal plasticity-related proteins in C57BL/6N mice. Neural Plasticity 2015: 627837. https://doi.org/10.1155/2015/627837CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Birke, L 2002 Effects of browse, human visitors and noise on the behaviour of captive orang utans. Animal Welfare 11: 189202. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0962728600028141CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Block, JH and Block, J 1980 The role of ego-control and ego resiliency in the organization of behavior. In: Collins WA (ed) Minnesota Symposium on Child Psychology pp 39101. Erlbaum: Hillsdale, NJ, USA.Google Scholar
Bloomsmith, MA, Brent, LY and Schapiro, SJ 1991 Guidelines for developing and managing an environmental enrichment program for nonhuman primates. Laboratory Animal Science 41: 372377.Google ScholarPubMed
Bolhuis, JE, Schouten, WGP, Schrama, JW and Wiegant, VM 2006 Effects of rearing and housing environment on behaviour and performance of pigs with different coping characteristics. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 101: 6885. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2006.01.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bondi, CO, Rodriguez, G, Gould, GG, Frazer, A and Morilak, DA 2008 Chronic unpredictable stress induces a cognitive deficit and anxiety-like behavior in rats that is prevented by chronic antidepressant drug treatment. Neuropsychopharmacology 33: 320331. https://doi.org/10.1038/sj.npp.1301410CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bouchet, CA, Lloyd, BA, Loetz, EC, Farmer, CE, Ostrovskyy, M, Haddad, N, Foright, RM and Greenwood, BN 2017 Acute exercise enhances the consolidation of fear extinction memory and reduces conditioned fear relapse in a sex-dependent manner. Learning and Memory 24: 358368. https://doi.org/10.1101/lm.045195.117CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bourke, CH and Neigh, GN 2011 Behavioral effects of chronic adolescent stress are sustained and sexually dimorphic. Hormones and Behavior 60: 112120. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2011.03.011CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bowman, RE, Zrull, MC and Luine, VN 2001a Chronic restraint stress enhances radial arm maze performance in female rats. Brain Research 904: 279289. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0006-8993(01)02474-xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bowman, RE, Zrull, MC and Luine, VN 2001b Chronic stress effects on physiology and cognition in female rats. Brain Research 904: 279280. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0006-8993(01)02474-xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bravo-Tobar, ID, Fernández, P, Sáez, JC and Dagnino-Subiabre, A 2021 Long-term effects of stress resilience: Hippocampal neuroinflammation and behavioral approach in male rats. Journal of Neuroscience Research 99: 24932510. https://doi.org/10.1002/jnr.24902CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brockhurst, J, Cheleuitte-Nieves, C, Buckmaster, CL, Schatzberg, AF and Lyons, DM 2015 Stress inoculation modeled in mice. Translational Psychiatry 5: 15. https://doi.org/10.1038/tp.2015.34CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, PL, Hurley, C, Repucci, N and Drugan, RC 2001 Behavioral analysis of stress controllability effects in a new swim stress paradigm. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior 68: 263272. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0091-3057(00)00460-3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, R 2015 Building children and young people’s resilience: Lessons from psychology. International Journal of Disaster Risk Reduction 14: 115124. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijdrr.2015.06.007CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brüne, M, Brüne-Cohrs, U, McGrew, WC and Preuschoft, S 2006 Psychopathology in great apes: Concepts, treatment options and possible homologies to human psychiatric disorders. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 30: 12461259. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2006.09.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brunson, KL, Kramár, E, Lin, B, Chen, Y, Colgin, LL, Yanagihara, TK, Lynch, G and Baram, TZ 2005 Mechanisms of late-onset cognitive decline after early-life stress. Journal of Neuroscience 25: 93289338. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2281-05.2005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bryda, EC 2013 The mighty mouse: The impact of rodents on advances in biomedical research. Missouri Medicine 110: 2072011.Google ScholarPubMed
Burgdorf, J, Kroes, RA and Moskal, JR 2017 Rough-and-tumble play induces resilience to stress in rats. NeuroReport 28: 11221126. https://doi.org/10.1097/WNR.0000000000000864CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Calandreau, L, Bertin, A, Boissy, A, Arnould, C, Constantin, P, Desmedt, A, Guémené, D, Nowak, R and Leterrier, C 2011 Effect of one week of stress on emotional reactivity and learning and memory performances in Japanese quail. Behavioural Brain Research 217: 104110. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2010.10.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caldji, C, Francis, D, Sharma, S, Plotsky, PM and Meaney, MJ 2000 The effects of early rearing environment on the development of GABA(A) and central benzodiazepine receptor levels and novelty-induced fearfulness in the rat. Neuropsychopharmacology 22: 219229. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0893-133X(99)00110-4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Calpe-López, C, Martínez-Caballero, MA, García-Pardo, MP and Aguilar, MA 2022 Intermittent voluntary wheel running promotes resilience to the negative consequences of repeated social defeat in mice. Physiology and Behavior 254: 113916. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2022.113916CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Campos-Cardoso, R, Novaes, LS, Godoy, LD, dos Santos, NB, Perfetto, JG, Lazarini-Lopes, W, Garcia-Cairasco, N, Padovan, CM and Munhoz, CD 2023 The resilience of adolescent male rats to acute stress-induced delayed anxiety is age-related and glucocorticoid release-dependent. Neuropharmacology 226: 109385. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropharm.2022.109385CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cant, JGH 1992 Positional behavior and body size of arboreal primates: A theoretical framework for field studies and an illustration of its application. American Journal of Physical Anthropology 88: 273283. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajpa.1330880302CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Capitanio, JP 2011 Individual differences in emotionality: Social temperament and health. American Journal of Primatology 73: 507515. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.20870CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Castro, JE, Diessler, S, Varea, E, Márquez, C, Larsen, MH, Cordero, MI and Sandi, C 2012 Personality traits in rats predict vulnerability and resilience to developing stress-induced depression-like behaviors, HPA axis hyper-reactivity and brain changes in pERK1/2 activity. Psychoneuroendocrinology 37: 12091223. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2011.12.014CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chaby, LE, Cavigelli, SA, Hirrlinger, AM, Caruso, MJ and Braithwaite, VA 2015a Chronic unpredictable stress during adolescence causes long-term anxiety. Behavioural Brain Research 278: 492495. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2014.09.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chaby, LE, Cavigelli, SA, White, A, Wang, K and Braithwaite, VA 2013 Long-term changes in cognitive bias and coping response as a result of chronic unpredictable stress during adolescence. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 7: 328. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00328CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chaby, LE, Sheriff, MJ, Hirrlinger AM and Braithwaite VA 2015b Does early stress prepare individuals for a stressful future? Stress during adolescence improves foraging under threat. Animal Behaviour 105: 3745. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2015.03.028CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chandler, G and Roberts, S 2015 Resilience intervention for young adults with adverse childhood experiences. Journal of the American Psychiatric Nurses Association 21: 406416. https://doi.org/10.1177/1078390315620609CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chappell, J and Thorpe, SKS 2022 The role of great ape behavioral ecology in One Health: Implications for captive welfare and re-habilitation success. American Journal of Primatology 84. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.23328CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chen, CV, Chaby, LE, Nazeer, S and Liberzon, I 2018 Effects of trauma in adulthood and adolescence on fear extinction and extinction retention: Advancing animal models of post-traumatic stress disorder. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience 12: 247. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2018.00247CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chiba, S, Numakawa, T, Ninomiya, M, Richards, MC, Wakabayashi, C and Kunugi, H 2012 Chronic restraint stress causes anxiety- and depression-like behaviors, downregulates glucocorticoid receptor expression, and attenuates glutamate release induced by brain-derived neurotrophic factor in the prefrontal cortex. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry 39: 112119. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pnpbp.2012.05.018CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Christianson, JP, Paul, ED, Irani, M, Thompson, BM, Kubala, KH, Yirmiya, R, Watkins, LR and Maier, SF 2008 The role of prior stressor controllability and the dorsal raphé nucleus in sucrose preference and social exploration. Behavioural Brain Research 193: 8793. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2008.04.024CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, FE 2011 Great ape cognition and captive care: Can cognitive challenges enhance well-being? Applied Animal Behaviour Science 135: 112. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2011.10.010CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cole, EF and Quinn, JL 2014 Shy birds play it safe: personality in captivity predicts risk responsiveness during reproduction in the wild. Biology Letters 10: 20140178. