Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-94fs2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-13T01:09:10.828Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Genetic and phenotypic evidence of the predictive validity of preschool parent reports of hyperactivity/impulsivity and inattention

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 March 2024

Ginette Dionne*
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, Université Laval, Québec City, Canada
Sara Mascheretti
Affiliation:
Department of Brain and Behavioral Sciences, University of Pavia, Pavia, Italy Child Psychopathology Unit, Scientific Institute, IRCCS Eugenio Medea, Bosisio Parini, Italy
Bei Feng
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, Université Laval, Québec City, Canada
Hélène Paradis
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, Université Laval, Québec City, Canada
Mara Brendgen
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Université du Québec à Montréal, Montréal, Canada
Frank Vitaro
Affiliation:
School of Psychoeducation, Université de Montréal, Montréal, Canada
Richard Tremblay
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Université de Montréal, Montréal, Canada
Michel Boivin
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, Université Laval, Québec City, Canada
*
Corresponding author: G. Dionne; Email: ginette.dionne@psy.ulaval.ca
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

To determine the validity of parent reports (PRs) of ADHD in preschoolers, we assessed hyperactivity/impulsivity (HI) and inattention (IN) in 1114 twins with PRs at 1.5, 2.5, 4, 5, 14, 15, and 17 years, and teacher-reports at 6, 7, 9, 10, and 12. We examined if preschool PRs (1) predict high HI/IN trajectories, and (2) capture genetic contributions to HI/IN into adolescence. Group-based trajectory analyses identified three 6–17 years trajectories for both HI and IN, including small groups with high HI (N = 88, 10.4%, 77% boys) and IN (N = 158, 17.3%, 75% boys). Controlling for sex, each unit of HI PRs starting at 1.5 years and at 4 years for IN, increased more than 2-fold the risk of belonging to the high trajectory, with incremental contributions (Odds Ratios = 2.5–4.5) at subsequent ages. Quantitative genetic analyses showed that genetic contributions underlying preschool PRs accounted for up to a quarter and a third of the heritability of later HI and IN, respectively. Genes underlying 1.5-year HI and 4-year IN contributed to 6 of 8 later HI and IN time-points and largely explained the corresponding phenotypic correlations. Results provide phenotypic and genetic evidence that preschool parent reports of HI and IN are valid means to predict developmental risk of ADHD.

Type
Regular Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) is the most common neurobehavioral disorder in childhood and one of the most heritable, with estimates generally in the 50–80% range across the lifespan (see Grimm et al., Reference Grimm, Kranz and Reif2020 for a recent meta-analysis). It is marked by developmentally inappropriate levels of two behavioral dimensions (Narad et al., Reference Narad, Garner, Peugh, Tamm, Antonini, Kingery, Simon and Epstein2015) hyperactivity/impulsivity (HI) and inattention (IN), that interfere with functioning or development (Subcommittee on Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder, Steering Committee on Quality Improvement and Management, 2011). A lifelong disorder with roots in early childhood, it affects 5%–8% of children (Danielson et al., Reference Danielson, Bitsko, Ghandour, Holbrook, Kogan and Blumberg2018; Willoughby et al., Reference Willoughby, Pek and Greenberg2012) and 9%–14% of adolescents (Danielson et al., Reference Danielson, Bitsko, Ghandour, Holbrook, Kogan and Blumberg2018; Merikangas et al., Reference Merikangas, He, Burstein, Swanson, Avenevoli, Cui, Benjet, Georgiades and Swendsen2010), with a male to female ratio in the general population of 2 : 1 by middle school (American Psychiatric Association, 2013; Danielson et al., Reference Danielson, Bitsko, Ghandour, Holbrook, Kogan and Blumberg2018). Although symptom levels vary during development and stability of ADHD dimensions is moderate in population studies, up to 90% of children diagnosed in childhood retain significant impairments in early adulthood (Sibley et al., Reference Sibley, Arnold, Swanson, Hechtman, Kennedy, Owens, Molina, Jensen, Hinshaw, Roy, Chronis-Tuscano, Newcorn and Rohde2022).

ADHD is generally diagnosed in primary school (Visser et al., Reference Visser, Danielson, Bitsko, Holbrook, Kogan, Ghandour, Perou and Blumberg2014), but clinicians are posing them earlier (Danielson et al., Reference Danielson, Bitsko, Ghandour, Holbrook, Kogan and Blumberg2018, Halperin & Marks, Reference Halperin and Marks2019), and applying pharmacological treatments as early as age 3 (Cortese et al., Reference Froehlich, Anixt, Loe, Chirdkiatgumchai, Kuan and Gilman2022; Young et al., Reference Young, Yanagihara, Dew and Kollins2021). A survey by the American Center for Disease Control showed the prevalence of ADHD in 2–5-year-olds hovered around 2.1% in 2016, but there is recent evidence that the preschool prevalence is similar to school-age (Tobarra-Sanchez et al., Reference Tobarra-Sanchez, Riglin, Agha, Stergiakouli, Thapar and Langley2022). Yet, some scholars caution that preschool is too early for a reliable assessment (Overgaard et al., Reference Overgaard, Oerbeck, Friis, Pripp, Aase, Biele, Ingeborgrud, Polanczyk and Zeiner2022). Thus, there is no consensus as to how early ADHD can be detected (Halperin & Marks, Reference Halperin and Marks2019).

Assessment concerns in preschoolers

In preschoolers, the challenge is to distinguish HI and IN levels that may be disruptive-but-normative from pathological levels that reflect both the genetic loading of ADHD (Tobarra-Sanchez et al., Reference Tobarra-Sanchez, Riglin, Agha, Stergiakouli, Thapar and Langley2022) and the risk of a persistent trajectory (Halperin & Marks, Reference Halperin and Marks2019; Overgaard et al., Reference Overgaard, Oerbeck, Friis, Pripp, Aase, Biele, Ingeborgrud, Polanczyk and Zeiner2022; Vergunst et al., Reference Vergunst, Tremblay, Galera, Nagin, Vitaro, Boivin and Cote2019). The choice of information is a controversial issue in that respect (Schneider et al., Reference Schneider, Ryan and Mahone2020). Systematic observation is time consuming (Chen et al., Reference Chen, Lee, Wang, Chang, Lin and Ko2022; Schneider et al., Reference Schneider, Ryan and Mahone2020), and although multiple sources of information can be integrated (Chen et al., Reference Chen, Lee, Wang, Chang, Lin and Ko2022), it is often unfeasible. For practical reasons, structured interviews with parents and parent reports (PRs) are the norm to assess HI/IN in preschoolers, whether to inform diagnosis or conduct research. The rationale is that, although they may be biased (Chen et al., Reference Chen, Hwang-Gu, Ni, Liang, Lin, Lin, Tseng, Gau and Lidzba2017; Moens et al., Reference Moens, Weeland, Van der Giessen, Chhangur and Overbeek2018), parents have more opportunities than other source to observe their preschooler over long periods and in multiple settings (Schneider et al., Reference Schneider, Ryan and Mahone2020). By primary school, teachers are deemed a more valid source namely because they have access to a comparison group and the learning environment makes ADHD symptoms more salient (Narad et al., Reference Narad, Garner, Peugh, Tamm, Antonini, Kingery, Simon and Epstein2015). For these reasons, the validity of PRs of preschool HI and IN need to be assessed.

On way to address the issue is to test the predictive validity of preschool PRs across developmental periods and informants. Predictive validity refers to how well a measure predicts future outcomes. As a bidimensional disorder, ADHD lies at the extreme end of the continuous distributions of HI and IN, or IN alone for ADHD, predominantly inattentive presentation. In a lifelong disorder (Sibley et al., Reference Sibley, Arnold, Swanson, Hechtman, Kennedy, Owens, Molina, Jensen, Hinshaw, Roy, Chronis-Tuscano, Newcorn and Rohde2022), with a strong genetic underpinning (Grimm et al., Reference Grimm, Kranz and Reif2020), we expect valid preschool assessments of HI/IN to (1) predict developmental trajectories over time, and (2) capture the genetic liability of HI/IN through time.

