Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-g7gxr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-13T02:26:08.263Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Decreased sensitivity to antidepressant drugs in Wistar Hannover rats submitted to two animal models of depression

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  14 September 2022

Kennia M. Silveira
Affiliation:
Department of Biomolecular Sciences, School of Pharmaceutical Sciences of Ribeirão Preto, University of São Paulo, Ribeirão Preto, SP, Brazil Translational Neuropsychiatry Unit, Department of Clinical Medicine, Aarhus University, Aarhus, Denmark
Ariandra G. Sartim
Affiliation:
Department of Biomolecular Sciences, School of Pharmaceutical Sciences of Ribeirão Preto, University of São Paulo, Ribeirão Preto, SP, Brazil
Letícia Vieira
Affiliation:
Department of Biomolecular Sciences, School of Pharmaceutical Sciences of Ribeirão Preto, University of São Paulo, Ribeirão Preto, SP, Brazil
Sabrina F. Lisboa
Affiliation:
Department of Biomolecular Sciences, School of Pharmaceutical Sciences of Ribeirão Preto, University of São Paulo, Ribeirão Preto, SP, Brazil
Gregers Wegener
Affiliation:
Translational Neuropsychiatry Unit, Department of Clinical Medicine, Aarhus University, Aarhus, Denmark
Sâmia R. L. Joca*
Affiliation:
Department of Biomolecular Sciences, School of Pharmaceutical Sciences of Ribeirão Preto, University of São Paulo, Ribeirão Preto, SP, Brazil Department of Biomedicine, Aarhus University, Aarhus, Denmark
*
Author for correspondence: Sâmia R. L. Joca, Email: sjoca@biomed.au.dk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

The Wistar Hannover rat (WHR) is a strain commonly used for toxicity studies but rarely used in studies investigating depression neurobiology. In this study, we aimed to characterise the behavioural responses of WHR to acute and repeated antidepressant treatments upon exposure to the forced swim test (FST) or learned helplessness (LH) test. WHR were subjected to forced swimming pre-test and test with antidepressant administration (imipramine, fluoxetine, or escitalopram) at 0, 5 h and 23 h after pre-test. WHR displayed high immobility in the test compared to unstressed controls (no pre-swim) and failed to respond to the antidepressants tested. The effect of acute and repeated treatment (imipramine, fluoxetine, escitalopram or s-ketamine) was then tested in animals not previously exposed to pre-test. Only imipramine (20 mg/kg, 7 days) and s-ketamine (acute) reduced the immobility time in the test. To further investigate the possibility that the WHR were less responsive to selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors, the effect of repeated treatment with fluoxetine (20 mg/kg, 7 days) was investigated in the LH model. The results demonstrated that fluoxetine failed to reduce the number of escape failures in two different protocols. These data suggest that the WHR do not respond to the conventional antidepressant treatment in the FST or the LH. Only s-ketamine and repeated imipramine were effective in WHR in a modified FST protocol. Altogether, these results indicate that WHR may be an interesting tool to investigate the mechanisms associated with the resistance to antidepressant drugs and identify more effective treatments.

Type
Original Article
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of Scandinavian College of Neuropsychopharmacology

Significant outcomes

  • Acute and subchronic administration of selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors, fluoxetine and escitalopram, failed to promote antidepressant-like effects in WHR submitted to the FST with and without pre-test session.

  • Subchronic imipramine and acute S-ketamine induced antidepressant-like effects in WHR submitted to the FST without a pre-test session.

  • Repeated treatment with fluoxetine and imipramine failed to promote antidepressant-like effects in WHR submitted to the learned helplessness model.

Limitations

  • We did not investigate the effects of the drugs when injected via different routes of administration and variable time-schedule to assess possible pharmacokinetic changes in WHR.

  • We did not test the effect of combining different treatments to investigate if there would be any possible facilitation of the antidepressant effect, as observed in patients showing resistance to monotherapy.

  • We did not investigate the behaviour of female WHR or compared this strain with other Wistar rats.

Introduction

Science has been facing a "reproducibility crisis" in the last few years, consisting of difficulties in replicating clinical and preclinical studies (Begley & Ioannidis, Reference Begley and Ioannidis2015; Hunter, Reference Hunter2017; Fanelli, Reference Fanelli2018). Many factors are being seen as causal, such as the absence of internal and external validity, lack of complete dataset information, and under-powered designs (Olorisade et al., Reference Olorisade, Brereton and Andras2017; Fanelli, Reference Fanelli2018; Pound and Ritskes-Hoitinga, Reference Pound and Ritskes-Hoitinga2018), as well as poor reporting of study results (Avey et al., 2016). Besides that, in preclinical studies, other factors seem to correlate with the lack of reliability, such as the strain, sex and origin of the animals, the genotype, the season of the year and time of day when experiments were performed, the cage density, among others (Chesler et al., Reference Chesler, Wilson, Lariviere, Rodriguez-Zas and Mogil2002; Tillmann, Reference Tillmann2017; Einat et al., Reference Einat, Ezer, Kara and Belzung2018). Therefore, it is fundamentally essential to in-depth characterise the behaviour of a given strain in different animal models, try to reduce the reproducibility deficit and comply with the 3R principles (reduction, replacement and refinement) (Russel & Burch, Reference Russel and Burch1959).

Outbred animals are commonly used in the studies to evaluate or modulate the human population for presenting a genetic variability, being widely used in the investigation of depression neurobiology and treatment (Chia et al., Reference Chia, Achilli, Festing and Fisher2005; Lohmiller & Swing, Reference Lohmiller and Swing2006). The Wistar Hannover rats (WHR), also known as Wistar Han or Han Wistar, were originated in 1964, at Hannover Institute (Germany), after selected breeding from "regular" Wistar rats based on specific phenotypic characteristics, which have not been described in details (Giknis & Clifford, Reference Giknis and Clifford2009). These animals are more commonly used for toxicity studies but are also used in oncology studies, safety and efficacy tests, and a few behavioural studies (Rockett et al., Reference Rockett, Narotsky, Thompson, Thillainadarajah, Blystone, Goetz, Ren, Best, Murrell, Nichols, Schmid, Wolf and Dix2006; Hayakawa et al., Reference Hayakawa, Mimura, Tachibana, Furuya, Kodama, Aoki, Hosokawa, Fukui, Shibata, Yoshida, Masuyama, Narita, Kuwagata, Hisada and Maki2013; Noritake et al., Reference Noritake, Ikeda, Ito, Miwa and Senuma2013; Charles River Laboratories International, 2015; Soares-Cunha et al., Reference Soares-Cunha, Coimbra, Borges, Domingues, Silva, Sousa and Rodrigues2018; Gauvin et al., Reference Gauvin, Dalton, Harter, Holdsworth, May, Tapp, Zimmermann, Kilgus and Baird2019). When exposed to chronic mild stress (CMS), WHR displayed reduced body weight and locomotor activity in the open field test (OFT), increased grooming, and reduced sucrose consumption, characterising an anhedonia-like behaviour (Theilmann et al., Reference Theilmann, Kleimann, Rhein, Bleich, Frieling, Löscher and Brandt2016). In the FST, Kawai and colleagues (2018) showed that both acute and chronic imipramine (30 mg/kg) induced an antidepressant-like effect, although with varying responses depending on the injection time. Other studies have used the WHR in antidepressant research without characterising their response to different antidepressant drugs (Kawai et al., Reference Kawai, Kodaira, Tanaka, Ishibashi, Kudo, Kawashima and Mitsumoto2018).