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2014.0178CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Colorado, RA, Shumake, J, Conejo, NM, Gonzalez-Pardo, H and Gonzalez-Lima, F 2006 Effects of maternal separation, early handling, and standard facility rearing on orienting and impulsive behavior of adolescent rats. Behavioural Processes 71: 5158. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2005.09.007CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Corcoran, CA, Pierre, PJ, Haddad, T, Bice, C, Suomi, SJ, Grant, KA, Friedman, DP and Bennett, AJ 2012 Long-term effects of differential early rearing in rhesus macaques: Behavioral reactivity in adulthood. Developmental Psychobiology 54: 546555. https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.20613CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cotella, EM, Gómez, AS, Lemen, P, Chen, C, Fernández, G, Hansen, C, Herman, JP and Paglini, MG 2019 Long-term impact of chronic variable stress in adolescence versus adulthood. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry 88: 303310. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pnpbp.2018.08.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crawford, LE, Knouse, LE, Kent, M, Vavra, D, Harding, O, LeServe, D, Fox, N, Hu, X, Li, P, Glory, C and Lambert, KG 2020 Enriched environment exposure accelerates rodent driving skills. Behavioural Brain Research 378: 12309. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2019.112309CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cui, M, Yang, Y, Yang, J, Zhang, J, Han, H, Ma, W, Li, H, Mao, R, Xu, L, Hao, W and Cao, J 2006 Enriched environment experience overcomes the memory deficits and depressive-like behavior induced by early life stress. Neuroscience Letters 404: 208212. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2006.05.048CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dalle Molle, R, Portella, AK, Goldani, MZ, Kapczinski, FP, Leistner-Segala, S, Salum, GA, Manfro, GG and Silveira, PP 2012 Associations between parenting behavior and anxiety in a rodent model and a clinical sample: Relationship to peripheral BDNF levels. Translational Psychiatry 2: e195. https://doi.org/10.1038/tp.2012.126CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Damerius, LA, Graber, SM, Willems, EP and van Schaik, CP 2017 Curiosity boosts orang-utan problem-solving ability. Animal Behaviour 134: 5770. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2017.10.005CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Daniels, WMU, Pietersen, CY, Carstens, ME and Stein, DJ 2004 Maternal separation in rats leads to anxiety-like behavior and a blunted ACTH response and altered neurotransmitter levels in response to a subsequent stressor. Metabolic Brain Disease 19: 314. https://doi.org/10.1023/B:MEBR.0000027412.19664.b3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Jong, IC, Prelle, IT, van De Burgwal, JA, Lambooij, E, Korte, SM, Blokhuis, HJ and Koolhaas, JM 2000 Effects of environmental enrichment on behavioral responses to novelty, learning, and memory, and the circadian rhythm in cortisol in growing pigs. Physiology and Behavior 68: 571578. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0031-9384(99)00212-7CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Delaney, C, Barrere, C, Robertson, S, Zahourek, R, Diaz, D and Lachapelle, L 2016 Pilot testing of the NURSE stress management intervention. Journal of Holistic Nursing 34: 369389. https://doi.org/10.1177/0898010115622295CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Denny, RR, Connelly, KL, Ghilotti, MG, Meissler, JJ, Yu, D, Eisenstein, TK and Unterwald, EM 2021 Artificial intelligence identified resilient and vulnerable female rats after traumatic stress and ethanol exposure: Investigation of neuropeptide Y pathway regulation. Frontiers in Neuroscience 15: 772946. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2021.772946CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dishman, RK, Renner, KJ, Youngstedt, SD, Reigle, TG, Bunnell, BN, Burke, KA, Yoo, HS, Mougey, EH and Meyerhoff, JL 1997 Activity wheel running reduces escape latency and alters brain monoamine levels after footshock. Brain Research Bulletin 42: 399406. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0361-9230(96)00329-2CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Doremus-Fitzwater, TL, Varlinskaya, EI and Spear, LP 2009 Social and non-social anxiety in adolescent and adult rats after repeated restraint. Physiology and Behavior 97: 484494. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2009.03.025.SocialCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
dos Santos Guilherme, M, Tsoutsouli, T, Chongtham, MC, Winter, J, Gerber, S, Müller, MB and Endres, K 2022 Selective targeting of chronic social stress-induced activated neurons identifies neurogenesis-related genes to be associated with resilience in female mice. Psychoneuroendocrinology 139: 105700. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2022.105700CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Douglas, C, Bateson, M, Walsh, C, Bédué, A and Edwards, SA 2012 Environmental enrichment induces optimistic cognitive biases in pigs. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 139: 6573. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2012.02.018CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Doyle, RE, Fisher, AD, Hinch, GN, Boissy, A and Lee, C 2010 Release from restraint generates a positive judgement bias in sheep. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 122: 2834. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2009.11.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ducottet, C and Belzung, C 2004 Behaviour in the elevated plus-maze predicts coping after subchronic mild stress in mice. Physiology and Behavior 81: 417426. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2004.01.013CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dweck, CS 1975 The role of expectations and attributions in the alleviation of learned helplessness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 31: 674685. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0077149CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eagle, AL, Fitzpatrick, CJ and Perrine, SA 2013 Single prolonged stress impairs social and object novelty recognition in rats. Behavioural Brain Research 256: 591597. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2013.09.014.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Egeland, B, Carlson, E and Sroufe, LA 1993 Resilience as process. Development and Psychopathology 5: 517528. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579400006131CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eiland, L and McEwen, BS 2012 Early life stress followed by subsequent adult chronic stress potentiates anxiety and blunts hippocampal structural remodeling. Hippocampus 22: 8291. https://doi.org/10.1002/hipo.20862CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Epstein, JM and Reed, J 2019 Tree climbing methodology for orangutan conservation. Primate Conservation: 5964.Google Scholar
Fabrega, H 2002 The origins of psychopathology: The phylogenetic and cultural basis of mental illness. Rutgers University Press: New Brunswick, NJ, USA.Google Scholar
Farrell, MR, Holland, FH, Shansky, RM and Brenhouse, HC 2016 Sex-specific effects of early life stress on social interaction and prefrontal cortex dendritic morphology in young rats. Behavioural Brain Research 310: 119125. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2016.05.009CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Feng, X, Wang, L, Yang, S, Qin, D, Wang, J, Li, C, Lv, L, Ma, Y and Hu, X 2011 Maternal separation produces lasting changes in cortisol and behavior in rhesus monkeys. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 108: 1431214317. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1010943108CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fernandez, CA, Choi, KW, Marshall, BDL, Vicente, B, Saldivia, S, Kohn, R, Koenen, KC, Arheart, KL and Buka, SL 2020 Assessing the relationship between psychosocial stressors and psychiatric resilience among Chilean disaster survivors. British Journal of Psychiatry 217: 630637. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.2020.88CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fletcher, D and Sarkar, M 2013 Psychological resilience: A review and critique of definitions, concepts, and theory. European Psychologist 18: 1223. https://doi.org/10.1027/1016-9040/a000124CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Forgeard, MJC and Seligman, MEP 2012 Seeing the glass half full: A review of the causes and consequences of optimism. Pratiques Psychologiques 18: 107120. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.prps.2012.02.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Foster, K, Cuzzillo, C and Furness, T 2018 Strengthening mental health nurses’ resilience through a workplace resilience programme: A qualitative inquiry. Journal of Psychiatric and Mental Health Nursing 25: 338348. https://doi.org/10.1111/jpm.12467CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Francis, DD, Diorio, J, Plotsky, PM and Meaney, MJ 2002 Environmental enrichment reverses the effects of maternal separation on stress reactivity. Journal of Neuroscience 22: 78407843. https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.22-18-07840.2002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fulk, LJ, Stock, HS, Lynn, A, Marshall, J, Wilson, MA and Hand, GA 2004 Chronic physical exercise reduces anxiety-like behavior in rats. International Journal of Sports Medicine 25: 7882. https://doi.org/10.1055/s-2003-45235Google ScholarPubMed
Fuss, J, Ben Abdallah, NMB, Vogt, MA, Touma, C, Pacifici, PG, Palme, R, Witzemann, V, Hellweg, R and Gass, P 2010 Voluntary exercise induces anxiety-like behavior in adult C57BL/6J mice correlating with hippocampal neurogenesis. Hippocampus 20: 364376. https://doi.org/10.1002/hipo.20634CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Galdikas, BMF 1985 Orangutan sociality at Tanjung-Puting. American Journal of Primatology 9: 101119. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.1350090204CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gallegos-Guajardo, J, Ruvalcaba-Romero, NA, Langley, A and Villegas-Guinea, D 2015 Selective prevention for anxiety and resilience promotion: Outcomes of an anxiety prevention and resilience program with girls at risk. Pensando Psicología 11: 1123. https://doi.org/10.16925/pe.v11i18.1001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