Phenotypic stability of HI/IN from preschool onward

We found four studies reporting longitudinal correlations of HI and IN using PRs in preschoolers. Rietveld et al. (Reference Rietveld, Hudziak, Bartels, Van Beijsterveldt and Boomsma2003) reported correlations ranging from .44 to .65 between mother ratings of ADHD composites at ages 1.5, 3, and 5 years in a twin sample. Leblanc et al. (Reference Leblanc, Boivin, Dionne, Brendgen, Vitaro, Tremblay and Perusse2008) reported correlations ranging from .29 to .55 and .39 to 50 in HI symptoms based on father- and mother-reports, respectively, between ages 1.5, 2.5, 4, and 5 years in a population-based sample of 1,112 twins (Leblanc et al., Reference Leblanc, Boivin, Dionne, Brendgen, Vitaro, Tremblay and Perusse2008). Using a composite measure of ADHD, Price et al. (Reference Price, Simonoff, Asherson, Curran, Kuntsi, Waldman and Plomin2005) found correlations ranging from .46 to .60 across 3 yearly assessments in 2-, 3-, and 4-year-olds in a cohort of over 6000 twin pairs (Price et al., Reference Price, Simonoff, Asherson, Curran, Kuntsi, Waldman and Plomin2005). Kuntsi et al. (Reference Kuntsi, Rijsdijk, Ronald, Asherson and Plomin2005) extended the study of the same sample up to age 8 and found increasing correlations with age from preschool to primary school (r = .26 from ages 2 to 8; r = .37 from ages 3 to 8; and r = .46 from ages 4 to 8). Although PRs show some stability through time, no study spanned beyond early primary school, and they all relied on a single informant (father or mother), which can inflate correlations.

Other longitudinal studies in preschool and from preschool onward looked at stability by mapping developmental trajectories. They used single informant PRs in preschool, teacher-reports in primary school and self-reports in adolescence. The studies did not report cross-time correlations, but they consistently identified a group of children showing high symptom levels from onset. From 1.5 to 5 years, Salla et al. (Reference Salla, Michel, Pingault, Lacourse, Paquin, Galéra, Falissard, Boivin, Tremblay and Côté2016) identified chronic HI and IN levels in 13.4% and 2.5% of preschoolers, respectively, in a population-based longitudinal birth cohort. Covering the same age-range, Leblanc et al. (Reference Leblanc, Boivin, Dionne, Brendgen, Vitaro, Tremblay and Perusse2008) identified a subgroup with 7.1% of preschoolers with chronic HI levels. One study spanning preschool into primary school in a birth cohort from the general population (Galera et al., Reference Galera, Cote, Bouvard, Pingault, Melchior, Michel and Tremblay2011; 1.5–8 years), found that 16.1% and 13% of children followed high trajectories of HI and IN, respectively. Finally, one study spanning a birth cohort from the general population from preschool into adolescence (Vergunst et al., 2018; 1.5–17 years) combined two high trajectories of HI and IN comprising 21.4% and 20.2% of the sample, respectively. In sum, regardless of informant change (or not), trajectory studies starting at 1.5 years provide some evidence that preschool PRs detect the onset of high chronic levels of HI/IN. However, the studies spanning beyond preschool did not test if PRs in preschool predict future trajectories.

Genetic stability of HI/IN from preschool onward

A handful of studies examined the role of genetic factors in the phenotypic stability of HI/IN dimensions in preschoolers, all using maternal PRs and ADHD composites. In a population-based sample investigating parent-reported ADHD symptoms, Eilertsen et al. (Reference Eilertsen, Gjerde, Kendler, Røysamb, Aggen, Gustavson, Reichborn-Kjennerud and Ystrom2019) found that genetic correlations range from .77 between ages 1.5 and 3 years and .89 between ages 3 and 5 years. Price et al., (Reference Price, Simonoff, Waldman, Asherson and Plomin2001, Reference Price, Simonoff, Asherson, Curran, Kuntsi, Waldman and Plomin2005) found that genetic factors explained 91% of the covariance between ADHD composites at ages 2, 3 and 4 years in a twin cohort exploring genetic and environmental continuity and change of parent-reported ADHD symptoms. With the same sample followed over 4 time-points from ages 2 to 8 years, Kuntsi et al. (Reference Kuntsi, Rijsdijk, Ronald, Asherson and Plomin2005) found that genetic factors at ages 2, 3, and 4 contributed, respectively, to 32%, 45%, and 51% of the heritability of ADHD at age 8. Thus, across the preschool and early primary school years, genetic factors overlap and largely explain stability.

One possible caveat is that estimates in these studies could be inflated by the use of a single parent informant. In genetic studies using a combination of parent, teacher and/or self-reports spanning primary school to late adolescence (Chang et al., Reference Chang, Lichtenstein, Asherson and Larsson2013; Faraone et al., Reference Faraone, Asherson, Banaschewski, Biederman, Buitelaar, Ramos-Quiroga, Rohde, Sonuga-Barke, Tannock and Franke2015; Greven et al., Reference Greven, Asherson, Rijsdijk and Plomin2011; Larsson et al., Reference Larsson, Chang, D'Onofrio and Lichtenstein2014; Larsson et al., Reference Larsson, Larsson and Lichtenstein2004; Pingault et al., Reference Pingault, Viding, Galera, Greven, Zheng, Plomin and Rijsdijk2015), the genetic overlap across age was larger within the same informant than across informants. Nonetheless, regardless of informant, the results showed that the stability of HI/IN in primary school and adolescence stems from a cascade of broad genetic factors (additive or dominant) present at onset, with systematic age-specific genetic and unique environment contributions (Chang et al., Reference Chang, Lichtenstein, Asherson and Larsson2013; Greven et al., Reference Greven, Asherson, Rijsdijk and Plomin2011; Larsson et al., Reference Larsson, Dilshad, Lichtenstein and Barker2011; Pingault et al., Reference Pingault, Viding, Galera, Greven, Zheng, Plomin and Rijsdijk2015).

The present study

In sum, PRs are the norm to assess preschoolers. They show increasing phenotypic stability during preschool and a similar genetic architecture to what is observed in primary school and adolescence with other informants. Yet, their validity remains controversial. Given the changing normative nature of HI and IN across development, we need empirical evidence that preschool PRs of HI and IN are valid means of capturing the early development of ADHD and its underlying genetic liability. A genetically informed longitudinal design that starts with PRs in preschool and with multiple assessments by various informants through late adolescence is well-suited for this purpose. First, at the phenotypic level, it can examine if preschool PRs predict later HI/IN from different informants as well as chronic developmental courses of HI and IN. Second, at the genetic level, it can assess if genetic factors underlying preschool PRs of HI/IN overlap with genetic influences detected at later ages. In a population-based birth cohort of twins with multi-informant assessments of HI and IN at 12 time-points from ages 1.5 to 17 years, we examined: (1) if PRs at ages 1.5, 2.5, 4, and 5 years predict subsequent high HI/IN trajectories from primary school into late adolescence (Objective 1) and, (2) if genetic contributions to preschool PRs of HI/IN span into adolescence across informants (Objective 2).

Method

Sample

Data are issued from the Quebec Newborn Twin Study (QNTS; Boivin et al., Reference Boivin, Brendgen, Dionne, Dubois, Pérusse, Robaey, Tremblay and Vitaro2013). Parents of all twins born in the Greater Montreal area between April 1995 and December 1998 (989 families) were identified through birth records and invited by letter or phone to participate; 662 families agreed to participate and met inclusion criteria (parent fluency in either French or English, infants born without major medical conditions and available birth records). Quasi-annual assessments on a range of behavioral, cognitive, social, biological, genetic, and family characteristics were carried out starting at 6 months and are ongoing (Boivin et al., Reference Boivin, Brendgen, Dionne, Ouellet-Morin, Dubois, Pérusse, Robaey, Tremblay and Vitaro2019). Ethical approvals, parent, teacher and participant consent were obtained before each data collection. Non-identifying information was used in all analyses. The QNTS sample’s sociodemographic characteristics are comparable to those of an epidemiological sample of singletons born during the same period in the same province.

We used the Goldsmith (Reference Goldsmith1991) questionnaire on physical similarity to determine zygosity. DNA tests on a subsample of same-sex twin pairs (n = 123) showed a 96% accuracy rate (Forget-Dubois et al., Reference Forget-Dubois, Pérusse, Turecki, Girard, Billette, Rouleau, Boivin, Malo and Tremblay2003). In 2007, zygosity was reassessed in inconclusive cases through a brief telephone interview (adapted from Spitz et al., Reference Spitz, Moutier, Reed, Busnel, Marchaland, Roubertoux and Carlier1996). The initial sample included 254 monozygotic (MZ) pairs (125 male and 129 female), 210 same-sex dizygotic (DZ) pairs (105 male and 105 female), and 203 opposite-sex DZ pairs (Boivin et al., Reference Boivin, Brendgen, Dionne, Dubois, Pérusse, Robaey, Tremblay and Vitaro2013).