Given the current establishment of the WHR colony in our campus and the lack of detailed information regarding their response to antidepressants, we evaluated the behavioural and the antidepressant response of WHR in two widely used animal models of depression: the forced swim test (FST) and the learned helplessness paradigm (LH). Both FST and LH are extensively used in the screening of new antidepressant drugs and investigation of their mechanisms of action (Cryan et al., Reference Cryan, Markou and Lucki2002; Pryce et al., Reference Pryce, Azzinnari, Spinelli, Seifritz, Tegethoff and Meinlschmidt2011; Slattery & Cryan, Reference Slattery and Cryan2014; Kara et al., Reference Kara, Stukalin and Einat2018; Ribeiro et al., Reference Ribeiro, Casarotto, Staquini, Silva, Biojone, Wegener and Joca2019). Porsolt et al. (Reference Porsolt, Le and Jalfre1977) originally described that the FST is based on the animal’s pre-exposure to a stressful event, the forced swim (pre-test). In the test session, usually performed 24 h after the pre-test, the animal initially presents intensive struggling to escape from stress, followed by an immobile behaviour, which is reduced by acute/subacute treatment (3× in 24 h) with different classes of antidepressant drugs (Porsolt et al., Reference Porsolt, Le and Jalfre1977, Reference Porsolt, Bertin and Jalfre1978b; Cryan & Lucki, Reference Cryan and Lucki2000). The FST has low cost, easy implementation and relatively good reproducibility. Therefore, it is one of the most widely used behavioural readouts in the study of depression neurobiology and searches for new treatments (Cryan et al., Reference Cryan, Markou and Lucki2002, Reference Cryan, Valentino and Lucki2005; Abelaira et al., Reference Abelaira, Réus and Quevedo2013; Slattery & Cryan, Reference Slattery and Cryan2014; Wang et al., Reference Wang, Timberlake, Prall and Dwivedi2017).

In the LH, the rodents are subjected to inescapable and unpredictable shocks that lead to cognitive deficits characterised by an increased number of escape failures presented in during the subsequent session with escapable aversive stimuli (test) (Joca et al., Reference Joca, Padovan and Guimarães2003, Reference Joca, Zanelati and Guimarães2006; Pryce et al., Reference Pryce, Azzinnari, Spinelli, Seifritz, Tegethoff and Meinlschmidt2011; Maier & Seligman, Reference Maier and Seligman2016). This model’s relevance is due to its good face and construct validity (Vollmayr & Gass, Reference Vollmayr and Gass2013). Exposure to inescapable aversive stimuli leads to behavioural and pathophysiological changes that resemble those observed in patients with depression (Pryce et al., Reference Pryce, Azzinnari, Spinelli, Seifritz, Tegethoff and Meinlschmidt2011; Ribeiro et al., Reference Ribeiro, Casarotto, Staquini, Silva, Biojone, Wegener and Joca2019). In addition, LH meets the predictive validity criteria since helplessness behaviour can be reversed by chronic treatment with antidepressants, while treatment with anxiolytics, neuroleptics, stimulants or depressants of the central nervous system does not affect behaviour in this test (Sherman et al., Reference Sherman, Sacquitne and Petty1982).

Differences in strains of mice and rats can be a source of considerable variability in the FST results (Porsolt et al., Reference Porsolt, Bertin and Jalfre1978b; Lahmanme & Armario, Reference Lahmanme and Armario1996; Lucki et al., Reference Lucki, Dalvi and Mayorga2001; Will et al., Reference Will, Aird and Redei2003; Jin et al., Reference Jin, Chen, Ran, Li, Xiong, Zheng, Gao and Li2017). For instance, Will et al. (Reference Will, Aird and Redei2003) demonstrated that Wistar-Kyoto (WKY) rats are more immobile than Wistar, Fisher 344 and Sprague-Dawley (SD) rats in FST (Will et al., Reference Will, Aird and Redei2003). The discrepant behaviour of different mice strains on FST can modify the response of these animals to the different antidepressant classes (Porsolt et al., Reference Porsolt, Bertin and Jalfre1978b; Lucki et al., Reference Lucki, Dalvi and Mayorga2001; Jin et al., Reference Jin, Chen, Ran, Li, Xiong, Zheng, Gao and Li2017). Moreover, some rat strains present ‘endogenous phenotypes’ associated with depression or resistance to treatment, such as the Flinders Sensitive Line (FSL) and the WKY rats, respectively (Paré, Reference Paré1994; Lahmanme & Armario, Reference Lahmanme and Armario1996; Overstreet et al., Reference Overstreet, Friedman, Mathé and Yadid2005; Overstreet & Wegener, Reference Overstreet and Wegener2013). In such cases, pre-exposure to the pre-test session is not necessary to reveal the antidepressant effect of drugs (Lahmame et al., Reference Lahmame, Del, Pazos, Yritia and Armario1997; Overstreet & Wegener, Reference Overstreet and Wegener2013). Although there is less evidence than with the FST, the strain also seems to influence behaviour in the LH. Wieland et al. (Reference Wieland, Boren, Consroe and Martin1986) demonstrated that the inbred WKY strain and SD rats from Charles River Holtzman are more susceptible to LH training than other strains or suppliers, which may influence both strain and suppliers helplessness behaviour.

In light of insufficient evidence regarding the response of WHR to antidepressants and the increasing use of such strain in psychopharmacology studies, we aimed to characterise the behaviour of WHR in two different protocols of FST and LH. For FST, we used a similar version of Porsolt’s protocol, with pre-test (15 min) and test (5 min), with 24-h interval (Porsolt et al., Reference Porsolt, Le and Jalfre1977; Detke et al., Reference Detke, Rickels and Lucki1995), and a single session of FST (10 min, no pre-swim, as previously described for identifying the behavioural effect of antidepressant drugs in animals with increased vulnerability to stress, such as the FSL animals) (Schiller et al., Reference Schiller, Pucilowski, Wienicke and Overstreet1992a; Pucilowski & Overstreet, Reference Pucilowski and Overstreet1993; Overstreet et al., Reference Overstreet, Friedman, Mathé and Yadid2005). Concerning the LH, we used: 1. a traditional protocol consisting of two sessions: pre-test (inescapable shocks for 40 min) and test (30 escapable shocks and preceded by a warning tone) (Joca et al., Reference Joca, Padovan and Guimarães2003, Reference Joca, Zanelati and Guimarães2006; Stanquini et al., Reference Stanquini, Biojone, Guimarães and Joca2017); 2. an adapted protocol with three sessions: pre-test (inescapable shocks for 40 min), test 1 and test 2 (both with 30 escapable shocks preceded by a warning tone), which considers the animals’ response pattern to stress (Reinés et al., Reference Reinés, Cereseto, Ferrero, Sifonios, Podestá and Wikinski2008; Macedo et al., Reference Macedo, Cladouchos, Sifonios, Cassaneli and Wikinski2013). Moreover, we investigated the response of WHR to antidepressants of different pharmacological classes after single and repeated treatment.