García-Capdevila, S, Portell-Cortés, I, Torras-Garcia, M, Coll-Andreu, M and Costa-Miserachs, D 2009 Effects of long-term voluntary exercise on learning and memory processes: dependency of the task and level of exercise. Behavioural Brain Research 202: 162170. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2009.03.020CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garcia-Marquez, C and Armario, A 1987 Chronic stress depresses exploratory activity and behavioral performance in the forced swimming test without altering ACTH response to a novel acute stressor. Physiology and Behavior 40: 3338. https://doi.org/10.1016/0031-9384(87)90182-XCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gardner, KL, Thrivikraman, K V., Lightman, SL, Plotsky, PM and Lowry, CA 2005 Early life experience alters behavior during social defeat: Focus on serotonergic systems. Neuroscience 136: 181191. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.07.042CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gerber, MM, Frankfurt, SB, Contractor, AA, Oudshoorn, K, Dranger, P and Brown, LA 2018 Influence of multiple traumatic event types on mental health outcomes: Does count matter? Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment 40: 645654. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10862-018-9682-6CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gillen, HB and Gibala, MJ 2014 Is high-intensity interval training a time-efficient exercise strategy to improve health and fitness? Interval Training 39: 409412. https://doi.org/10.1139/apnm-2013-0187@apnm-it.issue01Google ScholarPubMed
Gonzalez, ST, Marty, V, Spigelman, I, Reise, SP and Fanselow, MS 2021 Impact of stress resilience and susceptibility on fear learning, anxiety, and alcohol intake. Neurobiology of Stress 15: 100335. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ynstr.2021.100335CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodwill, HL, Manzano-Nieves, G, Gallo, M, Lee, HI, Oyerinde, E, Serre, T and Bath, KG 2019 Early life stress leads to sex differences in development of depressive-like outcomes in a mouse model. Neuropsychopharmacology 44: 711720. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41386-018-0195-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grassi-Oliveira, R, Honeycut, J, Holland, FH, Ganguly, P, Brenhouse, HC, Yang, G, Sau, C, Lai, W, Cichon, J and Li, W 2016 Cognitive impairment effects of early life stress in adolescents can be predicted with early biomarkers: Impacts of sex, experience, and cytokines. Psychoneuroendocrinology 71: 1930. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2016.04.016.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Great Ape Taxon Advisory Group 2018 Orangutan EEP best practice guidelines. Great Ape Taxon Advisory Group, EAZA: Amsterdam, The NetherlandsGoogle Scholar
Green, MR, Barnes, B and McCormick, CM 2013 Social instability stress in adolescence increases anxiety and reduces social interactions in adulthood in male long-evans rats. Developmental Psychobiology 55: 849859. https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.21077CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenwood, BN, Foley, TE, Burhans, D, Maier, SF and Fleshner, M 2005 The consequences of uncontrollable stress are sensitive to duration of prior wheel running. Brain Research 1033: 164178. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2004.11.037CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenwood, BN, Foley, TE, Day, HEW, Campisi, J, Hammack, SH, Campeau, S, Maier, SF and Fleshner, M 2003 Freewheel running prevents learned helplessness/behavioral depression: Role of dorsal raphe serotonergic neurons. Journal of Neuroscience 23: 28892898. https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.23-07-02889.2003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenwood, BN, Loughridge, AB, Sadaoui, N, Christianson, JP and Fleshner, M 2012 The protective effects of voluntary exercise against the behavioral consequences of uncontrollable stress persist despite an increase in anxiety following forced cessation of exercise. Behavioural Brain Research 233: 314321. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2012.05.017.TheCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenwood, BN, Spence, KG, Crevling, DM, Clark, PJ, Craig, WC and Fleshner, M 2013 Exercise-induced stress resistance is independent of exercise controllability and the medial prefrontal cortex. European Journal of Neuroscience 37: 469478. https://doi.org/10.1111/ejn.12044CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenwood, BN, Strong, PV, Dorey, AA and Fleshner, M 2007 Therapeutic effects of exercise: Wheel running reverses stress-induced interference with shuttle box escape. Behavioral Neuroscience 121: 9921000. https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7044.121.5.992CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greiveldinger, L, Veissier, I and Boissy, A 2007 Emotional experience in sheep: Predictability of a sudden event lowers subsequent emotional responses. Physiology and Behavior 92: 675683. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.05.012CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greiveldinger, L, Veissier, I and Boissy, A 2009 Behavioural and physiological responses of lambs to controllable vs. uncontrollable aversive events. Psychoneuroendocrinology 34: 805814. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2008.10.025CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grippo, AJ, Gerena, D, Huang, J, Kumar, N, Shah, M and Carter, CS 2007 Social isolation induces behavioral and neuroendocrine disturbances relevant to depression in female and male prarie voles. Psychoneuroendocrinology 32: 966980.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grippo, AJ, Wu, KD, Hassan, I and Carter, CS 2008 Social isolation in prairie voles induces behaviors relevant to negative affect: toward the development of a rodent model focused. Depression and Anxiety 25: E17E26. https://doi.org/10.1002/da.20375.SocialCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grotberg, E 1995 A guide to promoting resilience in children: strengthening the human spirit. Early Childhood Development: Practice and Reflections 8: 139.Google Scholar
Halsey, LG, Coward, SRL, Crompton, RH and Thorpe, SKS 2017 Practice makes perfect: Performance optimisation in ‘arboreal’ parkour athletes illuminates the evolutionary ecology of great ape anatomy. Journal of Human Evolution 103: 4552. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jhevol.2016.11.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Han, X, Wang, W, Xue, X, Shao, F and Li, N 2011 Brief social isolation in early adolescence affects reversal learning and forebrain BDNF expression in adult rats. Brain Research Bulletin 86: 173178. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainresbull.2011.07.008CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harati, H, Barbelivien, A, Herbeaux, K, Muller, MA, Engeln, M, Kelche, C, Cassel, JC and Majchrzak, M 2013 Lifelong environmental enrichment in rats: Impact on emotional behavior, spatial memory vividness, and cholinergic neurons over the lifespan. Age 35: 10271043. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11357-012-9424-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harlow, HF 1958 The nature of love. American Psychologist 13: 673685.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hattori, S, Hashimoto, R, Miyakawa, T, Yamanaka, H, Maeno, H, Wada, K and Kunugi, H 2007 Enriched environments influence depression-related behavior in adult mice and the survival of newborn cells in their hippocampi. Behavioural Brain Research 180: 6976. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2007.02.036CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henshall, C, Davey, Z and Jackson, D 2020 The implementation and evaluation of a resilience enhancement programme for nurses working in the forensic setting. International Journal of Mental Health Nursing 29: 508520. https://doi.org/10.1111/inm.12689CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hobfoll, SE 2002 Social and psychological resources and adaptation. Review of General Psychology 6: 307324. https://doi.org/10.1037/1089-2680.6.4.307CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoffman, AN, Krigbaum, A, Ortiz, JB, Mika, A, Hutchinson, KM, Bimonte-Nelson, HA and Conrad, CD 2011 Recovery after chronic stress within spatial reference and working memory domains: Correspondence with hippocampal morphology. European Journal of Neuroscience 34: 10231030. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-9568.2011.07820.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hong, S, Flashner, B, Chiu, M, ver Hoeve, E, Luz, S and Bhatnagar, S 2012 Social isolation in adolescence alters behaviors in the forced swim and sucrose preference tests in female but not in male rats. Physiology and Behavior 105: 269275. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2011.08.036CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hosey, GR 2000 Zoo animals and their human audiences: What is the visitor effect? Animal Welfare 9: 343357. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0962728600022946CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hourani, L, Tueller, S, Kizakevich, P, Lewis, G, Strange, L, Weimer, B, Bryant, S, Bishop, E, Hubal, R and Spira, J 2016 Toward preventing post-traumatic stress disorder: Development and testing of a pilot predeployment stress inoculation training program. Military Medicine 181: 11511160. https://doi.org/10.7205/MILMED-D-15-00192CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hulshof, HJ, Novati, A, Sgoifo, A, Luiten, PGM, Den Boer, JA and Meerlo, P 2011 Maternal separation decreases adult hippocampal cell proliferation and impairs cognitive performance but has little effect on stress sensitivity and anxiety in adult Wistar rats. Behavioural Brain Research 216: 552560. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2010.08.038CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hurley, J, Barrett, P and Reet, P 2006 ‘Let a hundred flowers blossom, let a hundred schools of thought contend’: a case for therapeutic pluralism in mental health nursing. Journal of Psychiatric and Mental Health Nursing 13: 173179. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2850.2006.00938.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ilin, Y and Richter-Levin, G 2009 Enriched environment experience overcomes learning deficits and depressive-like behavior induced by juvenile stress. PLoS ONE 4. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0004329CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jacobson-Pick, S, Audet, MC, Nathoo, N and Anisman, H 2011 Stressor experiences during the juvenile period increase stressor responsivity in adulthood: Transmission of stressor experiences. Behavioural Brain Research 216: 365374. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2010.08.016CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jaeggi, AV, Dunkel, LP, van Noordwijk, MA, Wich, SA, Sura, AAL and van Schaik, CP 2010 Social learning of diet and foraging skills by wild immature Bornean orangutans: Implications for culture. American Journal of Primatology 72: 6271. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.20752CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Janoff-Bulman, R and Brickman, P 1982 Expectations and what people learn from failure. In: Feather, NT (ed) Expectations and Actions: Expectancy-Value Models in Psychology pp 207237. Routledge: London, UK.Google Scholar