For this study, we used data collected when the twins were 1.5 (M = 1.55, SD = 0.05), 2.5 (M = 2.58, SD = 0.07), 4 (M = 4.11, SD = 0.15), 5 (M = 5.28, SD = 0.27), 6 (M = 6.04, SD = 0.27), 7 (M = 7.06, SD = 0.27), 9 (M = 9.07, SD = 0.29), 10 (M = 10.00, SD = 0.28), 12 (M = 12.09, SD = 0.28), 14 (M = 14.075, SD = 0.29), 15 (M = 15.09, SD = 0.26), and 17 (M = 17.07, SD = 0.30) years of age. Selected participants had to have complete HI/IN data for at least one time-point. The final sample included 1114 participants at age 1.5 years, 1045 at 2.5, 912 at 4, 937 at 5, 788 at 6, 838 at 7, 754 at 9, 779 at 10, 629 at 12, 831 at 14, 792 at 15, and 820 at 17. Attrition rate from ages 1.5 to 17 years was 27.5%, an average of 1.77% per year. Ns vary across assessments for a variety of reasons (families/participants lost to the study, families not participating at specific data collections, individual teacher’s agreement to participate or missing items). Though Little’s MCAR test shows data were not missing completely at random (MCAR) over the course of the study (χ 2 = 6026.49, df = 4919, p < .001), data were MCAR during the preschool (χ 2 = 112.52, df = 97, p = .13) and primary school years (χ 2 = 79.76, df = 73, p = .28) and MCAR between the first and last time-points (1.5–17 years: χ 2 = 6.55, df = 8, p = .59). Nonetheless, we implemented maximum likelihood strategies in trajectory analyses, binary logistic models and genetic models to avoid bias due to missing values.

Measures

HI and IN symptoms included, respectively, four and three items from the Social Behavior Questionnaire (SBQ) (see Collet et al., Reference Collet, Orri, Tremblay, Boivin and Côté2022). Items for HI were (1) can’t sit still, (2) is restless or hyperactive, (3) impulsive, acts without thinking, and (4) difficulty waiting his/her turn in games/activities; items for IN were (1) cannot concentrate, cannot pay attention for a long time, (2) is inattentive, and (3) is easily distracted. At each age, items were rated on a three-point scale (0 = never, 1 = sometimes, 2 = often), and averaged to yield HI and IN scores between 0 and 2. The SBQ has good psychometric properties (Collet et al., Reference Collet, Orri, Tremblay, Boivin and Côté2022). Ordinal alphas in this sample ranged between .74 and .94 for HI, and between .74 and .97 for IN.

Fathers and mothers rated HI and IN symptoms for both twins through face-to-face computerized interviews or questionnaires (depending on where the data collection took place – home/laboratory visit or by mail – and the parent’s choice when available) from ages 1.5 to 5 years (94%–98% of mothers and 53%–72% of fathers, regardless of zygosity) and from ages 14 to 17 years (95%–96% of mothers and 60%–62% of fathers). Correlations between father and mother for the same child at the same age ranged from .44 to .47 in preschool and .45 to .49 in adolescence for HI ratings, and from .28 to .39 in preschool and .61 to .64 in adolescence for IN ratings. Mother- and father- ratings were averaged at each age allowing one missing value to keep multiple raters when available. Different teachers each year (home room teachers only) assessed symptoms on the same scales using questionnaires from ages 6 to 12 years. Most twin pairs were not assessed by the same teacher (60.8%–76.4%) across primary school. Teachers were not solicited in adolescence because students change teachers for different subjects and it would have been difficult to identify a specific teacher very knowledgeable of the participants’ HI and IN behaviors.

Table 1 presents correlations across ages between 1.5 and 17 years for HI (below diagonal) and IN (above diagonal), highlighting correlations across preschool and later measures (bottom left for HI and top right for IN). Overall, correlations between preschool PRs and later measures were at best modest for HI (rs = .09–.37) and IN (rs = .05–36) but increased from ages 1.5 to 5 years, similarly for HI and IN (Fisher z test results comparing correlations are available on the Supplementary Information document – Table M1).

Table 1. Homotypic correlations for hyperactivity/impulsivity (below diagonal) and inattention (above diagonal). Correlations between preschool and later measures are highlighted. Adjacent time-point correlations appear in bold

Statistical analyses

Trajectory analyses

We used group-based trajectory analyses with the PROC TRAJ procedure (Daniel S. Nagin & Tremblay, Reference Nagin and Tremblay2005) on SAS version 9.4 (SAS Institute Inc, 2014) to identify trajectory patterns for HI and IN from ages 6 to 17 including teacher ratings from ages 6 to 12 and parent ratings from ages 14 to 17 years. Participants with a minimum of 2 out of 8 measures were included (n = 957; 49.8% boys, 43.8% MZ twins). The analyses generated solutions for 2–6-trajectory groups based on minimizing within- and maximizing between-trajectory differences. For each solution, slopes were tested. Analyses used a maximum likelihood estimator and data were modeled using a censored normal distribution. Model selection was based on seven criteria: (1) the Bayesian Information Criterion (BIC > .90) and the Akaike Information Criterion (AIC – lowest value) for fit adequacy and parsimony, (2) group intercept and slope significance, (3) average probability of group membership > .80, (4) no group membership < 5%, (5) odds of correct classification ≥ 5 for all groups, (6) difference between estimated group probabilities πj (i.e., the population size of trajectory group j estimated by the model) and the proportion Pj assigned to the group (i.e., the actual proportion of individuals assigned to group j) using the maximum probability rule, and (7) coherence with theory.

Binomial logistic regressions

We used binomial regression models implemented in Mplus version 8.1 (Muthén & Muthén, Reference Muthén and Muthen2017) with a maximum likelihood estimator to predict membership versus non-membership in the high 6–17 years trajectories of HI and IN. Sex was entered first in all models. Measures at 1.5, 2.5, 4, and 5 years were entered in successive models (2–5) to estimate their added contributions through time.

Genetic modeling

We used the Cholesky decomposition model implemented in Mplus version 8.1 (Muthén & Muthén, Reference Muthén and Muthen2017) to decompose the variances and covariances of HI and IN across 12 time-points. This choice of model was based on one study (Kuntsi et al., Reference Kuntsi, Rijsdijk, Ronald, Asherson and Plomin2005) showing the Cholesky model fit longitudinal ADHD symptoms data better than five alternate models (i.e., the independent pathway model, the common pathway model, the simplex model and the state-trait model). Participants with missing data were included using full information maximum likelihood to handle missing values. We used the usual fit indices (Akaike Information Criteria, Comparison Fit Index and Root Mean Square Error of Approximation) to assess and compare nested model fit and 95% confidence intervals to assess the significance of estimates.

Genetic modeling decomposes phenotypic variances and covariances into their genetic (additive A and/or dominant D), shared environment (C) and unique environment (E; including measurement error) components. The rationale is based on the fact that, by descent, monozygotic twins (MZ) share 100% of their segregating genes while dizygotic twins (DZ) share on average 50% and that both types of twin pairs grow in the same family. The extent to which MZ twins are more similar than DZ twins reflects contributions from genes. Similarity within families, regardless of zygosity, reflects shared environment contributions. Unique environment reflects differences within MZ twin pairs. The model postulates equal environments for MZ and DZ, whereby environmentally caused similarity does not differ for MZ and DZ twins. The model further assumes the additivity of variance and covariance components. The Cholesky decomposition estimates the contributions of A (and/or D), C, and E components specific to each time-point and from all previous data points (Loehlin et al., Reference Loehlin, Neiderhiser and Reiss2005).

Results

Descriptive statistics

Table 2 presents means and SDs for the total sample, by sex and by zygosity for HI (top panel) and IN (bottom panel).

Table 2. Means (SD) for the total sample, by sex, zygosity, and age for hyperactivity/impulsivity (top) and inattention (bottom) by age

HI means peaked at age 4, whereas IN peaked at age 7. Overall, t-tests and Levene tests (see Supplementary Information document – Table S1) identified (1) higher levels of HI in preschool PRs (.73–.92) than in primary school teacher-reports (.34–.56) and adolescence PRs (.31–.44); (2) higher levels of IN in primary school teacher-reports (.66–.85) than preschool (.50–.70) and adolescence (.45–.62) PRs (except at age 4 years); (3) sex mean and variance differences, with higher levels in boys than girls of both HI and IN at all time-points, except IN at 1.5 years, and greater variances on all measures in boys from age 6 years onward; (4) lower levels of HI and IN in MZs at 6, 7, 17 years and 7 and 17 years than in DZ, and (5) lower variances in HI and IN in MZs than DZ at 1.5, 2.5, 4, 7, 15, and 17 years, and at 5 and 17, respectively. Nonetheless, constraining sex and zygosity means and variances to equality in genetic models did not deteriorate model fit (data available on request) indicating no consistent scalar or contrast effect. We did not pursue further testing of age-specific contrast or scalar effects.

Objective 1

Trajectories analyses were implemented separately for HI and IN, testing 2–6-trajectory solutions between ages 6 and 17 years (see Supplementary Information document – Table S2).