Experimental procedures

Animals and housing

Male WHR [Crl:WI(Han)] with 6–7 weeks (200–250 g), purchased from the local farm facility of the University of São Paulo, Campus of Ribeirão Preto, were used. The animals were brought to the animal house of the Laboratory of Pharmacology (School of Pharmaceutical Sciences) and remained undisturbed for at least 1 week before the start of the experiments. Animals were housed in a controlled temperature room (24 ± 1°C), under a 12-h light/dark cycles (lights on at 6 a.m.) in pairs in acrylic boxes (20 × 30 × 13 cm) with wood bedding (sawdust), no environmental enrichment, with ad libitum access to food (commercial rodent chow, Nuvilab – Quimtia – Paraná, Brazil) and tap water. In the LH, after the pre-test session, the rats were kept isolated in individual acrylic boxes (20 × 30 × 13 cm). The bedding was changed three times per week. All the efforts were made to reduce the suffering of the animals during the experiments, and all the procedures were conducted in conformity with the National Council for the Control of Animal Experimentation (CONCEA), complying with international laws and politics. The local Ethics Committee (University of São Paulo, Campus of Ribeirão Preto) approved all the experimental protocols (19.1.582.60.3 and 19.1.248.60.6). The total number of animals used in the present study was 305.

Drugs

The following drugs were as follows: imipramine (Abcam, USA or Sigma Aldrich, #I7379), a tricyclic antidepressant, at the doses 5, 10, 15 and 20 mg/kg; fluoxetine (Prati-Donaduzzi, BR), a selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor (SSRI), at the doses of 5, 10 and 20 mg/kg; escitalopram (Prati-Donaduzzi, BR), an SSRI, at the doses of 3, 10 and 20 mg/kg; and s-ketamine (Cristália, BR), a dissociative anaesthetic with fast-acting antidepressant effects (Berman et al., Reference Berman, Cappiello, Anand, Oren, Heninger, Charney and Krystal2000; Murrough et al., Reference Murrough, Iosifescu, Chang, Al Jurdi, Green, Perez, Pillemer, Foulkes and Mathew2013), at the dose of 10 mg/kg. All drugs were freshly diluted in saline 0,9%, excepted fluoxetine, which was dissolved on tween 80 and 90% saline 0,9%. The drugs and their respective vehicle were systemically administered (i.p.) in a volume of 1 ml/kg. The doses used were based on previously published studies and preliminary data from our group (Porsolt et al., Reference Porsolt, Anton, Blavet and Jalfre1978a; Zazpe et al., Reference Zazpe, Artaiz, Labeaga, Lucero and Orjales2007; Reinés et al., Reference Reinés, Cereseto, Ferrero, Sifonios, Podestá and Wikinski2008; France et al., Reference France, Li, Owens, Koek, Toney and Daws2009; Macedo et al., Reference Macedo, Cladouchos, Sifonios, Cassaneli and Wikinski2013; Fernández-guasti et al., Reference Fernández-guasti, Olivares-nazario, Reyes and Martínez-mota2017; Kawai et al., Reference Kawai, Kodaira, Tanaka, Ishibashi, Kudo, Kawashima and Mitsumoto2018).

Behavioural experiments

Animals were brought to the experimental room for acclimatisation 30 min before the start of the experiments. All experiments were performed between 8 a.m. and 2 p.m for FST and between 8 a.m. and 4 p.m for LH. All rooms had standard conditions of illumination (lights on from 6 a.m. to 6 p.m.), ventilation and temperature (23 ± 1C) throughout the experiment. The animals were in blocks randomly assigned to the different experimental conditions. Similarly, the animals were also randomly assigned to testing to avoid potential circadian bias.

To characterise the behaviour and response to antidepressant drugs, independent groups of WHR were submitted to two different protocols of FST and LH.

FST: (a) with pre-swimming session 24 h before the test (Fig. 1a); (b) without pre-swimming (Fig. 2a). All FST experiments were recorded using a Sony video camera (model DCR-SR47), and data were analysed by an experimenter blind to treatment conditions.

Fig. 1. Effect of imipramine, fluoxetine and escitalopram in WHR submitted to forced swim test with pre-swim (b, d) and open field test (OFT) (c, e). (a) Experimental design of forced swim test with pre-swim: the animals were submitted to the pre-test (PT) section of forced swim and, immediately after, received the first i.p. injection (0 h), the second and third injection were administrated 5 h and 23 h after PT, respectively. One hour after the last injection, the WHR were submitted to OFT and FST. The bars represent mean ± standard error (one-way ANOVA followed by Dunnett’s post hoc test or Student’s t-test, *p < 0.05 compared to the vehicle group (Veh), n = 6–10 per group).

Fig. 2. Effects of the acute and repeated administration of fluoxetine (20 mg/kg), imipramine (20 mg/kg) and s-ketamine (10 mg/kg) in rats submitted to forced swimming test (FST) without pre-swim and open field test (OFT). (a) Experimental design of forced swim test without pre-swim: the animals were treated systematically during 7 days with antidepressant or vehicle (subchronic treatment) or received the vehicle for 6 days and received the drugs only on the last day (acute treatment). On the seventh day, the OFT and FST were performed 1 h after the last treatment. (b) Immobility time during 10 min of FST; (c) locomotor activity during 10 min of OFT; (d) immobility time in the first 5 min of FST; and (e) immobility time in the last 5 min of FST. The bars represent mean ± standard error (one-way ANOVA followed by Dunnett’s post hoc test, *p < 0.05 compared to the vehicle group (Veh), n = 8–10 per group).

LH: (a) with pre-test and test, described previously by Joca et al. (Reference Joca, Padovan and Guimarães2003, Reference Joca, Zanelati and Guimarães2006) and Stanquini et al. (Reference Stanquini, Biojone, Guimarães and Joca2017) (Fig. 4a); (b) with three sessions, pre-test, test 1 and test 2, (Figs. 5a and 6a). All parameters analysed (number of failures, escapes, avoidances or intertrial crossings) were recorded by Esquiva Ativa software, which also controls the shocks release, intervals and sounds during the sessions.

Fig. 3. Effects of the acute and repeated administration of escitalopram (20 mg/kg) and s-ketamine (10 mg/kg) in rats submitted to forced swim test (FST) without pre-swim and open field test (OFT). (a) Experimental design of FST without pre-swim; (b) immobility time during 10 min of FST; (c) locomotor activity during 10 min of OFT; (d) immobility time in the first 5 min of FST; and (e) immobility time in the last 5 min of FST. The bars represent mean ± standard error (one-way ANOVA followed by Dunnett’s post hoc test, *p < 0.05 compared to the vehicle group (Veh), n = 9–10 per group).