Jing, XY, Wang, Y, Zou, HW, Li, ZL, Liu, YJ and Li, LF 2021 mGlu2/3 receptor in the prelimbic cortex is implicated in stress resilience and vulnerability in mice. European Journal of Pharmacology 906: 174231. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ejphar.2021.174231CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Joyce, S, Hons, BA, Shand, F, Bryant, RA, Lal, TJ and Harvey, SB 2018 Mindfulness-based resilience training in the workplace: Pilot study of the internet-based Resilience@Work (RAW) mindfulness program. Journal of Medical Internet Research 20: e10326. https://doi.org/10.2196/10326CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kalinichev, M, Easterling, KW, Plotsky, PM and Holtzman, SG 2002 Long-lasting changes in stress-induced corticosterone response and anxiety-like behaviors as a consequence of neonatal maternal separation in Long-Evans rats. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior 73: 131140. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0091-3057(02)00781-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Katz, RJ, Roth, KA and Carroll, BJ 1981 Acute and chronic stress effects on open field activity in the rat: Implications for a model of depression. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 5: 247251. https://doi.org/10.1016/0149-7634(81)90005-1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kent, M, Kovalev, D, Hart, B, Leserve, D, Handford, G, Vavra, D and Lambert, K 2022 The emotional impact of disrupted environmental contexts: Enrichment loss and coping profiles influence stress response recovery in Long–Evans rats. Journal of Neuroendocrinology 34: e13179. https://doi.org/10.1111/jne.13179CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
King, JE and Landau, VI 2003 Can chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) happiness be estimated by human raters? Journal of Research in Personality 37: 115. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0092-6566(02)00527-5CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Knott, CD 1998 Changes in orangutan caloric intake, energy balance, and ketones in response to fluctuating fruit availability. International Journal of Primatology 19: 10611079. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1020330404983CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kochi, C, Liu, H, Zaidi, S, Atrooz, F, Dantoin, P and Salim, S 2017 Prior treatmill exercise promotes resilience to vicarious trauma in rats. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry 77: 216221. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pnpbp.2017.04.018CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kozina, A 2020 School-based prevention of anxiety using the ‘My FRIENDS’ emotional resilience program: Six-month follow-up. International Journal of Psychology 55: 7077. https://doi.org/10.1002/ijop.12553CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krause, ET, Honarmand, M, Wetzel, J and Naguib, M 2009 Early fasting is long lasting: Differences in early nutritional conditions reappear under stressful conditions in adult female zebra finches. PLoS ONE 4: e5015. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0005015CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kriengwatana, B, Farrell, TM, Aitken, SDT, Garcia, L and MacDougall-Shackleton, SA 2015 Early-life nutritional stress affects associative learning and spatial memory but not performance on a novel object test. Behaviour 152: 195218. https://doi.org/10.1163/1568539X-00003239CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kubala, KH, Christianson, JP, Kaufman, RD, Watkins, LR and Maier, SF 2012 Short- and long-term consequences of stressor controllability in adolescent rats. Behavioural Brain Research 234: 278284. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2012.06.027CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kunz, JA, Duvot, GJ, van Noordwijk, MA, Willems, EP, Townsend, M, Mardianah, N, Utami Atmoko, SS, Vogel, ER, Nugraha, TP, Heistermann, M, Agil, M and Weingrill, T 2021 The cost of associating with males for Bornean and Sumatran female orangutans: a hidden form of sexual conflict? Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 75: 6. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-020-02948-4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lalanza, JF, Sanchez-Roige, S, Cigarroa, I, Gagliano, H, Fuentes, S, Armario, A, Capdevila, L and Escorihuela, RM 2015 Long-term moderate treadmill exercise promotes stress-coping strategies in male and female rats. Scientific Reports 5: 16166. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep16166CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lambert, K, Hyer, M, Bardi, M, Rzucidlo, A, Scott, S, Terhune-Cotter, B, Hazelgrove, A, Silva, I and Kinsley, C 2016 Natural-enriched environments lead to enhanced environmental engagement and altered neurobiological resilience. Neuroscience 330: 386394. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2016.05.037CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lambert, KG, Hyer, MM, Rzucidlo, AA, Bergeron, T, Landis, T and Bardi, M 2014 Contingency-based emotional resilience: Effort-based reward training and flexible coping lead to adaptive responses to uncertainty in male rats. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience 8: 124. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00124CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Law, A, Logan, H and Baron, RS 1994 Desire for control, felt control, and stress inoculation training during dental treatment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 67: 926936. https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.67.5.926CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leasure, JL and Jones, M 2008 Forced and voluntary exercise differentially affect brain and behavior. Neuroscience 156: 456465. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2008.07.041CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Le Dorze, C and Gisquet-Verrier, P 2016 Effects of multiple brief exposures to trauma-associated cues on traumatized resilient and vulnerable rats. Brain Research 1652: 7180. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2016.10.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, CW, Fang, YP, Chu, MC, Chung, YJ, Chi, H, Tang, CW, So, EC and Lin, HC 2021 Differential mechanisms of synaptic plasticity for susceptibility and resilience to chronic social defeat stress in male mice. Biochemical and Biophysical Research Communications 562: 112118. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbrc.2021.05.064CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, JH, Kim, HJ, Kim, JG, Ryu, V, Kim, BT, Kang, DW and Jahng, JW 2007 Depressive behaviors and decreased expression of serotonin reuptake transporter in rats that experienced neonatal maternal separation. Neuroscience Research 58: 3239. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neures.2007.01.008CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leggio, MG, Mandolesi, L, Federico, F, Spirito, F, Ricci, B, Gelfo, F and Petrosini, L 2005 Environmental enrichment promotes improved spatial abilities and enhanced dendritic growth in the rat. Behavioural Brain Research 163: 7890. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2005.04.009CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leussis, MP, Freund, N, Brenhouse, HC, Thompson, BS and Andersen, SL 2012 Depressive-like behavior in adolescents after maternal separation: Sex differences, controllability, and GABA. Developmental Neuroscience 34: 210217. https://doi.org/10.1159/000339162CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Li, B, Zhang, H, Cao, Y, Li, Z, Xu, X, Song, Z and hui, Wang J 2022 Molecular changes in nucleus accumbens due to amelioration of depressive-like behavior by housing with companion. Brain Research Bulletin 189: 3443. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainresbull.2022.08.010CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Li, ZL, Wang, Y, Zou, HW, Jing, XY, Liu, YJ and Li, LF 2021 GABA(B) receptors within the lateral habenula modulate stress resilience and vulnerability in mice. Physiology and Behavior 230: 113311. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2021.113311CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liu, H, Atrooz, F, Salvi, A and Salim, S 2017 Behavioral and cognitive impact of early life stress: insights from an animal model. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry 78: 8895. https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.20449.ChallengesCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lo, L, Iasiello, M, Carey, M and van Agteren, J 2020 Improving the wellbeing of female prisoners via psychological skills training: A feasability study. International Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative Criminology 64: 15711586. https://doi.org/10.1177/0306624X20928029CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lu, J, Gong, X, Yao, X, Guang, Y, Yang, H, Ji, R, He, Y, Zhou, W, Wang, H, Wang, W, Bai, S, Guo, H, Guo, ZV and Xie, P 2021 Prolonged chronic social defeat stress promotes less resilience and higher uniformity in depression-like behaviors in adult male mice. Biochemical and Biophysical Research Communications 553: 107113. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbrc.2021.03.058CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lucas, M, Ilin, Y, Anunu, R, Kehat, O, Xu, L, Desmedt, A and Richter-Levin, G 2014 Long-term effects of controllability or the lack of it on coping abilities and stress resilience in the rat. Stress 17: 423430. https://doi.org/10.3109/10253890.2014.930430CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luine, V 2002 Sex differences in chronic stress effects on memory in rats. Stress 5: 205216. https://doi.org/10.1080/1025389021000010549CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luine, V, Martinez, C, Villegas, M, Magariños, AM and McEwen, BS 1996 Restraint stress reversibly enhances spatial memory performance. Physiology and Behavior 59: 2732. https://doi.org/10.1016/0031-9384(95)02016-0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luine, V, Villegas, M, Martinez, C and McEwen, BS 1994 Repeated stress causes reversible impairments of spatial memory performance. Brain Research 639: 167170. https://doi.org/10.1016/0006-8993(94)91778-7CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luo, L, van der Zande, LE, Marwijk, MA, Knol, EF, Rodenburg, TB, Bolhuis, JE and Parois, SP 2022 Impact of enrichment and repeated mixing on resilience in pigs. Frontiers in Veterinary Science 9: 829060. https://doi.org/10.3389/fvets.2022.829060CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lyons, DM, Buckmaster, CL and Schatzberg, AF 2018 Learning to actively cope with stress in female mice. Psychoneuroendocrinology 96: 7883. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2018.06.010CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lyons, DM and Parker, KJ 2007 Stress inoculation-induced indications of resilience in monkeys. Journal of Traumatic Stress 20: 423433. https://doi.org/10.1002/jtsCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lyte, JM, Koester, LR, Daniels, KM and Lyte, M 2022 Distinct cecal and fecal microbiome responses to stress are accompanied by sex- and diet-dependent changes in behavior and gut serotonin. Frontiers in Neuroscience 16: 827343. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2022.827343CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Machado, TD, Dalle Molle, R, Laureano, DP, Portella, AK, Werlang, ICR, Benetti, CDS, Noschang, C and Silveira, PP 2013 Early life stress is associated with anxiety, increased stress responsivity and preference for ‘comfort foods’ in adult female rats. Stress 16: 549556. https://doi.org/10.3109/10253890.2013.816841CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maier, SF 2001 Exposure to the stressor environment prevents the temporal dissipation of behavioral depression/learned helplessness. Biological Psychiatry 49: 763773. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0006-3223(00)01095-7CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mällo, T, Matrov, D, Kõiv, K and Harro, J 2009 Effect of chronic stress on behavior and cerebral oxidative metabolism in rats with high or low positive affect. Neuroscience 164: 963974. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2009.08.041CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Malone, N, Fuentes, A and White, FJ 2012 Variation in the social systems of extant hominoids: Comparative insight into the social behavior of early hominins. International Journal of Primatology 33: 12511277. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10764-012-9617-0CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mandleco, BL and Peery, JC 2000 An organizational framework for conceptualising resilience in children. Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Nursing 13: 99111. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1744-6171.2000.tb00086.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marais, L, van Rensburg, SJ, van Zyl, JM, Stein, DJ and Daniels, WMU 2008 Maternal separation of rat pups increases the risk of developing depressive-like behavior after subsequent chronic stress by altering corticosterone and neurotrophin levels in the hippocampus. Neuroscience Research 61: 106112. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neures.2008.01.011CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marco, EM, Valero, M, De La Serna, O, Aisa, B, Borcel, E, Ramirez, MJ and Viveros, MP 2013 Maternal deprivation effects on brain plasticity and recognition memory in adolescent male and female rats. Neuropharmacology 68: 223231. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropharm.2012.08.014CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martland, R, Korman, N, Firth, J, Vancampfort, D, Thompson, T and Stubbs, B 2022 Can high-intensity interval training improve mental health outcomes in the general population and those with physical illnesses? A systematic review and meta-analysis of 53 randomized controlled trials. British Journal of Sports Medicine 56: 279291. https://doi.org/10.1136/bjsports-2021-103984CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Masten, AS, Best, KM and Garmezy, N 1990 Resilience and development: Contributions from the study of children who overcome adversity. Development and Psychopathology 2: 425444. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579400005812CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCormick, CM, Smith, C and Mathews, IZ 2008 Effects of chronic social stress in adolescence on anxiety and neuroendocrine response to mild stress in male and female rats. Behavioural Brain Research 187: 228238. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2007.09.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McIntosh, J, Anisman, H and Merali, Z 1999 Short- and long-periods of neonatal maternal separation differentially affect anxiety and feeding in adult rats: Gender-dependent effects. Developmental Brain Research 113: 97106. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0165-3806(99)00005-XCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meichenbaum, D 1985 Stress Inoculation Training. Pergamon Press: Elmsford, New York, NY, USA.Google Scholar
Meichenbaum, D and Cameron, R 1983 Stress inoculation training: Toward a general paradigm for training coping skills. In: Meichenbaum, D and Jaremko, M (Eds.) Stress Reduction and Prevention pp 115154. Plenum: New York, NY, USA.Google Scholar
Meijaard, E, Buchori, D, Hadiprakarsa, Y, Utami-Atmoko, SS, Nurcahyo, A, Tjiu, A, Prasetyo, D, Nardiyono, Christie L, Ancrenaz, M, Abadi, F, Antoni, ING, Armayadi, D, Dinato, A, Ella, , Gumelar, P, Indrawan, TP, Kussaritano, Munajat C, Priyono, CWP, Purwanto, Y, Puspitasari, D, Putra, MSW, Rahmat, A, Ramadani, H, Sammy, J, Siswanto, D, Syamsuri, M, Andayani, N, Wu, H, Wells, JA and Mengersen, K 2011 Quantifying killing of orangutans and human-orangutan conflict in Kalimantan, Indonesia. PLoS ONE 6: e27491. https://doi.org/10.1371/JOURNAL.PONE.0027491CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mineur, YS, Belzung, C and Crusio, WE 2007 Functional implications of decreases in neurogenesis following chronic mild stress in mice. Neuroscience 150: 251259. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2007.09.045CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mitra Setia, T and van Schaik, CP 2007 The response of adult orang-utans to flanged male long calls: Inferences about their function. Folia Primatologica 78: 215226. https://doi.org/10.1159/000102317CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morgan, KN and Tromborg, CT 2007 Sources of stress in captivity. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 102: 262302. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.APPLANIM.2006.05.032CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mourlon, V, Baudin, A, Blanc, O, Lauber, A, Giros, B, Naudon, L and Daugé, V 2010 Maternal deprivation induces depressive-like behaviours only in female rats. Behavioural Brain Research 213: 278287. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2010.05.017CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mrdalj, J, Murison, R, Soulé, J, Kinn Rød, AM, Milde, AM, Pallesen, S and Grønli, J 2016 Mild daily stressors in adulthood may counteract behavioural effects after constant presence of mother during early life. Physiology and Behavior 165: 313321. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2016.08.018CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muhammad, A and Kolb, B 2011 Maternal separation altered behavior and neuronal spine density without influencing amphetamine sensitization. Behavioural Brain Research 223: 716. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2011.04.015CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mul, JD, Soto, M, Cahill, ME, Ryan, RE, Takahashi, H, So, K, Zheng, J, Croote, DE, Hirshman, MF, La Fleur, SE, Nestler, EJ and Goodyear, LJ 2018 Voluntary wheel running promotes resilience to chronic social defeat stress in mice: A role for nucleus accumbens ΔfosB. Neuropsychopharmacology 43: 19341942. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41386-018-0103-zCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nahvi, RJ, Tanelian, A, Nwokafor, C, Godino, A, Parise, E, Estill, M, Shen, L, Nestler, EJ and Sabban, EL 2023 Transcriptome profiles associated with resilience and susceptibility to single prolonged stress in the locus coeruleus and nucleus accumbens in male sprague-dawley rats. Behavioural Brain Research 439: 114162. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2022.114162CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Navaee, M and Kaykha, H 2019 The effects of stress inoculation training on coping strategies among midwives in primary health-care centers. Nursing and Midwifery Studies 8: 176182. https://doi.org/10.4103/nms.nms_71_18Google Scholar
National Child Traumatic Stress Network (NCTSN) 2023 About child trauma. https://www.nctsn.org/what-is-child-trauma/about-child-traumaGoogle Scholar
Nijman, V 2005 In Full Swing: An Assessment of Trade in Orang-utans and Gibbons on Java and Bali, Indonesia. TRAFFIC Southeast Asia: Selangor, Indonesia.Google Scholar
Novak, MA and Harlow, HF 1975 Social recovery of monkeys isolated for the first year of life: I. Rehabilitation and therapy. Developmental Psychology 11: 453465. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0076661Google Scholar
Núñez, JF, Ferré, P, Escorihuela, RM, Tobeña, A and Fernández-Teruel, A 1996 Effects of postnatal handling of rats on emotional, HPA-Axis, and prolactin reactivity to novelty and conflict. Physiology and Behavior 60: 13551359. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0031-9384(96)00225-9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Núñez, JF, Ferré, P, García, E, Escorihuela, RM, Fernández-Teruel, A and Tobeña, A 1995 Postnatal handling reduces emotionality ratings and accelerates two-way active avoidance in female rats. Physiology and Behavior 57: 831835. https://doi.org/10.1016/0031-9384(94)00308-RCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ogawa, T, Mikuni, M, Kuroda, Y, Muneoka, K, Mori, KJ and Takahashi, K 1994 Periodic maternal deprivation alters stress response in adult offspring: Potentiates the negative feedback regulation of restraint stress-induced adrenocortical response and reduces the frequencies of open field-induced behaviors. Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior 49: 961967. https://doi.org/10.1016/0091-3057(94)90250-XCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Overmier, JB and Seligman, ME 1967 Effects of inescapable shock upon subsequent escape and avoidance responding. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 63: 2833. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0024166CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Palmer, A 2020 Wild, well, or free? Ethical debates in rehabilitation methods. Ethical Debates in Orangutan Conservation. Routledge: Oxford, UK.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Parihar, V, Hattiangady, B, Kuruba, R, Shuai, B and Shetty, A 2011 Predictable chronic mild stress improves mood, hippocampal neurogenesis and memory. Physiology and Behavior 16: 171183. https://doi.org/10.1038/mp.2009.130Google ScholarPubMed