For both HI and IN, based on predefined criteria, the 3-trajectory solution with significant multinomial quadratic terms was retained (Fig. 1). The 3-trajectory solutions for HI and IN are consistent with theory and identify a high decreasing subgroup with membership proportions (10.4% for HI and 17.3% for IN).

Figure 1. Trajectories of hyperactivity/impulsivity (top panel) and inattention (bottom panel) from 6 to 17 years. Dotted lines illustrate estimated values; bold lines illustrate observed values.

Table 3 presents binomial logistic regression results of models predicting membership versus non-membership in the high decreasing trajectories of HI (top) and IN (bottom) from preschool PRs.

Table 3. Binary logistic regression models predicting high decreasing trajectories of HI (top) and IN (bottom) from 1.5, 2.5, 4, and 5-year parent reports of HI and IN

Note. Constant is included in all models but not shown; LRT = Likelihood Ratio Test.

Odds ratios (ORs) for sex varied between 3.10 and 3.77 in all models indicating that being a boy increased about 3–4-fold the risk of belonging to high trajectories of both HI and IN. When entered, HI at ages 1.5, 2.5, 4, and 5 years all predicted membership, above and beyond sex and previous HI. Every level unit of the newly introduced HI measure increased the risk of belonging to the high trajectory 2–4.5-fold (ORs = 2.09, 4.59). In the final model, only sex and HI at age 5 years uniquely predicted membership. Similarly, when entered, IN at ages 4 and 5 years predicted membership above and beyond sex and previous IN. Every level unit of the newly introduced IN increased the risk of belonging to the high trajectory 2.5–4-fold (ORs = 2.53, 4.09). In the final model, only sex and IN at age 5 years uniquely predicted membership.

Objective 2

Table 4 presents intraclass correlations (ICCs) for MZ and DZ twins for HI (right) and IN (left). As MZ ICCs exceeded double the DZ ICCs for all HI measures, and eight out of 12 IN measures, ADE (additive genetic, dominant genetic and unique environment model), ACE (additive genetic, shared environment and unique environment model) and nested models AE (additive genetic and unique environment model) were tested. MZ and DZ variances and means were constrained to equality without deteriorating model fit. For both HI and IN, the best fitting model was an AE model (Table 5).

Table 4. MZ and DZ intraclass correlations (ICC) for hyperactivity/impulsivity (left) and inattention (right) by age

Table 5. Comparisons of Cholesky model fits for HI and IN. The best fitting models appear in bold

Note. ADE = Additive genetic, dominant genetic and unique environment model; ACE = Additive genetic, shared environment and unique environment model; AE = Additive genetic and unique environment model; DF = Degrees of freedom; AIC = Akaike information criterion; BIC = Bayesian information criterion; CFI = Comparison fit index; RMSEA = Root mean square error of approximation.

Table 6 provides standardized estimates of A (left) and E (right) for all time-points for HI (top) and IN (bottom) and the contributions of preschool time-points to all later time-points (with 95% confidence intervals).

Table 6. standardized parameter estimates from Cholesky model for HI and IN of additive genetic (A) and nonshared environment (E) for preschool measures to subsequent measures. Significant parameters based on 95% confidence intervals are presented in bold

HI heritability was relatively stable from ages 1.5 to 17 years (.51– .79) with no consistent differences across ages and informants. Together, preschool measures accounted for 24%, 23%, 13%, 11%, 16%, 23%, 22%, and 16.5%, respectively, of the total heritability of HI at ages 6 to 17 years (i.e., the sum of the A contribution to each preschool measure divided by the total heritability). Notably, genetic factors at age 1.5 years accounted for 4%–13% of the heritability of all eight primary school and adolescence measures except for two at ages 10 and 17. Genes at age 1.5 years explained 92%, 100%, 85%, 94%, 81%, and 68% of the albeit modest correlations with measures at ages 6, 7, 9, 12, 14, and 15 respectively. Persisting genetic contributions from teacher-reports at age 6 through primary school accounted for 28%–59% of the heritability of later measures (see Supplementary Information document – Table S3). Contributions of E overlapped in preschool measures but were largely age-specific thereafter.

IN heritability was also relatively stable from ages 1.5 to 17 years (.41–.62), albeit more moderate than HI heritability overall, and with no consistent differences across ages and informants. Together, preschool measures accounted for 11%, 19%, 21%, 24%, 18%, 20%, 36%, and 26% of the heritability of IN at ages 6, 7, 9, 10, 12, 14, 15, and 17, respectively. Similar to HI, we observed persisting genetic contributions from preschool IN except at age 4. Significant contributions accounted for 11% – 25% of the heritability of all eight primary school and adolescence measures except for two at ages 6 and 14. Genes at age 4 years explained 92%, 96%, 100%, 85%, 80%, and 79% of the phenotypic correlations with measures at 7, 9, 10, 12, 15, and 17, respectively. Persisting genetic contributions through primary school, accounting for 21%–48% of the heritability of later measures, were also observed from teacher-reports at age 6. Genetic factors from previous ages completely accounted for the heritability of IN from ages 14 to 17 (see Supplementary Information document – Table S3). Finally, contributions of E were mostly age specific.

Parameter estimates for the full Cholesky decomposition are provided in Supplementary Information document – Table S3. They show that, after age 6, additional genetic and unique environment contributions for both HI and IN are mostly age specific.

Discussion

This population-based study provides empirical evidence that preschool parent reports of hyperactivity/impulsivity and inattention capture both phenotypic and genetic liabilities that span over 15 years, and across multiple informants. As such, it offers new insights into the predictive validity of preschool PRs of HI and IN. At the phenotypic level, the findings show that, starting at age 1.5 years for HI and 4 years for IN, preschool parent reports predict trajectories depicting high levels of HI and IN from ages 6 to 17 years. Moreover, the prediction is incremental: subsequent preschool assessments increase the relative risk. At the genetic level, the most striking result is that the ages of onset of the phenotypic predictions coincide with new genetic influences that are largely responsible for the stability of ADHD dimensions from preschool to adolescence. Together, genes expressed before age 6 account for up to a quarter of the heritability of HI and a third of the heritability of IN up to age 17 years.

Predicting developmental trajectories

The first objective was to determine if preschool PRs predict subsequent high HI/IN trajectories into late adolescence. No study had previously examined the longitudinal stability of preschool HI and IN over this age range. We identified three-trajectory patterns with one high decreasing trajectory for both HI and IN. Similar three-trajectory patterns were observed in most trajectory/profile studies including preschoolers (Galera et al., Reference Galera, Cote, Bouvard, Pingault, Melchior, Michel and Tremblay2011; Leblanc et al., Reference Leblanc, Boivin, Dionne, Brendgen, Vitaro, Tremblay and Perusse2008; Salla et al., Reference Salla, Michel, Pingault, Lacourse, Paquin, Galéra, Falissard, Boivin, Tremblay and Côté2016) and older children and adolescents (Pingault et al., Reference Pingault, Tremblay, Vitaro, Carbonneau, Genolini, Falissard and Cote2011; Sasser et al., Reference Sasser, Kalvin and Bierman2016). Vergunst et al., (2018) is the only previous study to cover preschool through adolescence. These authors also found small groups of preschoolers following chronic trajectories of HI and IN but did not test if preschool levels predicted these outcomes. Our study provides evidence that high trajectories of HI/IN from primary school to adolescence, largely informed by different raters at different ages, can be predicted by monitoring preschool development. Monitoring is the key word: while individual preschool time-points, starting at age 1.5 years for HI and 4 years for IN, predict the high trajectory outcomes, regression models show that each subsequent time-point adds to the prediction. These results are also consistent with studies showing that HI in toddlers, and possibly earlier, is more predictive as a dimension of ADHD than IN (Joseph et al., Reference Joseph, Lorenzo, Fisher, Novick, Gibson, Rothenberger, Foust and Chronis‐Tuscano2023). Overall, the phenotypic results suggest that timely monitoring is needed to assess risk more reliably than single screening assessments and that parents of preschoolers provide valuable information in that respect.

Early onset genetic factors and their role in phenotypic stability

The second objective of this study was to test if genetic contributions to preschool PRs of HI/IN span into adolescence and across informants. The rationale is that, as valid predictors of later HI/IN, preschool PRs should capture the underlying genetic liability of what is stable in ADHD dimensions. The high heritability of both HI and IN from preschool onward is well documented (Grimm et al., Reference Grimm, Kranz and Reif2020). Estimates of heritability in this study are well within the expected range overall, slightly higher for HI than IN, with no systematic change across ages.