Fig. 4. Effects of fluoxetine (20 mg/kg) at number of failures, avoidances, escapes and intertrial crossings of Wistar Hannover rats subjected to learned helplessness (traditional protocol). (a) Experimental design used for LH traditional protocol. The animals were divided into non-stressed and stressed. On the first day of the experiment (pre-test), the non-stressed animals were just exposed to the shuttle box for 40 min without shocks, while the stressed ones were subjected to 40 inescapable shocks (0.6 mA, 10 s duration, 30–90 s interval). The animals were treated once daily for 7 days with vehicle (sterile saline) or fluoxetine 20 mg/kg/mL and 1 h after the last injection was tested with 30 escape shocks (0.6 mA, 10 s duration, 30–90 s interval) preceded by a warning tone (60 dB, 670 Hz, 5 s). (b) Number of failures. (c) Number of avoidance. (d) Number of escape. (e) Number of intertrial crossings. NST-VEH (non-stressed vehicle), ST-VEH (stressed-vehicle), ST-FLX (stressed-fluoxetine 20 mg/kg/mL). Values are expressed as mean ± SEM. Kruskal–Wallis test followed by Dunn’s test indicated that there was no statistical difference between the groups.

Fig. 5. Effects of fluoxetine (20 mg/kg) at number of failures, avoidances, escapes and intertrial crossings of Wistar Hannover rats subjected to learned helplessness (adapted protocol). (a) Experimental design used for adapted LH protocol. The animals were divided into non-stressed and stressed. On the first day of the experiment (pre-test), the non-stressed animals were just exposed to the shuttle box for 40 min without shocks, while the stressed ones were subjected to 40 inescapable shocks (0.6 mA, 10 s duration, 30–90 s interval). Twenty-four hours later, the stressed animals were subjected to test 1 (T1) (0.6 mA, 10 s duration, 30–90 s interval) and classified according to their number of failures in resilient (0–10 failures) or susceptible (15–30 failures). The non-stressed group was again just exposed to the shuttle box without shocks. The animals groups were treated once daily for 7 days with: non-stressed (sterile saline); stressed-resilient (sterile saline), stressed-susceptible-vehicle (sterile saline) and stressed-susceptible-fluoxetine (20 mg/kg/mL). One hour after the last administration, the animals were exposed to test 2 (0.6 mA, 10 s duration, 30–90 s interval) preceded by a warning tone (60 dB, 670 Hz, 5 s). (b) Number of failures after T1. (c) Number of avoidance after T1. (d) Number of escapes after T1. (e) Number of intertrial crossings after T1. Data are expressed as mean ± SEM. The Mann–Whitney test indicated a significant difference in the number of failures, avoidances and escapes between resilient (n = 6) and susceptible (n = 22) animals. *p < 0.05. (f) Number of failures after T2. (g) Number of avoidance after T2. (h) Number of escapes after T2. (i) Number of intertrial crossings after T2. NST-VEH (non-stressed vehicle), ST-RESIL (stressed-resilient), ST-SUSC-VEH (stressed-susceptible-vehicle), ST-SUSC FLX (stressed-susceptible-fluoxetine). Data are expressed as mean ± SEM. The Kruskal–Wallis test followed by Dunn’s test indicated that there was no significant difference in the number of failures between the ST-SUSC VEH × ST-SUSC FLX groups. *p < 0.05. n = 6–11 per group.

Fig. 6. Effects of imipramine (20 mg/kg) at number of failures, avoidances, escapes and intertrial crossings of Wistar Hannover rats subjected to learned helplessness (adapted protocol). (a) Experimental design used for adapted LH protocol. The animals were divided into non-stressed and stressed. On the first day of the experiment (pre-test), the non-stressed animals were just exposed to the shuttle box for 40 min without shocks, while the stressed ones were subjected to 40 inescapable shocks (0.6 mA, 10 s duration, 30–90 s interval). Twenty-four hours later, the stressed animals were subjected to test 1 (T1) (0.6 mA, 10 s duration, 30–90 s interval) and classified according to their number of failures in resilient (0–10 failures) or susceptible (15–30 failures). The non-stressed group was again just exposed to the shuttle box without shocks. The animals groups were treated once daily for 7 days with: non-stressed (sterile saline); stressed-resilient (sterile saline), stressed-susceptible-vehicle (sterile saline) and stressed-susceptible-fluoxetine (20 mg/kg/mL). One hour after the last administration, the animals were exposed to test 2 (0.6 mA, 10 s duration, 30–90 s interval) preceded by a warning tone (60 dB, 670 Hz, 5 s). (b) Number of failures after T1. (c) Number of avoidance after T1. (d) Number of escapes after T1. (e) Number of intertrial crossings after T1. Data are expressed as mean ± SEM. The Mann– Whitney test indicated a significant difference in the number of failures, avoidances, escapes and crossings between resilient (n = 19) and susceptible (n = 29) animals. *p < 0.05. (f) Number of failures after T2. (g) Number of avoidance after T2. (h) Number of escape after T2. (i) Number of intertrial crossings after T2. NST-VEH (non-stressed vehicle), ST-RESIL (stressed-resilient), ST-SUSC-VEH (stressed-susceptible-vehicle), ST-SUSC IMIP (stressed-susceptible-imipramine). Data are expressed as mean ± SEM. The Kruskal–Wallis test followed by Dunn’s test indicated that there was no significant difference in the number of failures between the ST-SUSC VEH × ST-SUSC IMIP groups. *p < 0.05. n = 10–19 per group.

(a) FST with pre-swim. It was performed according to a modified Porsolt’s protocol (Porsolt et al., Reference Porsolt, Le and Jalfre1977; Detke et al., Reference Detke, Rickels and Lucki1995; Cryan et al., Reference Cryan, Valentino and Lucki2005), with adaptations described in Diniz et al. (Reference Diniz, Casarotto, Fred, Biojone, Castrén and Joca2018). Briefly, the protocol consisted of placing the animals to individually swim in a plastic cylinder (30 cm in diameter, 40 cm in height, containing 30 cm of water, with a temperature of 24 ± 1°C, for 15 min – pre-test, PT). After this period, the animals were removed and placed separately to dry before returning to their original boxes. Twenty-four hours later, the animals were submitted to the OFT (see below) and, immediately after, to a 5-min forced swimming session (test, T). The total immobility time was analysed by an experimenter unaware of the treatments. The animal was considered immobile when it remained still performing only small movements necessary for flotation. In the test session, the water was changed after each animal to avoid the influence of possible alarm substances. The treatment protocol consisted of three injections in an interval of 24 h: the first injection was administered immediately after PT, the second at the end of the day (5 p.m.) and the third injection 1 h before the OFT.