Parker, KJ, Buckmaster, CL, Hyde, SA, Schatzberg, AF and Lyons, DM 2019 Nonlinear relationship between early life stress exposure and subsequent resilience in monkeys. Scientific Reports 9: 18. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-52810-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Parker, KJ, Buckmaster, CL, Justus, KR, Schatzberg, AF and Lyons, DM 2005 Mild early life stress enhances prefrontal-dependent response inhibition in monkeys. Biological Psychiatry 57: 848855. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.12.024CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Parker, KJ, Buckmaster, CL, Lindley, SE, Schatzberg, AF and Lyons, DM 2012 Hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis physiology and cognitive control of behavior in stress inoculated monkeys. International Journal of Behavioral Development 36: 543562. https://doi.org/10.1177/0165025411406864.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Parker, KJ, Buckmaster, CL, Schatzberg, AF and Lyons, DM 2004 Prospective investigation of stress inoculation in young monkeys. Archives of General Psychiatry 61: 933941. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.61.9.933CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Parker, KJ, Rainwater, KL, Buckmaster, CL, Schatzberg, AF, Lindley, SE and Lyons, DM 2007 Early life stress and novelty seeking behavior in adolescent monkeys. Psychoneuroendocrinology 32: 785792. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2007.05.008CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Patki, G, Solanki, N and Salim, S 2014 Witnessing traumatic events causes severe behavioral impairments in rats. International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology 17: 20172029. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1461145714000923CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peleg-Raibstein, D and Feldon, J 2011 Differential effects of post-weaning juvenile stress on the behaviour of C57BL/6 mice in adolescence and adulthood. Psychopharmacology 214: 339351. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-010-1991-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pelsker, R and Mayer, V 2008 Nonhuman primates mask signs of pain. Laboratory Primate Newsletter 47: 13.Google Scholar
Peng, L, Li, M, Zuo, X, Miao, Y, Chen, L, Yu, Y, Liu, B and Wang, T 2014 Application of the Pennsylvania resilience training program on medical students. Personality and Individual Differences 61–62: 4751. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.01.006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Penke, Z, Felszeghy, K, Fernette, B, Sage, D, Nyakas, C and Burlet, A 2001 Postnatal maternal deprivation produces long-lasting modifications of the stress response, feeding and stress-related behaviour in the rat. European Journal of Neuroscience 14: 747755. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.0953-816X.2001.01691.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Perrine, SA, Eagle, AL, George, SA, Mulo, K, Kohler, RJ, Gerard, J, Harutyunyan, A, Hool, SM, Susick, LL, Schneider, BL, Ghoddoussi, F, Galloway, MP, Liberzon, I and Conti, AC 2016 Severe, multimodal stress exposure induces PTSD-like characteristics in a mouse model of single prolonged stress. Behavioural Brain Research 303: 228237. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2016.01.056CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pietrelli, A, Di Nardo, M, Masucci, A, Brusco, A, Basso, N and Matkovic, L 2018 Lifelong aerobic exercise reduces the stress response in rats. Neuroscience 376: 94107. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2018.02.019CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pluess, M, Boniwell, I, Hefferon, K and Tunariu, A 2017 Preliminary evaluation of a school-based resilience-promoting intervention in a high- risk population: Application of an exploratory two-cohort treatment/control design. PLoS ONE 12: 118. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal. pone.0177191CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pohl, J, Olmstead, MC, Wynne-Edwards, KE, Harkness, K and Menard, JL 2007 Repeated exposure to stress across the childhood-adolescent period alters rats’ anxiety- and depression-like behaviors in adulthood: The importance of stressor type and gender. Behavioral Neuroscience 121: 462474. https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7044.121.3.462CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Premack, D and Woodruff, G 1978 Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behavioral and Brain Sciences 1: 515526. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00076512CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Preuschoft, S, Yassir, I, Putri, AI, Aoliya, N, Yuliani, E, Badriyah, SN, Corbi, P, Sugianto, Y, Sitepu, BS and Kalcher‐Sommersguter, E 2021 Learning to be an orangutan - implications of life history for orangutan rehabilitation. Animals 11: 123. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani11030767CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Prince, CR and Anisman, H 1984 Acute and chronic stress effects on performance in a forced-swim task. Behavioral and Neural Biology 42: 99119. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0163-1047(84)90942-7CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pryce, CR, Dettling, A, Spengler, M, Spaete, C and Feldon, J 2004 Evidence for altered monoamine activity and emotional and cognitive disturbance in marmoset monkeys exposed to early life stress. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1032: 245249. https://doi.org/10.1196/annals.1314.030CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Puppe, B, Ernst, K, Schön, PC and Manteuffel, G 2007 Cognitive enrichment affects behavioural reactivity in domestic pigs. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 105: 7586. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2006.05.016CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reimers, M, Schwarzenberger, F and Preuschoft, S 2007 Rehabilitation of research chimpanzees: Stress and coping after long-term isolation. Hormones and Behavior 51: 428435. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2006.12.011CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Remus, JL, Jamison, D and Johnson, JD 2013 An animal model of recurrent depression: Sensitized depression-like behavior when rats are re-exposed to chronic mild stress. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity 32: e4e5. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbi.2013.07.026CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robinson, S, Christ, CC, Cahill, MM, Aldrich, SJ and Taylor-Yeremeeva, E 2019 Voluntary exercise or systemic propranolol ameliorates stress-related maladaptive behaviors in female rats. Physiology and Behavior 198: 120133. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2018.10.012CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Röder, EL, Timmermans, PJAA and Vossen, JMHH 1989 Effects of rearing and exposure condition upon the acquisition of phobic behaviour in cynomolgus monkeys. Behaviour Research and Therapy 27: 221231. https://doi.org/10.1016/0005-7967(89)90040-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roelofs, S, Boleij, H, Nordquist, RE and van der Staay, FJ 2016 Making decisions under ambiguity: Judgment bias tasks for assessing emotional state in animals. Frontiers in Behavioural Neuroscience 10. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2016.00119CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rogerson, S, Meir, R, Crowley-McHattan, Z, Mcewen, K and Pastoors, R 2016 A randomized controlled pilot trial investigating the impact of a workplace resilience program during a time of significant organizational change. Journal of Occupational and Environmental Medicine 58: 329334. https://doi.org/10.1097/JOM.0000000000000677CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Romeo, RD, Mueller, A, Sisti, HM, Ogawa, S, McEwen, BS and Brake, WG 2003 Anxiety and fear behaviors in adult male and female C57BL/6 mice are modulated by maternal separation. Hormones and Behavior 43: 561567. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0018-506X(03)00063-1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roth, TS, Rianti, P, Fredriksson, GM, Wich, SA and Nowak, MG 2020 Grouping behavior of Sumatran orangutans (Pongo abelii) and Tapanuli orangutans (Pongo tapanuliensis) living in forest with low fruit abundance. American Journal of Primatology 82: e23123. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.23123CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Russon, AE 2008 Orangutan rehabilitation and reintroduction: Successes, failures, and role in conservation. In: Wich, SA, Utami Atmoko, SS, Mitra Setia, T, and van Schaik, CP (Eds.) Orangutans: Geographic Variation in Behavioral Ecology and Conservation pp 327350. Oxford University Press: Oxford, UKCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rygula, R, Golebiowska, J, Kregiel, J, Kubik, J and Popik, P 2015 Effects of optimism on motivation in rats. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience 9: 19. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00032CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sackett, GP 1972 Exploratory behavior of rhesus monkeys as a function of rearing experiences and sex. Developmental Psychology 6: 260270. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0032081CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sampedro-Piquero, P, Castilla-Ortega, E, Zancada-Menendez, C, Santín, LJ and Begega, A 2016 Environmental enrichment as a therapeutic avenue for anxiety in aged Wistar rats: Effect on cat odor exposition and GABAergic interneurons. Neuroscience 330: 1725. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2016.05.032CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Santarelli, S, Zimmermann, C, Kalideris, G, Lesuis, SL, Arloth, J, Uribe, A, Dournes, C, Balsevich, G, Hartmann, J, Masana, M, Binder, EB, Spengler, D and Schmidt, M V. 2017 An adverse early life environment can enhance stress resilience in adulthood. Psychoneuroendocrinology 78: 213221. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2017.01.021CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmidt, DA 2004 Nutrition. Orangutan Husbandry Manual. Orangutan Species Survival Plan. https://www.orangutanssp.org/Google Scholar