The most remarkable results pertain to the presence of early onset genetic factors, as early as age 1.5 years for HI and 4 years for IN, which show enduring contributions through adolescence. Genes underlying individual differences in HI at age 1.5 years, as reported by parents, account for a modest portion of the heritability of later HI but are almost entirely responsible for the stability of this dimension of ADHD from preschool to adolescence. This occurs later for IN, at age 4 years, even before the attentional demands of formal schooling. The daily routine of a 4-year-old may be less likely to make IN salient, yet our results show that parents are sensitive to heritable individual differences underlying the later stability of IN. Moreover, when genetic factors present between ages 1.5 and 5 are considered, together they explain a substantial proportion of the heritability of later HI and IN.

Previous studies had shown that ADHD symptoms were highly heritable at ages 1.5–2 years and that these genetic factors were shared with symptoms up to ages 4–5 (Eilertsen et al., Reference Eilertsen, Gjerde, Kendler, Røysamb, Aggen, Gustavson, Reichborn-Kjennerud and Ystrom2019; Price et al., Reference Price, Simonoff, Waldman, Asherson and Plomin2001, Reference Price, Simonoff, Asherson, Curran, Kuntsi, Waldman and Plomin2005) and age 8 (Kuntsi et al., Reference Kuntsi, Rijsdijk, Ronald, Asherson and Plomin2005). However, no behavior genetic study had shown that genetic influences identified in preschool explained part of the heritability of HI and IN into late adolescence. Nevertheless, these results are consistent with a recent systematic review (Bonvicini et al., Reference Bonvicini, Faraone and Scassellati2018) and a recent meta-analysis of genome-wide studies of ADHD (Rovira et al., Reference Rovira, Demontis, Sánchez-Mora, Zayats, Klein, Mota, Weber, Garcia-Martínez, Pagerols, Vilar-Ribó, Arribas, Richarte, Corrales, Fadeuilhe, Bosch, Martin, Almos, Doyle and Ribasés2020) showing that many ADHD candidate genes and SNPs overlap in children and adults.

Genetic factors than also span longitudinally were found at age 6 years, at school entry. These could underlie symptom changes as cognitive demands and behavioral regulation increase, but also the change from parent to teacher-reports. Hypotheses regarding the sources of change are more limited in this context. Nonetheless, the substantial genetic contributions of preschool PRs into primary school and adolescence, and again at age 6 with teacher-reports, could be reflected by the decrease in dopamine (DA) transporter density during life (Jucaite et al., Reference Jucaite, Forssberg, Karlsson, Halldin and Farde2010). DA transporter and receptor genes are the most important components in the etiology of ADHD (Wu et al., Reference Wu, Xiao, Sun, Zou and Zhu2012). As both hypo- and hyperdopaminergic states will impair the prefrontal cortex (PFC) functions (Arnsten, Reference Arnsten1998), and the PFC is primary in mediating executive functions whose impairments are correlated with ADHD symptoms (Willcutt et al., Reference Willcutt, Doyle, Nigg, Faraone and Pennington2005), it is plausible that the age-dependent decrease in DA transporters and receptors may impair the PFC functions, which in turn may affect the ADHD symptoms throughout adolescence and adulthood. Alternatively, common genetic variations underlying risk for other traits (e.g., developmental dyslexia, language development, general cognitive ability) may also contribute to HI and IN symptoms (Couto et al., Reference Couto, Gomez, Wigg, Ickowicz, Pathare, Malone, Kennedy, Schachar and Barr2009; Mascheretti et al., Reference Mascheretti, Trezzi, Giorda, Boivin, Plourde, Vitaro, Brendgen, Dionne and Marino2017; Wigg et al., Reference Wigg, Feng, Crosbie, Tannock, Kennedy, Ickowicz, Malone, Schachar and Barr2008). Another possibility is the presence of gene-by-environment interactions whereby the cognitive and social demands at specific periods (e.g., increasing demands for self-regulation in toddlers, increasing cognitive demands in late preschool and exposure to schooling in early primary school) foster the expression of genetic liabilities in a diathesis-stress fashion (Kovas & Plomin, Reference Kovas and Plomin2007).

Overall, the present results suggest that the genetic factors involved in individual differences of ADHD dimensions are in place before school entry, that PRs are able to detect them, and that the same genetic factors continue to play a role in later HI and IN heritability. This pattern is consistent with previous studies in primary school and adolescence (Pingault et al., Reference Pingault, Viding, Galera, Greven, Zheng, Plomin and Rijsdijk2015) and suggests the onset is probably earlier.

Environmental contributions to the longitudinal architecture of HI and IN

Unique environment factors contributed substantially to individual differences in IN (.37–.60) and to a lesser extent in HI (.22–.50), more so than in previous studies. There are two predominant patterns of environmental influences on ADHD: transient effects contributing to single measurement occasions and stable effects that persist over time (Livingstone et al., Reference Livingstone, Coventry, Corley, Willcutt, Samuelsson, Olson and Byrne2016). As in previous studies, we found mostly transient unique environment factors (Chang et al., Reference Chang, Lichtenstein, Asherson and Larsson2013; Faraone et al., Reference Faraone, Asherson, Banaschewski, Biederman, Buitelaar, Ramos-Quiroga, Rohde, Sonuga-Barke, Tannock and Franke2015; Greven et al., Reference Greven, Asherson, Rijsdijk and Plomin2011; Kuntsi et al., Reference Kuntsi, Rijsdijk, Ronald, Asherson and Plomin2005; Larsson et al., Reference Larsson, Larsson and Lichtenstein2004, Reference Larsson, Chang, D'Onofrio and Lichtenstein2014; Pingault et al., Reference Pingault, Viding, Galera, Greven, Zheng, Plomin and Rijsdijk2015).

Unique environment factors are rarely of a persistent nature in psychopathology. Traditional interpretations of environmental risks in psychopathology target family-level risks (Froehlich et al., Reference Froehlich, Anixt, Loe, Chirdkiatgumchai, Kuan and Gilman2011; Law et al., Reference Law, Sideridis, Prock and Sheridan2014; Sfelinioti & Livaditis, Reference Sfelinioti and Livaditis2017; Thapar et al., Reference Thapar, Cooper, Jefferies and Stergiakouli2012; Vergunst et al., Reference Vergunst, Tremblay, Galera, Nagin, Vitaro, Boivin and Cote2019; Wolford et al., Reference Wolford, Lahti, Tuovinen, Lahti, Lipsanen, Savolainen, Heinonen, Hämäläinen, Kajantie, Pesonen, Villa, Laivuori, Reynolds and Räikkönen2017). However, if these were affecting children from the same family similarly, we should find shared environment factors in the etiology of ADHD symptoms, yet very few studies do so (Wood et al., Reference Wood, Buitelaar, Rijsdijk, Asherson and Kuntsi2010). An explanation for the inconsistencies across genetic and phenotypic studies regarding what constitutes unique environment may be that presumably shared environmental risks affect children within families differently (Barkley, Reference Barkley2016; Capusan et al., Reference Capusan, Kuja-Halkola, Bendtsen, Viding, McCrory, Marteinsdottir and Larsson2016; Jimenez et al., Reference Jimenez, Wade, Schwartz-Soicher, Lin and Reichman2017; Lehn et al., Reference Lehn, Derks, Hudziak, Heutink, van Beijsterveldt and Boomsma2007; Pettersson et al., Reference Pettersson, Sjolander, Almqvist, Anckarsater, D’Onofrio, Lichtenstein and Larsson2015).

Biological processes occurring during foetal development such as post-twinning de novo copy number variants (Ehli et al., Reference Ehli, Abdellaoui, Hu, Hottenga, Kattenberg, van Beijsterveldt, Bartels, Althoff, Xiao, Scheet, de Geus, Hudziak, Boomsma and Davies2012) or methylation processes (Walton et al., Reference Walton, Pingault, Cecil, Gaunt, Relton, Mill and Barker2017) not shared by co-twins could create differences that emerge at an age when normative HI should start decreasing in most children. Tikhodeyev and Shcherbakova (Reference Tikhodeyev and Shcherbakova2019) argued that the stochasticity of molecular processes at critical stages of development can cause MZ dissimilarities and that most of the unique environment factors affecting a variety of phenotypes are internal in nature rather than external. These could influence the individual structure and functional capacity of brain networks involved in behavior and cognition (Faraone et al., Reference Faraone, Asherson, Banaschewski, Biederman, Buitelaar, Ramos-Quiroga, Rohde, Sonuga-Barke, Tannock and Franke2015), particularly of the frontal executive brain (Barkley, Reference Barkley2016).

Issues with parent reports?

PRs of HI and IN are the most frequent sources of information in preschoolers. Although most recognize the ecological validity of PRs at face value (Schneider et al., Reference Schneider, Ryan and Mahone2020), many question the ability of parents to provide unbiased assessments and to recognize what deviates from the normative development of behavior/attention regulation. Participants in this study were assessed almost yearly and by up to seven different raters at different time-points. Fathers and mothers provided concurrent assessments in preschool and in adolescence; teachers provided successive assessments during primary school. Our results replicate earlier findings of modest agreement across raters (Schneider et al., Reference Schneider, Ryan and Mahone2020) but also shed new light on what can and cannot be attributed to parent biases.