(b) FST without pre-swim. This protocol was adapted from the protocol used for testing the effect of antidepressants in FSL rats, characterised by higher vulnerability to stress, and no need to be exposed to pre-test to reveal the antidepressant effect of drugs (Schiller et al., Reference Schiller, Pucilowski, Wienicke and Overstreet1992a; Overstreet et al., Reference Overstreet, Friedman, Mathé and Yadid2005; Sales et al., Reference Sales, Fogaça, Sartim, Pereira, Wegener, Guimarães and Joca2018). The apparatus and conditions were the same described above; however, rats were submitted to a single session of OFT and FST, with 10 min of duration each. The water was changed after each test. The animals were treated daily for 7 days, and the test was performed 1 h after the last injection. The acutely treated group received the drug only on the last day, 1 h before the OFT, and vehicle on the other 6 days; the repeatedly treated group received the drugs (imipramine, fluoxetine or escitalopram) or vehicle during the 7 days of treatment.

Open field test (OFT). Before the FST, the rats were submitted to the OFT to investigate any possible influence of the drugs on locomotor activity. The WHR were placed individually into a wood square arena (with dimensions 72 × 72 × 40 cm high) for 5 or 10 min before FST with and without pre-swim, respectively. The locomotor activity was recorded and evaluated using AnyMaze software (Stoelting, United Kingdom).

(c) Learned helplessness (LH, protocol 1). The experiments were carried out in a shuttle box (37 × 33 × 54 cm, manufactured by Insight), with a metal grid on the floor, which delivers foot shocks. This apparatus consists of two compartments of equal sizes separated by a wall with a central opening through which the animals could cross from one compartment to another (Joca et al., Reference Joca, Padovan and Guimarães2003, Reference Joca, Zanelati and Guimarães2006; Ribeiro et al., Reference Ribeiro, Ferraz-de-Paula, Pinheiro and Palermo-Neto2009; Stanquini et al., Reference Stanquini, Biojone, Guimarães and Joca2017). The WHR were exposed to a pre-test (PT) session consisting of 40 inescapable foot shocks (0.6 mA, 10 s duration, 30–90 s interval) and then treated daily for 7 days with fluoxetine or vehicle until the test (T) that was performed 1 h after the last injection. At this session, the rats were submitted to 30 escapable foot shocks (0.4 mA, 10 s, 30–90 s interval), preceded (5 s) by a warning tone (60 dB, 670 Hz). In the test session, animals could avoid the shocks by crossing from one compartment to the other during tone presentation or escape by interrupting the shock when crossing the shuttle box during shock presentation. An escape failure was defined as the absence of avoidance or escape (Ribeiro et al., Reference Ribeiro, Ferraz-de-Paula, Pinheiro and Palermo-Neto2009; Stanquini et al., Reference Stanquini, Biojone, Guimarães and Joca2017). Repeated antidepressant treatments, such as fluoxetine, decrease the number of escape failures (Sherman et al., Reference Sherman, Sacquitne and Petty1982; Reinés et al., Reference Reinés, Cereseto, Ferrero, Sifonios, Podestá and Wikinski2008; Macedo et al., Reference Macedo, Cladouchos, Sifonios, Cassaneli and Wikinski2013). Therefore, escape failures were used in the present work as a parameter indicative of LH behaviour. The number of intertrial crossings was used as a parameter indicative of locomotor activity (Geoffroy & Christensen, Reference Geoffroy and Christensen1993). The animals were divided into two groups: stressed and non-stressed. The stressed group was submitted to both PT and T sessions. The non-stressed group was exposed to the shuttle box during the PT session without shocks, while the T session was conducted as usual.

(d) Learned helplessness (LH, protocol 2). This protocol was developed considering that, like humans, rats show significant heterogeneity in their response to stress and may be susceptible or resilient (resistant) (Reinés et al., Reference Reinés, Cereseto, Ferrero, Bonavita and Wikinski2004; Russo et al., Reference Russo, Murrough, Han, Charney and Nestler2012; Drugan et al., Reference Drugan, Christianson, Warner and Kent2013; Wood & Bhatnagar, Reference Wood and Bhatnagar2015; Febbraro et al., Reference Febbraro, Svenningsen, Tran and Wiborg2017). Rats were submitted to the same apparatus and conditions described above; however, they were submitted to three sessions, PT, T1 and T2. First, they were exposed to a pre-test that consisted of 40 inescapable foot shocks (0.6 mA, 10 s duration, 30–90 s interval). In this session, the non-stressed group was exposed to the shuttle box without shocks. Twenty-four hours later, the animals were subjected to T1 and, after this session, classified according to the number of failures: resilient (0–10 failures) or susceptible (15–30 failures). Only the susceptible animals were treated daily for 7 days with the antidepressants (fluoxetine or imipramine) until T2, which was performed 1 h after the last injection. At T1 and T2, 30 shocks (0.6 mA) were delivered, with the same duration and intervals as the PT session, but the shocks were escapable and preceded by a warning tone (60 dB, 670 Hz, 5 s) (Joca et al., Reference Joca, Padovan and Guimarães2003, Reference Joca, Zanelati and Guimarães2006; Reinés et al., Reference Reinés, Cereseto, Ferrero, Sifonios, Podestá and Wikinski2008; Macedo et al., Reference Macedo, Cladouchos, Sifonios, Cassaneli and Wikinski2013; Stanquini et al., Reference Stanquini, Biojone, Guimarães and Joca2017; Ribeiro et al., Reference Ribeiro, Casarotto, Staquini, Silva, Biojone, Wegener and Joca2019). The escape failures number was used as a parameter indicative of LH behaviour, and the number of intertrial crossings was used as a parameter of locomotor activity (Geoffroy & Christensen, Reference Geoffroy and Christensen1993).

Experimental design

Experiment 1: Effects of imipramine, fluoxetine and escitalopram in WHR submitted to the OFT and FST with pre-swim

This experiment aimed to investigate the behavioural response of WHR to antidepressants of different pharmacological classes in the FST under standardised experimental conditions. The animals received three systemic injections (0, 5 h and 23 h after PT) of imipramine (5, 10, 15 and 20 mg/kg), fluoxetine (3 and 10 mg/kg), escitalopram (3 and 10 mg/kg) or vehicle (1 mL/kg). The animals were submitted to the OFT 1 h after the last injection, followed by the FST immediately after.

Experiment 2: Effects of imipramine, fluoxetine, escitalopram and s-ketamine in WHR submitted to the OFT and FST without pre-swim

Some rat strains are more vulnerable to stress and do not require pre-exposure to forced swim to reveal increased immobility, such as the FSL rats, and repeated antidepressant treatment is often required to attenuate stress effects (Schiller et al., Reference Schiller, Pucilowski, Wienicke and Overstreet1992b; Pucilowski & Overstreet, Reference Pucilowski and Overstreet1993; Overstreet et al., Reference Overstreet, Friedman, Mathé and Yadid2005; Overstreet & Wegener, Reference Overstreet and Wegener2013; Schiller et al., Reference Schiller, Pucilowski, Wienicke and Overstreet1992a; Pucilowski & Overstreet, Reference Pucilowski and Overstreet1993; Overstreet et al., Reference Overstreet, Friedman, Mathé and Yadid2005; Overstreet & Wegener, Reference Overstreet and Wegener2013). Therefore, we aimed at investigating the behavioural response of WHR to different antidepressants in this protocol. Moreover, since s-ketamine is known to promote a rapid antidepressant effect in protocols that typically requires chronic antidepressant administration, we also investigated the effects induced by a single s-ketamine administration (10 mg/kg).