Schrijver, NCA, Bahr, NI, Weiss, IC and Würbel, H 2002 Dissociable effects of isolation rearing and environmental enrichment on exploration, spatial learning and HPA activity in adult rats. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior 73: 209224. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0091-3057(02)00790-6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schuppli, C, Forss, S, Meulman, E, Atmoko, SU and van Noordwijk, M 2017 The effects of sociability on exploratory tendency and innovation repertoires in wild Sumatran and Bornean orangutans. Scientific Reports 7: 15464. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-15640-xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Seery, MD, Holman, EA and Silver, RC 2010 Whatever does not kill us: Cumulative lifetime adversity, vulnerability, and resilience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 99: 10251041. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021344CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seery, MD, Leo, RJ, Lupien, SP, Kondrak, CL and Almonte, JL 2013 An upside to adversity? Moderate cumulative lifetime adversity is associated with resilient responses in the face of controlled stressors. Psychological Science 24: 11811189. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797612469210CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Seligman, ME, Maier, SF and Geer, JH 1968 Alleviation of learned helpless in the dog. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 73: 256262. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0025831CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Seligman, ME, Rosellini, RA and Kozak, MJ 1975 Learned helplessness in the rat: Time course, immunization, and reversibility. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 88: 542547. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0076431CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shapiro, KJ 1998 Animal Models of Human Psychology: Critique of Science, Ethics, and Policy. Hogrefe & Huber: Seattle, WA, USA.Google Scholar
Sherman, J, Ancrenaz, M and Meijaard, E 2020 Shifting apes: Conservation and welfare outcomes of Bornean orangutan rescue and release in Kalimantan, Indonesia. Journal for Nature Conservation 55: 125807. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.JNC.2020.125807CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shi, DD, Zhang, YD, Ren, YY, Peng, SY, Yuan, TF and Wang, Z 2021 Predictable maternal separation confers adult stress resilience via the medial prefrontal cortex oxytocin signaling pathway in rats. Molecular Psychiatry 26: 72967307. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41380-021-01293-wCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shi, P, Hu, L, Ren, H and Dai, Q 2023 Reward enhances resilience to chronic social defeat stress in mice: Neural ECs and mGluR5 mechanism via neuroprotection in VTA and DRN. Frontiers in Psychiatry 14: 1084367. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2023.1084367CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shu, C, Xiao, L, Tang, J, Wang, G, Zhang, X and Wang, X 2015 Blunted behavioral and molecular responses to chronic mild stress in adult rats with experience of infancy maternal separation. Tohoku Journal of Experimental Medicine 235: 1887. https://doi.org/10.1620/tjem.235.81CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sih, A and Del Giudice, M 2012 Linking behavioural syndromes and cognition: A behavioural ecology perspective. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 367: 27622772. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2012.0216CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Singleton, I, Wich, SA, Nowak, M, Usher, G and Utami-Atmoko, SS 2018 Pongo abelii, Sumatran Orangutan. The IUCN Red List of Threatened Species. IUCN: Cambridge, UK.Google Scholar
Siviy, SM and Harrison, KA 2008 Effects of neonatal handling on play behavior and fear towards a predator odor in juvenile rats (Rattus norvegicus). Journal of Comparative Psychology 122: 18. https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7036.122.1.1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Skynner, LA, Amory, JR and Hosey, G 2004 The effect of visitors on the self-injurious behaviour of a male pileated gibbon (Hylobates pileatus). Zoologische Garten 74: 3841.Google Scholar
Smith, B, Shatté, A, Perlman, A, Siers, M and Lynch, WD 2018 Improvements in resilience, stress, and somatic symptoms following online resilience training: A dose-response effect. Journal of Occupational and Environmental Medicine 60: 15. https://doi.org/10.1097/JOM.0000000000001142CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, TE, McGreer-Whitworth, B and French, JA 1998 Close proximity of the heterosexual partner reduces the physiological and behavioral consequences of novel-cage housing in black tufted-ear marmosets (Callithrix kuhli). Hormones and Behavior 34: 211222. https://doi.org/10.1006/hbeh.1998.1469CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stamatakis, A, Pondiki, S, Kitraki, E, Diamantopoulou, A, Panagiotaropoulos, T, Raftogianni, A and Stylianopoulou, F 2008 Effect of neonatal handling on adult rat spatial learning and memory following acute stress. Stress 11: 148159. https://doi.org/10.1080/10253890701653039CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Steinhardt, M and Doblier, C 2010 Evaluation of a resilience intervention to enhance coping strategies and protective factors and decrease symptomatology. Journal of American College Health 56: 445453. https://doi.org/10.3200/JACH.56.44.445-454CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sterlemann, V, Ganea, K, Liebl, C, Harbich, D, Alam, S, Holsboer, F, Müller, MB and Schmidt, MV 2008 Long-term behavioral and neuroendocrine alterations following chronic social stress in mice: Implications for stress-related disorders. Hormones and Behavior 53: 386394. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2007.11.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strekalova, T, Pavlov, D, Trofimov, A, Anthony, DC, Svistunov, A, Proshin, A, Umriukhin, A, Lyundup, A, Lesch, KP and Cespuglio, R 2022 Hippocampal over-expression of cyclooxygenase-2 (COX-2) is associated with susceptibility to stress-induced anhedonia in mice. International Journal of Molecular Sciences 23: 2061. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijms23042061CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strzelewicz, AR, Ordoñes Sanchez, E, Rondón-Ortiz, AN, Raneri, A, Famularo, ST, Bangasser, DA and Kentner, AC 2019 Access to a high resource environment protects against accelerated maturation following early life stress: A translational animal model of high, medium and low security settings. Hormones and Behavior 111: 4659. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2019.01.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sun, XM, Tu, WQ, Shi, YW, Xue, L and Zhao, H 2014 Female-dependent impaired fear memory of adult rats induced by maternal separation, and screening of possible related genes in the hippocampal CA1. Behavioural Brain Research 267: 111118. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2014.03.022CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Suo, L, Zhao, L, Si, J, Liu, J, Zhu, W, Chai, B, Zhang, Y, Feng, J, Ding, Z, Luo, Y, Shi, H, Shi, J and Lu, L 2013 Predictable chronic mild stress in adolescence increases resilience in adulthood. Neuropsychopharmacology 38: 13871400. https://doi.org/10.1038/npp.2013.67CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Suomi, SJ and Harlow, HF 1972 Social rehabilitation of isolate-reared monkeys. Developmental Psychology 6: 487496. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0032545CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Suomi, SJ, Harlow, HF and Domek, CJ 1970 Effect of repetitive infant-infant separation of young monkeys. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 76: 161172. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0029809CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swiergiel, AH, Zhou, Y and Dunn, AJ 2007 Effects of chronic footshock, restraint and corticotropin-releasing factor on freezing, ultrasonic vocalization and forced swim behavior in rats. Behavioural Brain Research 183: 178187. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2007.06.006CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Szabo, Z and Marian, M 2012 Stress inoculation training in adolescents: Classroom intervention benefits. Journal of Cognitive and Behavioral Psychotherapies 12: 175188.Google Scholar
Tanner, MK, Fallon, IP, Baratta, MV and Greenwood, BN 2019 Voluntary exercise enables stress resistance in females. Behavioural Brain Research 369: 111923. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2019.111923.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tecwyn, EC 2013 Physical Cognition in Great Apes: Planning & Object Compliance. University of Birmingham: UK.Google Scholar
Thorpe, SKS, Crompton, RH and Alexander, RMN 2007 Orangutans use compliant branches to lower the energetic cost of locomotion. Biology Letters 3: 253256. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2007.0049CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thorpe, SKS, Neufuss, J, Myatt, J, Tarrega, E, Wamba, G, Sulistyo, F, Benítez López, A and Chappell, J 2022 The EDT: An evidence‐based framework for improving captive great ape well‐being. In: Unwin, S, White, A, and Landiouw, A (Eds.) State of the Apes Volume V: Health and Disease at the Human‐Ape Interface. ARCUS Foundation: Kalamazoo, MI, USA.Google Scholar