First, although agreement was higher within PRs and across teachers, there was increasing agreement between teachers and parents by adolescence suggesting that lower agreement at earlier ages may reflect developmental changes in ADHD dimensions. Indeed, agreement across parent- and teacher-reports did not differ at the transition from primary school to adolescence. Second, low DZ correlations often attributed to parent contrast effects show similar patterns in teacher-reports where twins mostly have different teachers. Third, levels of HI are highest in preschool PRs but lowest in adolescence PRs. Often, high HI levels in PRs were deemed inflated, reflecting parents’ difficulties in dealing with hyperactive children (Chen et al., Reference Chen, Hwang-Gu, Ni, Liang, Lin, Lin, Tseng, Gau and Lidzba2017; Moens et al., Reference Moens, Weeland, Van der Giessen, Chhangur and Overbeek2018). Similarly, PRs were deemed less reliable in capturing IN in preschoolers. On the contrary, teachers are deemed a more valid source in young children because they often tend to consider child behavior as normative (Narad et al., Reference Narad, Garner, Peugh, Tamm, Antonini, Kingery, Simon and Epstein2015). However, mean levels of IN did not differ across parents and teachers from ages 4 to 12 but declined in adolescence PRs. The aggregation of concurrent mother- and father-reports likely reduced both contrast effects and inflated means (Sollie et al., Reference Sollie, Larsson and Mørch2013). Although we cannot exclude the effects of rater changes, these results suggest that successive parent- and teacher-reports could likely reflect developmental changes.

Limits

These results need to be considered within the limits of the study. First, the sample had low power to detect dominant genetic factors, although intraclass correlation patterns appeared to suggest them. Second, there was no way to assess cross-informant agreement at the same age because we lacked overlapping parent- and teacher-reports. However, we were able to document that preschool PRs of HI/IN predict later HI/IN and capture their etiological continuity, regardless of informant. Third, although we were able to assess the predictive validity of preschool PRs of HI/IN across primary school and adolescence, significant impairments are retained also into early adulthood. Future studies are therefore needed in order to assess phenotypic and etiological continuity from early ages through adulthood. Fourth, the SBQ used only seven items to assess HI/IN and some aspects of the IN dimension specific to preschooler may be lacking. However, a good construct validity was previously reported (Collet et al., Reference Collet, Orri, Tremblay, Boivin and Côté2022) and our results showed the current scales predict developmental trajectories. Fifth, the present study measures HI and IN dimensions in a population-based sample of twins; the obtained results cannot be therefore generalized to clinical samples.

Conclusion

Behavior genetic studies often neglect the theoretical or clinical questions their intricate models address. This study highlights the usefulness of complementary clinically driven phenotypic and genetic analyses. The American Academy of Pediatrics invites practitioners to consider ADHD as a diagnosis as early as age 4, to either provide environmental support, parent training and/or pharmacological treatments that shield preschoolers from other deleterious effects of ADHD. Our results, from both a phenotypic and a genetic perspective, provide empirical evidence that PRs are valid means to monitor highly hyperactive/impulsive and/or inattentive toddlers when close follow-ups are implemented to detect the persistence of symptoms. The consistent predictions of age 6–17 years high trajectories of ADHD dimensions from successive preschool PRs empirically support this conclusion as do the genetic influences persisting into adolescence. Thus, not only do preschool parents reports of HI/IN incrementally predict chronic trajectories into late adolescence at the phenotypic level, but they also capture the genetic liability largely responsible for the stability of ADHD dimensions from preschool onward. Both medical and educational professionals should therefore be encouraged to get information about children for early identification and treatment of at-risk kids. This may potentially change the trajectory of psychiatric morbidity later in life and improve functional outcomes.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S095457942400035X.

Funding statement

This work was supported by the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada (GD, grant numbers 410-2009-2303, 435-2012-1042, 435-2016-1548), (MB, grant numbers 410-2011-2491. 435-2014-1536), (MB grant number 435-2018-1217); and the Canadian Institute of Health Research. We thank the twins, their family and their teachers for their contribution to the Quebec Newborn Twin Study.

Competing interests

None.