Independent groups of WHR received one daily injection of imipramine (20 mg/kg/day), fluoxetine (20 mg/kg/day) or vehicle for 7 days (repeated/subchronic group) and were submitted to the OFT and the FST 1 h after the last injection (seventh day). To control for the effects of the single administration, independent groups of WHR received vehicle injection for 6 days and received a single injection of imipramine (20 mg/kg/day), fluoxetine (20 mg/kg/day) or vehicle on the seventh day (acute group). To investigate the rapid effect of s-ketamine, an independent group was treated with the vehicle for 6 days and a single s-ketamine administration on the seventh day, 1 h before the FST.

The animals were randomly assigned to the different treatment conditions, constituting of a block of six groups: vehicle, (vehicle from day 0 to day 7); repeated imipramine (20 mg/kg/day); acute imipramine group (20 mg/kg); repeated fluoxetine group (20 mg/kg/day); acute fluoxetine group (20 mg/kg); and s-ketamine group (10 mg/kg). To investigate the response of WHR to another SSRI, in addition to fluoxetine, we investigated the effects induced by acute and repeated treatment with escitalopram, using the same protocol as described above. Independent groups of WHR were randomised in four experimental groups: acute escitalopram group (10 mg/kg); repeated escitalopram group (10 mg/kg/day); and the vehicle and acute s-ketamine group (10 mg/kg). During the 6 days, the animals were treated from 11 a.m. to 1 p.m., and on day 7, the animals received the injection 1 h before the OFT.

Experiment 3: Effects of fluoxetine in WHR submitted to LH (protocol 1)

The animals were divided into three groups and received the following intraperitoneal treatments for 7 days: non-stressed or habituated: vehicle (sterile isotonic saline, 1 mL/kg); stressed: vehicle (sterile isotonic saline, 1 mL/kg); and stressed: fluoxetine 20 mg/kg/mL dissolved in tween 80 at 2% in sterile isotonic saline. During the 6 days, the animals were treated around 12 a.m and on day 7, 1 h before the test (Joca et al., Reference Joca, Padovan and Guimarães2003, Reference Joca, Zanelati and Guimarães2006; Stanquini et al., Reference Stanquini, Biojone, Guimarães and Joca2017; Ribeiro et al., Reference Ribeiro, Casarotto, Staquini, Silva, Biojone, Wegener and Joca2019).

Experiments 4 and 5: Effects of fluoxetine in WHR submitted to LH (protocol 2)

On the first day of the experiment, the animals were divided into habituated or stressed and subjected to pre-test. The next day, the animals were subjected to T1. In both PT and T1, the habituated group was exposed to the shuttle box without shocks. After T1, the animals were classified as resilient or susceptible and divided into four groups to receive the following treatments via intraperitoneal for 7 days: non-stressed or habituated: vehicle (sterile isotonic saline, 1 mL/kg); resilient-stressed: vehicle (sterile isotonic saline, 1 mL/kg); susceptible-stressed: vehicle (sterile isotonic saline, 1 mL/kg); and susceptible-stressed: fluoxetine 20 mg/kg/mL dissolved in tween 80 at 2% in sterile isotonic saline. The animals were treated around 12 a.m. and 1 h before the test on day 7 for 6 days. In another experiment with the same conditions, we evaluated the effects of imipramine, thus replacing the group treated with fluoxetine previously described.

Statistical Analyses

The statistical analyses were performed using the GraphPad Prism program (version 8.3.1, GraphPad Software, San Diego California, USA). The effect of stress in the vehicle groups, stressed and not stressed, was analysed by Student’s t-test in both OFT and FST. The effect of drugs in the FST was analysed by one-way ANOVA followed by Dunnett’s post-test. In both cases, differences with p < 0.05 were considered significant. For the first LH protocol, the behaviour parameters were analysed by Kruskal–Wallis test followed by Dunn’s test. For the second LH protocol, the behaviour parameters were analysed by the Mann–Whitney test to compare resilient × susceptible animals and the Kruskal–Wallis test, followed by Dunn’s test for T2. In both cases, differences with p < 0.05 were considered significant.

Results

Experiment 1: Imipramine, fluoxetine and escitalopram fail to induce antidepressant-like effects in WHR submitted to the FST with pre-swim (Fig. 1)

Imipramine treatment did not induce significant changes in the immobility time when compared to the vehicle group (n = 7–10, F 4,36 = 1.041, p = 0.3997; Fig. 1b). However, the doses of 10, 15 and 20 mg/kg significantly reduced the distance travelled in the OFT (n = 7-10, F 4,36 = 3.205, p = 0.0238; Fig. 1c). Moreover, neither fluoxetine nor escitalopram changed the immobility time in the FST (F 4,33 = 0.8726, p = 0.4908; Fig. 1d). The distance travelled in the OFT was also not changed by any of the treatments (F 4,32 = 1.195, p = 0.3318; Fig. 1e).

Experiment 2: Imipramine, fluoxetine, escitalopram and s-ketamine effects in WHR submitted to the FST without pre-swim (Figs. 2 and 3)

Since the conventional treatment with different antidepressants (0, 5 and 23 h after PT) was ineffective in the standard FST protocol, we tested the effect of the single and repeated treatments with antidepressants in WHR submitted to an adapted FST protocol, characterised by a single section of forced swimming (10 min). The immobility time was analysed for 10 min or blocks of 5 min.

In 10 min of FST, none of the treatments induced significant changes in the immobility time (n = 8–10, F 5,48 = 2.141, p = 0.0765; Fig. 2b). However, when analysing the immobility time in blocks of 5 min, both imipramine (7 days) and acute s-ketamine significantly reduced the immobility time in the first 5 min (n = 8–10, F 5,47 = 2.551, p = 0.0402; Fig. 2d), an effect that was not observed in the last 5 min (n = 7–10, F 5,47 = 1.859, p = 0.1197; Fig. 2e). In the OFT, acute and repeated imipramine treatment reduced the locomotor activity compared to the vehicle group during the 10 min of analysis (n = 8–10, F 5,47 = 9.746, p < 0.0001; Fig. 2c).

Since fluoxetine failed to change the immobility time, we investigated whether WHR would not respond to another SSRI, escitalopram (Fig. 3). S-ketamine was used as a positive control in these experiments. The acute administration of s-ketamine significantly reduced the immobility time in 10 min (n = 9–10, F 3,34 = 3.495, p = 0.0259; Fig. 3b) and in the first 5 min of the FST (n = 9–10, F 3,34 = 6.527, p = 0.0013; Fig. 3d), but not in the last 5 min (n = 9–10, F 3,34 = 0.3356, p = 0.7997; Fig. 3e). In contrast, the acute and repeated administration of escitalopram did not change immobility time in any of the analyses. None of the drugs changed the locomotor activity in the OFT when compared with the control group (n = 9–10, F 3,34 = 1.301, p = 0.2901; Fig. 3c).