Toledo-Rodriguez, M and Sandi, C 2007 Stress before puberty exerts a sex- and age-related impact on auditory and contextual fear conditioning in the rat. Neural Plasticity 2007: 71203. https://doi.org/10.1155/2007/71203CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Torrisi, SA, Lavanco, G, Maurel, OM, Gulisano, W, Laudani, S, Geraci, F, Grasso, M, Barbagallo, C, Caraci, F, Bucolo, C, Ragusa, M, Papaleo, F, Campolongo, P, Puzzo, D, Drago, F, Salomone, S and Leggio, GM 2021 A novel arousal-based individual screening reveals susceptibility and resilience to PTSD-like phenotypes in mice. Neurobiology of Stress 14: 100286. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ynstr.2020.100286CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Toth, E, Gersner, R, Wilf-Yarkoni, A, Raizel, H, Dar, DE, Richter-Levin, G, Levit, O and Zangen, A 2008 Age-dependent effects of chronic stress on brain plasticity and depressive behavior. Journal of Neurochemistry 107: 522532. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1471-4159.2008.05642.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tsoory, M, Cohen, H and Richter-Levin, G 2007 Juvenile stress induces a predisposition to either anxiety or depressive-like symptoms following stress in adulthood. European Neuropsychopharmacology 17: 245256. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.euroneuro.2006.06.007CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tsoory, M and Richter-Levin, G 2006 Learning under stress in the adult rat is differentially affected by ‘juvenile’ or ‘adolescent’ stress. International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology 9: 713728. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1461145705006255CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Utami-Atmoko, S, Traylor-Holzer, K, Rifqi, MA, Siregar, PG, Achmad, B, Priadjati, A, Husson, SSW, Hadisiswoyo, P, Saputra, F, Campbell-Smith, G, Kuncoro, P, Russon, A, Voigt, M, Santika, T, Nowak, M, Singleton, I, Sapari, I, Meididit, A, Chandradewi, DS, Ripoll Capilla, B, Ermayanti, A and Lees, CM 2017 Orangutan population and habitat viability assessment: Final Report. IUCN/SSC Conservation Breeding Specialist Group: Apple Valley, MN, USA.Google Scholar
Uysal, N, Ozdemir, D, Yalaz, G and Bediz, CS 2005 Effects of maternal deprivation on melatonin production and cognition in adolescent male and female rats. Neuroendocrinology Letters 26: 555560.Google ScholarPubMed
van Adrichem, GGJ, Utami, SS, Wich, SA, van Hooff, JARAM and Sterck, EHM 2006 The development of wild immature Sumatran orangutans (Pongo abelii) at Ketambe. Primates 47: 300309. https://doi.org/10.1007/S10329-006-0193-9/FIGURES/6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Agteren, J, Iasiello, M and Lo, L 2018 Improving the wellbeing and resilience of health services staff via psychological skills training. BMC Research Notes 11: 15. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13104-018-4034-xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Casteren, A, Sellers, WI, Thorpe, SKS, Coward, S, Crompton, RH, Myatt, JP and Ennos, AR 2012 Nest-building orangutans demonstrate engineering know-how to produce safe, comfortable beds. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 109: 68736877. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1200902109CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Doeselaar, L, Yang, H, Bordes, J, Brix, L, Engelhardt, C, Tang, F and Schmidt, MV 2021 Chronic social defeat stress in female mice leads to sex-specific behavioral and neuroendocrine effects. Stress 24: 168180. https://doi.org/10.1080/10253890.2020.1864319CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Noordwijk, MA, Sauren, SEB, Abham, AA, Morrogh-Bernard, HC, Atmoko, SSU and van Schaik, CP 2009 Development of independence: Sumatran and Bornean orangutans compared. In: Wich, SA, Atmoko, SSU, Setia, RM, and van Schaik, CP (Eds.) Orangutans: Geographic Variation in Behavioral Ecology and Conservation pp. 189203. Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA.Google Scholar
van Noordwijk, MA and van Schaik, CP 2005 Development of ecological competence in Sumatran orangutans. American Journal of Physical Anthropology 127: 7994. https://doi.org/10.1002/AJPA.10426CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Varker, T and Devilly, GJ 2012 An analogue trial of inoculation/resilience training for emergency services personnel: Proof of concept. Journal of Anxiety Disorders 26: 696701. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2012.01.009CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Walker, SC, Cavieres, A, Peñaloza-Sancho, V, El-Deredy, W, McGlone, FP and Dagnino-Subiabre, A 2022 C-low threshold mechanoafferent targeted dynamic touch modulates stress resilience in rats exposed to chronic mild stress. European Journal of Neuroscience 55: 29252938. https://doi.org/10.1111/ejn.14951CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wang, Y, Jiang, Y, Song, BL, Zou, HW, Li, ZL, Li, LF and Liu, YJ 2022 mGlu2/3 receptors within the ventral part of the lateral septal nuclei modulate stress resilience and vulnerability in mice. Brain Research 1779: 147783. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2022.147783CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wei, Y, Wang, G, Wang, H, He, J, Zhang, N, Wu, Z, Xiao, L and Yang, C 2018 Sex-dependent impact of different degrees of maternal separation experience on OFT behavioral performances after adult chronic unpredictable mild stress exposure in rats. Physiology and Behavior 194: 153161. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2018.04.034CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weintraub, A, Singaravelu, J and Bhatnagar, S 2010 Enduring and sex-specific effects of adolescent social isolation in rats on adult stress reactivity. Brain Research 1343: 8392. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2010.04.068CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weiss, A, Gartner, MC, Gold, KC and Stoinski, TS 2013 Extraversion predicts longer survival in gorillas: An 18-year longitudinal study. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 280: 20122231. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2012.2231CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wemelsfelder, F, Haskell, M, Mendl, MT, Calvert, S and Lawrence, AB 2000 Diversity of behaviour during novel object tests is reduced in pigs housed in substrate-impoverished conditions. Animal Behaviour 60: 385394. https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.2000.1466CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wich, SA, Singleton, I, Nowak, MG, Utami Atmoko, SS, Nisam, G, Arif, SM, Putra, RH, Ardi, R, Fredriksson, G, Usher, G, Gaveau, DL and Kühl, HS 2016 Land-cover changes predict steep declines for the Sumatran orangutan (Pongo abelii). Science Advances 2: e1500789. https://doi.org/10.1126/sciadv.1500789CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wich, SA, Struebig, M, Refisch, J, Wilting, A, Kramer-Schadt, S and Meijaard, E 2015 The Future of the Bornean Orangutan: Impacts of Change in Land Cover and Climate. UNEP/GRASP: Nairobi, Kenya.Google Scholar
Wilkin, M, Waters, P, McCormick, C and Menard, J 2012 Intermittent physical stress during early- and mid-adolescence differentially alters rats’ anxiety- and depression-like behaviors in adulthood. Behavioral Neuroscience 126: 344360. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0027258CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Willmore, L, Cameron, C, Yang, J, Witten, IB and Falkner, AL 2022 Behavioural and dopaminergic signatures of resilience. Nature 611: 124132. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-022-05328-2CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wisløff, U, Ellingsen, Ø and Kemi, OJ 2009 High-intensity interval training to maximize cardiac benefits of exercise training? Exercise and Sport Sciences Reviews 37: 139146. https://doi.org/10.1097/JES.0b013e3181aa65fcCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wommack, JC, Salinas, A, Melloni, RH and Delville, Y 2004 Behavioural and neuroendocrine adaptations to repeated stress during puberty in male golden hamsters. Journal of Neuroendocrinology 16: 767775. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2826.2004.01233.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wood, GE, Norris, EH, Waters, E, Stoldt, JT and McEwen, BS 2008 Chronic immobilization stress alters aspects of emotionality and associative learning in the rat. Behavioral Neuroscience 122: 282292. https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7044.122.2.282CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yan, L, Wang, M, Yang, F, Wang, Y, Wang, S, So, KF and Zhang, L 2023 Physical exercise mediates a cortical FMRP–mTOR pathway to improve resilience against chronic stress in adolescent mice. Translational Psychiatry 13: 16. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41398-023-02311-xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yohn, NL and Blendy, JA 2017 Adolescent chronic unpredictable stress exposure is a sensitive window for long-term changes in adult behavior in mice. Neuropsychopharmacology 42: 16701678. https://doi.org/10.1038/npp.2017.11CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zalosnik, MI, Pollano, A, Trujillo, V, Suárez, MM and Durando, PE 2014 Effect of maternal separation and chronic stress on hippocampal-dependent memory in young adult rats: Evidence for the match-mismatch hypothesis. Stress 17: 445450. https://doi.org/10.3109/10253890.2014.936005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zambrana, C, Marco, EM, Arranz, L, De Castro, NM, Viveros, MP and De La Fuente, M 2007 Influence of aging and enriched environment on motor activity and emotional responses in mice. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1100: 543552. https://doi.org/10.1196/annals.1395.060CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zandberg, L, Quinn, JL, Naguib, M and van Oers, K 2017 Personality-dependent differences in problem-solving performance in a social context reflect foraging strategies. Behavioural Processes 134: 95102. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2016.09.007CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zhang, J, He, Z, Qu, Y, Li, L, Wang, L, Yuan, W, Hou, W, Zhu, Y, Cai, W, Zhang, X, Guo, Q, An, SC, Jia, R and Tai, F 2021 Different baseline physical activity predicts susceptibility and resilience to chronic social defeat stress in mice: Involvement of dopamine neurons. European Neuropsychopharmacology 45: 1528. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.euroneuro.2021.02.011CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zimmerman, PH and Koene, P 1998 The effect of loss of predictability and controllability of reward during frustration on behaviour in two strains of laying hens, Gallus gallus domesticus. Netherlands Journal of Zoology 48: 255265. https://doi.org/10.1163/156854298x00101Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Common themes in human resilience interventions

Figure 1

Table 2(a). Summary of generic stressor/stress treatment effects by taxonomic group, and sex

Figure 2

Table 2(b). Summary of maternal/group separation treatment effects by taxonomic group, and sex (96/266 papers reviewed)

Figure 3

Table 2(c). Summary of environmental conditions treatment effects by taxonomic group, and sex (41/266 papers reviewed)

Figure 4

Table 2(d). Summary of social stress treatment effects by taxonomic group, and sex (26/266 papers reviewed)

Figure 5

Table 2(e). Summary of exercise treatment effects by taxonomic group, and sex (21/266 papers reviewed)

Supplementary material: File

Bridgeland-Stephens et al. supplementary material
Download undefined(File)
File 260.1 KB