References

American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). American Psychiatric Association. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596.Google Scholar
Arnsten, A. F. (1998). Catecholamine modulation of prefrontal cortical cognitive function. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2(11), 436447. https://doi.org/10.1016/s1364-6613(98)01240-6 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barkley, R. A. (2016). Recent longitudinal studies of childhood attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Important themes and questions for further research. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 125(2), 248255. https://doi.org/10.1037/abn0000125 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boivin, M., Brendgen, M., Dionne, G., Dubois, L., Pérusse, D., Robaey, P., Tremblay, R. E., & Vitaro, F. (2013). The quebec newborn twin study into adolescence: 15 years later. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 16(1), 6469. https://doi.org/10.1017/thg.2012.129 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boivin, M., Brendgen, M., Dionne, G., Ouellet-Morin, I., Dubois, L., Pérusse, D., Robaey, P., Tremblay, R. E., & Vitaro, F. (2019). The quebec newborn twin study at 21. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 22(6), 475481. https://doi.org/10.1017/thg.2019.74 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bonvicini, C., Faraone, S. V., & Scassellati, C. (2018). Common and specific genes and peripheral biomarkers in children and adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. The World Journal of Biological Psychiatry, 19(2), 80100. https://doi.org/10.1080/15622975.2017.1282175 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Capusan, A. J., Kuja-Halkola, R., Bendtsen, P., Viding, E., McCrory, E., Marteinsdottir, I., & Larsson, H. (2016). Childhood maltreatment and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder symptoms in adults: A large twin study. Psychological Medicine, 46(12), 26372646. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291716001021 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chang, Z., Lichtenstein, P., Asherson, P. J., & Larsson, H. (2013). Developmental twin study of attention problems: High heritabilities throughout development. JAMA Psychiatry, 70(3), 311318. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.287 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chen, I. C., Lee, P. W., Wang, L. J., Chang, C. H., Lin, C. H., & Ko, L. W. (2022). Incremental validity of multi-method and multi-informant evaluations in the clinical diagnosis of preschool ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders, J Atten Disord, 26(10), 12931303. https://doi.org/10.1177/10870547211045739 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chen, Y.-C., Hwang-Gu, S.-L., Ni, H.-C., Liang, S. H..-Y., Lin, H.-Y., Lin, C.-F., Tseng, Y.-H., Gau, S. S..-F., & Lidzba, K. (2017). Relationship between parenting stress and informant discrepancies on symptoms of ADHD/ODD and internalizing behaviors in preschool children. PLoS One, 12(10), e0183467. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0183467 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Collet, O. A., Orri, M., Tremblay, R. E., Boivin, M., & Côté, S. M. (2022). Psychometric properties of the social behavior questionnaire (SBQ) in a longitudinal population-based sample. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 47(2), 180189. https://doi.org/10.1177/01650254221113472 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cortese, S., Sabé, M., Chen, C., Perroud, N., & Solmi, M. (2022). Half a century of research on attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A scientometric study. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 140, 104769. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2022.104769 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Couto, J. M., Gomez, L., Wigg, K., Ickowicz, A., Pathare, T., Malone, M., Kennedy, J. L., Schachar, R., & Barr, C. L. (2009). Association of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder with a candidate region for reading disabilities on chromosome 6p. Biological Psychiatry, 66(4), 368375. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.02.016 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Danielson, M. L., Bitsko, R. H., Ghandour, R. M., Holbrook, J. R., Kogan, M. D., & Blumberg, S. J. (2018). Prevalence of parent-reported ADHD diagnosis and associated treatment among U.S. children and adolescents. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 47(2), 199212. https://doi.org/10.1080/15374416.2017.1417860 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ehli, E. A., Abdellaoui, A., Hu, Y., Hottenga, J. J., Kattenberg, M., van Beijsterveldt, T., Bartels, M., Althoff, R. R., Xiao, X., Scheet, P., de Geus, E. J., Hudziak, J. J., Boomsma, D. I., & Davies, G. E. (2012). De novo and inherited CNVs in MZ twin pairs selected for discordance and concordance on attention problems. European Journal of Human Genetics, 20(10), 10371043. https://doi.org/10.1038/ejhg.2012.49 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eilertsen, E. M., Gjerde, L. C., Kendler, K. S., Røysamb, E., Aggen, S. H., Gustavson, K., Reichborn-Kjennerud, T., & Ystrom, E. (2019). Development of ADHD symptoms in preschool children: Genetic and environmental contributions. Development and Psychopathology, 31(4), 12991305. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579418000731 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Faraone, S. V., Asherson, P., Banaschewski, T., Biederman, J., Buitelaar, J. K., Ramos-Quiroga, J. A., Rohde, L. A., Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S., Tannock, R., & Franke, B. (2015). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Nature Reviews Disease Primers, 1(1), 15020. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrdp.2015.20 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Forget-Dubois, N., Pérusse, D., Turecki, G., Girard, A., Billette, J.-M., Rouleau, G., Boivin, M., Malo, J., & Tremblay, R. E. (2003). Diagnosing zygosity in infant twins: Physical similarity, genotyping, and chorionicity. Twin Research, 6(6), 479485. https://doi.org/10.1375/136905203322686464 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Froehlich, T. E., Anixt, J. S., Loe, I. M., Chirdkiatgumchai, V., Kuan, L., & Gilman, R. C. (2011). Update on environmental risk factors for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Current Psychiatry Reports, 13(5), 333344. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11920-011-0221-3 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Galera, C., Cote, S. M., Bouvard, M. P., Pingault, J. B., Melchior, M., Michel, G., & Tremblay, R. E. (2011). Early risk factors for hyperactivity-impulsivity and inattention trajectories from age 17 months to 8 years. Archives of General Psychiatry, 68(12), 12671275. https://doi.org/10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.138 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldsmith, H. H. (1991). A zygosity questionnaire for young twins: A research note. Behavior Genetics, 21(3), 257269. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01065819 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greven, C. U., Asherson, P., Rijsdijk, F. V., & Plomin, R. (2011). A longitudinal twin study on the association between inattentive and hyperactive-impulsive ADHD symptoms. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 39(5), 623632. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-011-9513-7 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grimm, O., Kranz, T. M., & Reif, A. (2020). Genetics of ADHD: What should the clinician know? Current Psychiatry Reports, 22(4), 18. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11920-020-1141-x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Halperin, J. M., & Marks, D. J. (2019). Practitioner review: Assessment and treatment of preschool children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 60(9), 930943. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.13014 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jimenez, M. E., Wade, R. J., Schwartz-Soicher, O., Lin, Y., & Reichman, N. E. (2017). Adverse childhood experiences and ADHD diagnosis at age 9 years in a national urban sample. Academic Pediatrics, 17(4), 356361. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.acap.2016.12.009 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Joseph, H. M., Lorenzo, N. E., Fisher, N., Novick, D. R., Gibson, C., Rothenberger, S. D., Foust, J. E., & Chronis‐Tuscano, A. (2023). Research review: A systematic review and meta-analysis of infant and toddler temperament as predictors of childhood attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 64(5), 715735. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.13753 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jucaite, A., Forssberg, H., Karlsson, P., Halldin, C., & Farde, L. (2010). Age-related reduction in dopamine D1 receptors in the human brain: From late childhood to adulthood, a positron emission tomography study. Neuroscience, 167(1), 104110. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2010.01.034 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kovas, Y., & Plomin, R. (2007). Learning abilities and disabilities: Generalist genes, specialist environments. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 16(5), 284288. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2006.03.004 Google ScholarPubMed
Kuntsi, J., Rijsdijk, F., Ronald, A., Asherson, P., & Plomin, R. (2005). Genetic influences on the stability of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms from early to middle childhood. Biological Psychiatry, 57(6), 647654. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.12.032 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Larsson, H., Chang, Z., D'Onofrio, B. M., & Lichtenstein, P. (2014). The heritability of clinically diagnosed attention deficit hyperactivity disorder across the lifespan. Psychological Medicine, 44(10), 22232229. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291713002493 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Larsson, H., Dilshad, R., Lichtenstein, P., & Barker, E. D. (2011). Developmental trajectories of DSM-IV symptoms of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Genetic effects, family risk and associated psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 52(9), 954963. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2011.02379.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Larsson, J. O., Larsson, H., & Lichtenstein, P. (2004). Genetic and environmental contributions to stability and change of ADHD symptoms between 8 and 13 years of age: A longitudinal twin study. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 43(10), 12671275. https://doi.org/10.1097/01.chi.0000135622.05219.bf CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Law, E. C., Sideridis, G. D., Prock, L. A., & Sheridan, M. A. (2014). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in young children: Predictors of diagnostic stability. Pediatrics, 133(4), 659667. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2013-3433 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leblanc, N., Boivin, M., Dionne, G., Brendgen, M., Vitaro, F., Tremblay, R. E., & Perusse, D. (2008). The development of hyperactive-impulsive behaviors during the preschool years: The predictive validity of parental assessments. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 36(7), 977987. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-008-9227-7 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lehn, H., Derks, E. M., Hudziak, J. J., Heutink, P., van Beijsterveldt, T. C., & Boomsma, D. I. (2007). Attention problems and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in discordant and concordant monozygotic twins: Evidence of environmental mediators. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 46(1), 8391. https://doi.org/10.1097/01.chi.0000242244.00174.d9 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Livingstone, L. T., Coventry, W. L., Corley, R. P., Willcutt, E. G., Samuelsson, S., Olson, R. K., & Byrne, B. (2016). Does the environment have an enduring effect on ADHD? A longitudinal study of monozygotic twin differences in children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 44(8), 14871501. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-016-0145-9 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Loehlin, J. C., Neiderhiser, J. M., & Reiss, D. (2005). Genetic and environmental components of adolescent adjustment and parental behavior: A multivariate analysis. Child Development, 76(5), 11041115. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00900.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mascheretti, S., Trezzi, V., Giorda, R., Boivin, M., Plourde, V., Vitaro, F., Brendgen, M., Dionne, G., & Marino, C. (2017). Complex effects of dyslexia risk factors account for ADHD traits: Evidence from two independent samples. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 58(1), 7582. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12612 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Merikangas, K. R., He, J.-P., Burstein, M., Swanson, S. A., Avenevoli, S., Cui, L., Benjet, C., Georgiades, K., & Swendsen, J. (2010). Lifetime prevalence of mental disorders in U.S. adolescents: Results from the national comorbidity survey replication--adolescent supplement (NCS-A). Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 49(10), 980989. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2010.05.017 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moens, M. A., Weeland, J., Van der Giessen, D., Chhangur, R. R., & Overbeek, G. (2018). In the eye of the beholder? parent-observer discrepancies in parenting and child disruptive behavior assessments. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 46(6), 11471159. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-017-0381-7 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muthén, L. K., & Muthen, B. (2017). Mplus user’s guide (8th ed.). Muthén, L.K.; Muthén, B.O.Google Scholar