Experiment 3: Fluoxetine failed to induce antidepressant-like effects in WHR submitted to LH protocol 1 (Fig. 4) (n = 6–10)

Fluoxetine did not change the number of failures (H = 3.632, p = 0.1627, Kruskal–Wallis test), intertrial crossings (H = 4.738; p = 0.0936, Kruskal–Wallis test), avoidances (H = 3.275, p = 0.1945, Kruskal–Wallis test) and escapes (H = 2.797, p = 0.2469, Kruskal–Wallis test) presented by animals in the LH compared to vehicle-stressed group. Moreover, there was no significant difference between vehicle-stressed versus non-stressed vehicle groups.

Experiment 4: Fluoxetine failed to induce antidepressant-like effects in WHR submitted to LH protocol 2 (Fig. 5) (n = 6–11)

In the T1, there was a significant difference in the number of failures (U = 0.0, p < 0.0001), avoidances (U = 32.00, p = 0.0433) and escapes (U = 3.000, p < 0.0001) between resilient and susceptible animals (Fig. 5; Mann–Whitney). This effect does not seem to be associated with non-specific locomotor alterations, since there was no difference in the number of intertrial crossing (U = 34.50, p = 0.0692). In the T2, the repeated treatment of susceptible animals with fluoxetine 20 mg/kg did not induce an antidepressant effect when compared to the control group (stressed-susceptible-vehicle), characterised by the number of failures reduction (H = 19.68, p = 0.0002, Kruskal–Wallis test).

Experiment 5: Imipramine failed to induce antidepressant-like effects in WHR submitted to LH protocol 2 (Fig. 6) (n = 10–19)

In the T1, there was a significant difference in the number of failures (U = 0.0, p < 0.0001), avoidances (U = 92.00, p < 0.0001), escapes (U = 6.500, p < 0.0001) and intertrial crossings (U = 150.0, p = 0.0059) between resilient and susceptible animals (Fig. 6, Mann–Whitney test). In the T2, repeated treatment of susceptible animals with imipramine 20 mg/kg did not induce an antidepressant effect (stressed-susceptible-vehicle), characterised by the number of failures reduction (H = 26.86, p < 0.0001, Kruskal–Wallis test).

Discussion

Our work is the first to investigate the effect of different classes of antidepressant drugs in the WHR strain in two animal models of depression, FST and LH. Unexpectedly, the main finding in the present study is that subacute treatment with three different antidepressants (imipramine, fluoxetine and escitalopram, three injections during 24 h) was ineffective in WHR submitted to the traditional FST protocol (pre-test and test). Interestingly, in WHR not exposed to the pre-test, the repeated treatment with imipramine or acute S-ketamine administration induced antidepressant-like effects, but not fluoxetine or escitalopram treatment. Furthermore, in the LH model, both the subchronic treatment with fluoxetine or imipramine failed to modify the helplessness behaviour of WHR in two different protocols.

Considering that the WHR showed higher immobility during the test, compared with the outbred Wistar rats previously used in our lab (Sartim et al., Reference Sartim, Moreira and Joca2017; Diniz et al., Reference Diniz, Casarotto, Fred, Biojone, Castrén and Joca2018), we hypothesised that the lack of response to acute antidepressant administration in the FST could be related to an increased sensitivity of WHR to stress (average 200 s of immobility in a test lasting a total of 300 s). Therefore, we investigated the effect of acute and repeated treatment with antidepressants in WHR that had not been previously exposed to PT. Interestingly, the average immobility of WHR without pre-test was similar to the immobility reported in the literature for WKY and FSL rats, with an average of 200 s in the first swimming session (5 min) (Overstreet, Reference Overstreet1986; Lahmanme & Armario, Reference Lahmanme and Armario1996; Lahmame et al., Reference Lahmame, Del, Pazos, Yritia and Armario1997; Brand & Harvey, Reference Brand and Harvey2017; Sales et al., Reference Sales, Fogaça, Sartim, Pereira, Wegener, Guimarães and Joca2018). Moreover, our results revealed that only repeated imipramine treatment (7 days) and acute ketamine intervention reduced the immobility time in the WHR, whereas both fluoxetine and escitalopram were ineffective after either acute or repeated (7 days) treatment. This data is consistent with the observation that repeated treatment with tricyclics, monoamine oxidase inhibitors or 8-OH-DPAT is required to promote antidepressant effects in congenitally depressive-like rats, such as the FSL and the WKY rats (Overstreet, Reference Overstreet1986; Paré, Reference Paré1994; Marti & Armario, Reference Marti and Armario1996; Lahmame et al., Reference Lahmame, Del, Pazos, Yritia and Armario1997; Will et al., Reference Will, Aird and Redei2003; Overstreet & Wegener, Reference Overstreet and Wegener2013; Brand & Harvey, Reference Brand and Harvey2017), while fluoxetine is ineffective (López-Rubalcava & Lucki, Reference López-Rubalcava and Lucki2000; Will et al., Reference Will, Aird and Redei2003).

S-ketamine is an NMDA receptor antagonist that promotes acute and fast antidepressant effects; and it is effective in depressed patients that fail to respond to monoaminergic drugs (Berman et al., Reference Berman, Cappiello, Anand, Oren, Heninger, Charney and Krystal2000; Murrough et al., Reference Murrough, Iosifescu, Chang, Al Jurdi, Green, Perez, Pillemer, Foulkes and Mathew2013). Considering that acute ketamine intervention was effective in WHR, as described for FSL and WKY rats (Tizabi et al., Reference Tizabi, Bhatti, Manaye, Das and Akinfiresoye2012; Du Jardin et al., Reference Du Jardin, Liebenberg, Cajina, Müller, Elfving, Sanchez and Wegener2018; Sales et al., Reference Sales, Fogaça, Sartim, Pereira, Wegener, Guimarães and Joca2018), our results indicate that the WHR might be a strain with some level of resistance to monoaminergic drugs, especially SSRIs. Previous studies have reported mixed results regarding the effects of antidepressant treatment in WHR submitted to the FST, both in line and in contrast with our findings. For example, Kawai and colleagues (Kawai et al., Reference Kawai, Kodaira, Tanaka, Ishibashi, Kudo, Kawashima and Mitsumoto2018) demonstrated that both single and repeated injection of imipramine (30 mg/kg) promoted an antidepressant-like effect in WHR exposed to the FST. The discrepancy with our findings can result from several factors, ranging from differences in the protocol, apparatus and origin of animals. For instance, in the PT section, Kawai et al. (Reference Kawai, Kodaira, Tanaka, Ishibashi, Kudo, Kawashima and Mitsumoto2018) submitted the animals only to 10 min of swim FST and not 15 min as in the present study. Moreover, despite the temperature and depth of water being similar to ours, the diameter of the tank cylinder was smaller than ours (24 cm), which can also interfere with the behaviour of animals submitted to FST (Calil & Marcondes, Reference Calil and Marcondes2006; Bogdanovaa et al., Reference Bogdanovaa, Kanekara, D’Ancid and Renshawa2013). For example, the initial difficulty to detect the effect of SSRIs was overcome by changing the water depth and cylinder diameter (Detke et al., Reference Detke, Rickels and Lucki1995; Lucki, Reference Lucki1997).