Nagin, D. S., & Tremblay, R. E. (2005). Developmental trajectory groups: Fact or a useful statistical fiction? Criminology, 43(4), 873904. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-9125.2005.00026.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Narad, M. E., Garner, A. A., Peugh, J. L., Tamm, L., Antonini, T. N., Kingery, K. M., Simon, J. O., & Epstein, J. N. (2015). Parent-teacher agreement on ADHD symptoms across development. Psychological Assessment, 27(1), 239248. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0037864 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Overgaard, K. R., Oerbeck, B., Friis, S., Pripp, A. H., Aase, H., Biele, G., Ingeborgrud, C. B., Polanczyk, G. V., & Zeiner, Pål (2022). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder from preschool to school age: Change and stability of parent and teacher reports. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 32(10), 19471955. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-022-02019-1 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pettersson, E., Sjolander, A., Almqvist, C., Anckarsater, H., D’Onofrio, B. M., Lichtenstein, P., & Larsson, H. (2015). Birth weight as an independent predictor of ADHD symptoms: A within-twin pair analysis. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 56(4), 453459. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12299 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pingault, J. B., Tremblay, R. E., Vitaro, F., Carbonneau, R., Genolini, C., Falissard, B., & Cote, S. M. (2011). Childhood trajectories of inattention and hyperactivity and prediction of educational attainment in early adulthood: A 16-year longitudinal population-based study. American Journal of Psychiatry, 168(11), 11641170. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2011.10121732 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pingault, J. B., Viding, E., Galera, C., Greven, C. U., Zheng, Y., Plomin, R., & Rijsdijk, F. (2015). Genetic and environmental influences on the developmental course of attention-deficit/Hyperactivity disorder symptoms from childhood to adolescence. JAMA Psychiatry, 72(7), 651658. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2015.0469 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Price, T. S., Simonoff, E., Asherson, P., Curran, S., Kuntsi, J., Waldman, I., & Plomin, R. (2005). Continuity and change in preschool ADHD symptoms: Longitudinal genetic analysis with contrast effects. Behavior Genetics, 35(2), 121132. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10519-004-1013-x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Price, T. S., Simonoff, E., Waldman, I., Asherson, P., & Plomin, R. (2001). Hyperactivity in preschool children is highly heritable. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 40(12), 13621364. https://doi.org/10.1097/00004583-200112000-00002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rietveld, M. J., Hudziak, J. J., Bartels, M., Van Beijsterveldt, C. E. M., & Boomsma, D. I. (2003). Heritability of attention problems in children: I. cross-sectional results from a study of twins, age 3-12 years. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part B: Neuropsychiatric Genetics, 117B(1), 102113. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajmg.b.10024 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rovira, P., Demontis, D., Sánchez-Mora, C., Zayats, T., Klein, M., Mota, N. R., Weber, H., Garcia-Martínez, I., Pagerols, M., Vilar-Ribó, L., Arribas, L., Richarte, V., Corrales, M., Fadeuilhe, C., Bosch, R., Martin, G. E.ñol, Almos, P., Doyle, A. E., … & Ribasés, M. (2020). Shared genetic background between children and adults with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology, 45(10), 16171626. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41386-020-0664-5 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salla, J., Michel, G., Pingault, J. B., Lacourse, E., Paquin, S., Galéra, Cédric, Falissard, B., Boivin, M., Tremblay, R. E., & Côté, S. M. (2016). Childhood trajectories of inattention-hyperactivity and academic achievement at 12 years. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 25(11), 11951206. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-016-0843-4 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
SAS Institute Inc. 2014. SAS software, version [8]. Computer Software.Google Scholar
Sasser, T. R., Kalvin, C. B., & Bierman, K. L. (2016). Developmental trajectories of clinically significant attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) symptoms from grade 3 through 12 in a high-risk sample: Predictors and outcomes. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 125(2), 207219. https://doi.org/10.1037/abn0000112 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schneider, H., Ryan, M., & Mahone, E. M. (2020). Parent versus teacher ratings on the BRIEF-preschool version in children with and without ADHD. Child Neuropsychology, 26(1), 113128. https://doi.org/10.1080/09297049.2019.1617262 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sfelinioti, S., & Livaditis, M. (2017). Association of maternal depression with children’s attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Psychiatriki, 28(3), 251258. https://doi.org/10.22365/jpsych.2017.283.251 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sibley, M. H., Arnold, L. E., Swanson, J. M., Hechtman, L. T., Kennedy, T. M., Owens, E., Molina, B. S. G., Jensen, P. S., Hinshaw, S. P., Roy, A., Chronis-Tuscano, A., Newcorn, J. H., Rohde, L. A., & for the MTA Cooperative Group (2022). Variable patterns of remission from ADHD in the multimodal treatment study of ADHD. American Journal of Psychiatry, 179(2), 142151. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2021.21010032 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sollie, H., Larsson, B., & Mørch, W. T. (2013). Comparison of mother, father, and teacher reports of ADHD core symptoms in a sample of child psychiatric outpatients. Journal of Attention Disorders, 17(8), 699710. https://doi.org/10.1177/1087054711436010 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spitz, E., Moutier, R., Reed, T., Busnel, M. C., Marchaland, C., Roubertoux, P. L., & Carlier, M. (1996). Comparative diagnoses of twin zygosity by SSLP variant analysis, questionnaire, and dermatoglyphic analysis. Behavior Genetics, 26(1), 5563. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02361159 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Subcommittee on Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder, Steering Committee on Quality Improvement and Management (2011). ADHD: Clinical practice guideline for the diagnosis, evaluation, and treatment of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in children and adolescents. Pediatrics, 128(5), 10071022. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2011-2654 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thapar, A., Cooper, M., Jefferies, R., & Stergiakouli, E. (2012). What causes attention deficit hyperactivity disorder? Archives of Disease in Childhood, 97(3), 260265. https://doi.org/10.1136/archdischild-2011-300482 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tikhodeyev, O. N., & Shcherbakova, O. C. (2019). The problem of non-shared environment in behavioral genetics. Behavior Genetics, 49(3), 259269. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10519-019-09950-1 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tobarra-Sanchez, E., Riglin, L., Agha, S. S., Stergiakouli, E., Thapar, A., & Langley, K. (2022). Preschool development, temperament and genetic liability as early markers of childhood ADHD: A cohort study. JCPP Advances, 2(3), e12099. https://doi.org/10.1002/jcv2.12099 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vergunst, F., Tremblay, R. E., Galera, C., Nagin, D., Vitaro, F., Boivin, M., & Cote, S. M. (2019). Multi-rater developmental trajectories of hyperactivity-impulsivity and inattention symptoms from 1.5 to 17 years: A population-based birth cohort study. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 28(7), 973983. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-018-1258-1 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Visser, S. N., Danielson, M. L., Bitsko, R. H., Holbrook, J. R., Kogan, M. D., Ghandour, R. M., Perou, R., & Blumberg, S. J. (2014). Trends in the parent-report of health care provider-diagnosed and medicated attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: United states, 2003-2011. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 53(1), 3446.e2. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2013.09.001 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Walton, E., Pingault, J. B., Cecil, C. A., Gaunt, T. R., Relton, C. L., Mill, J., & Barker, E. D. (2017). Epigenetic profiling of ADHD symptoms trajectories: A prospective, methylome-wide study. Molecular Psychiatry, 22(2), 250256. https://doi.org/10.1038/mp.2016.85 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wigg, K. G., Feng, Y., Crosbie, J., Tannock, R., Kennedy, J. L., Ickowicz, A., Malone, M., Schachar, R., & Barr, C. L. (2008). Association of ADHD and the protogenin gene in the chromosome 15q21.3 reading disabilities linkage region. Genes, Brain and Behavior, 7(8), 877886. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1601-183X.2008.00425.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Willcutt, E. G., Doyle, A. E., Nigg, J. T., Faraone, S. V., & Pennington, B. F. (2005). Validity of the executive function theory of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A meta-analytic review. Biol Psychiatry, 57(11), 13361346. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.02.006 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Willoughby, M. T., Pek, J., Greenberg, M. T., & Family Life Project Investigators (2012). Parent-reported attention deficit/Hyperactivity symptomatology in premiddle-schoold children: Factor structure, developmental change, and early risk factors. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 40(8), 13011312. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-012-9641-8 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wolford, E., Lahti, M., Tuovinen, S., Lahti, J., Lipsanen, J., Savolainen, K., Heinonen, K., Hämäläinen, E, Kajantie, E., Pesonen, A.-K.,  Villa, P.M., Laivuori, H., Reynolds, R. M., & Räikkönen, K. (2017). Maternal depressive symptoms during and after pregnancy are associated with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms in their 3-to 6-year-old children. PLoS One, 12(12). https://doi.org/10.1037/ort0000365 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wood, A. C., Buitelaar, J., Rijsdijk, F., Asherson, P., & Kuntsi, J. (2010). Rethinking shared environment as a source of variance underlying attention-defcit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms: Comment on Burt, 2009. Psychological Bulletin, 136(3), 331340. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0019048 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wu, J., Xiao, H., Sun, H., Zou, L., & Zhu, L. (2012). Role of dopamine receptors in ADHD: A systematic meta-analysis. Molecular Neurobiology, 45(3), 605620. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12035-012-8278-5 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Young, J. R., Yanagihara, A., Dew, R., & Kollins, S. H. (2021). Pharmacotherapy for preschool children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): Current status and future directions. CNS Drugs, 35(4), 403424. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40263-021-00806-z CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Homotypic correlations for hyperactivity/impulsivity (below diagonal) and inattention (above diagonal). Correlations between preschool and later measures are highlighted. Adjacent time-point correlations appear in bold

Figure 1

Table 2. Means (SD) for the total sample, by sex, zygosity, and age for hyperactivity/impulsivity (top) and inattention (bottom) by age

Figure 2

Figure 1. Trajectories of hyperactivity/impulsivity (top panel) and inattention (bottom panel) from 6 to 17 years. Dotted lines illustrate estimated values; bold lines illustrate observed values.

Figure 3

Table 3. Binary logistic regression models predicting high decreasing trajectories of HI (top) and IN (bottom) from 1.5, 2.5, 4, and 5-year parent reports of HI and IN

Figure 4

Table 4. MZ and DZ intraclass correlations (ICC) for hyperactivity/impulsivity (left) and inattention (right) by age

Figure 5

Table 5. Comparisons of Cholesky model fits for HI and IN. The best fitting models appear in bold

Figure 6

Table 6. standardized parameter estimates from Cholesky model for HI and IN of additive genetic (A) and nonshared environment (E) for preschool measures to subsequent measures. Significant parameters based on 95% confidence intervals are presented in bold

Supplementary material: File

Dionne et al. supplementary material

Dionne et al. supplementary material
Download Dionne et al. supplementary material(File)
File 54.9 KB