The origin of the WHR might also play an important role in conflicting results between studies, since local colonies can be subject to founder effects and genetic drift, with substantial genetic divergence from other colonies of the same outbred strain (Brekke et al., Reference Brekke, Steele and Mulley2018). Kawai et al. (Reference Kawai, Kodaira, Tanaka, Ishibashi, Kudo, Kawashima and Mitsumoto2018) obtained the animals from CLEA laboratories (Japan), in contrast to our WHR which were purchased from Charles River Laboratories (USA – 2015) and then bred at the University of São Paulo. The WHR strain was originated in 1964, at Hannover Institute (ZentralInstitut für Versuchstierzucht), after selected breeding for certain phenotypes of Wistar rats(Giknis & Clifford, Reference Giknis and Clifford2009). In 1989, the Han Wistar rats were transferred to Biomedical Research Laboratories (BRL; later Research Consulting Company, RCC; Switzerland), posteriorly to GlaxoWellcome (Wood et al., Reference Wood, Nicolas, Choi, Roman, Nylander, Fernandez-Teruel, Kiianmaa, Bienkowski, de Jong, Colombo, Chastagnier, Wafford, Collingridge, Wildt, Conway-Campbell, Robinson and Lodge2017) and to Charles River, the UK, and North America in 1996 and 1997, respectively (Charles River Laboratories International, 2011, 2015). Furthermore, the WHR were also distributed directly from Hannover Institute to Javier labs, and from BRL to Taconic and CLEA laboratories (Wood et al., Reference Wood, Nicolas, Choi, Roman, Nylander, Fernandez-Teruel, Kiianmaa, Bienkowski, de Jong, Colombo, Chastagnier, Wafford, Collingridge, Wildt, Conway-Campbell, Robinson and Lodge2017; CLEA Japan, Inc., 2020). The behavioural differences in WHR according to their origins can result from the prevalence of specific mutations. For instance, Wood et al. (Reference Wood, Nicolas, Choi, Roman, Nylander, Fernandez-Teruel, Kiianmaa, Bienkowski, de Jong, Colombo, Chastagnier, Wafford, Collingridge, Wildt, Conway-Campbell, Robinson and Lodge2017) demonstrated that WHR presented more prevalence of the mutation at cysteine 407 on mGlu2, which is associated with altered emotionality and impulsivity than other Wistar strains. It was also demonstrated that the frequency of this mutation is not the same depending on the origin/vendor (Wood et al., Reference Wood, Nicolas, Choi, Roman, Nylander, Fernandez-Teruel, Kiianmaa, Bienkowski, de Jong, Colombo, Chastagnier, Wafford, Collingridge, Wildt, Conway-Campbell, Robinson and Lodge2017).

Accordingly, Theilmann et al. (Reference Theilmann, Kleimann, Rhein, Bleich, Frieling, Löscher and Brandt2016) demonstrated that, depending on the breeder, the WHR submitted to CMS could present different mRNA levels of S100 protein p11 (S100A10), a protein involved with the stress susceptibility. In male WHR from Javier labs, CMS did not change behaviour in the open field, elevated plus-maze or sucrose consumption tests. In contrast, WHR from Charles River showed higher anhedonia and anxiolytic-like phenotypes (Theilmann et al., Reference Theilmann, Kleimann, Rhein, Bleich, Frieling, Löscher and Brandt2016). Interestingly, females WHR from Charles River and Javier presented an anxiogenic behaviour when compared to females Wistar rats from Harlan-Winkelmann in the OFT. On elevated plus-maze, the females from Charles River lab presented low locomotor activity and more anxious behaviour than from Javier lab (Honndorf et al., Reference Honndorf, Lindemann, Töllner and Gernert2011). Other suppliers demonstrated different behaviour in the elevated plus-maze, light/dark emergence test, the object recognition memory, prepulse inhibition, and the ethanol intake and preference of male WHR (Goepfrich et al., Reference Goepfrich, Gluch, Friemel and Schneider2013; Momeni et al., Reference Momeni, Segerström and Roman2015). These differences are not exclusive for WHR, since similar behavioural differences have been reported for WKY and in SD depending on their origin (Paré & Kluczynski, Reference Paré and Kluczynski1997; Pecoraro et al., Reference Pecoraro, Ginsberg, Warne, Gomez, la and Dallman2006).

Different suppliers can also be a source of variability in detecting the effect of antidepressant drugs. Porsolt and colleagues (Porsolt et al., Reference Porsolt, Bertin and Jalfre1978b) demonstrated that three injections of imipramine promoted an antidepressant-like effect in Wistar rats from Charles River on FST; however, no effect was observed in the same strain from Iffa Credo. Similarly, Bupropion reduced the immobility time of WKY rats obtained from Charles River and Harlan laboratories, but not from Taconic (Browne et al., Reference Browne, van and Lucki2015). Altogether, differences in the strain, the origin and suppliers of the Wistar rats can interfere in behavioural responses to antidepressants in the FST, thus compromising the reproducibility of the data. Nevertheless, our results indicates that WHR purchased from Charles River Laboratories (USA – 2015) are more resistant to monoaminergic drugs when testesd in the FST, since several studies have described that these drugs reduce the immobility time of different rat strains when exposed to similar protocols, at similar doses (Porsolt et al., Reference Porsolt, Anton, Blavet and Jalfre1978a; Borsini et al., Reference Borsini, Lecci, Sessarego, Frassine and Meli1989; Slattery et al., Reference Slattery, Desrayaud and Cryan2005; France et al., Reference France, Li, Owens, Koek, Toney and Daws2009; Fernández-Guasti et al., Reference Fernández-guasti, Olivares-nazario, Reyes and Martínez-mota2017).

To further confirm our results, we used the LH model, which is one of the most relevant models for the study of depression neurobiology and new drugs with antidepressant activity (Pryce et al., Reference Pryce, Azzinnari, Spinelli, Seifritz, Tegethoff and Meinlschmidt2011; Ribeiro et al., Reference Ribeiro, Casarotto, Staquini, Silva, Biojone, Wegener and Joca2019). Among the neurochemical changes that lead to helplessness, those associated with the noradrenergic and serotoninergic systems stand out (Anisman & Sklar, Reference Anisman and Sklar1979; Sherman & Petty, Reference Sherman and Petty1980; Petty et al., Reference Petty, Kramer and Wilson1992). Regarding the latter, it has been suggested that LH is related to low serotonin levels in specific areas in the brain (Sherman & Petty, Reference Sherman and Petty1980; Petty et al., Reference Petty, Kramer and Wilson1992), which would be restored by repeated antidepressant treatment (Sherman et al., Reference Sherman, Sacquitne and Petty1982). Accordingly, we had previously demonstrated that 7 days treatment with desipramine, imipramine (15 mg/kg) and fluoxetine (20 mg/kg) were able to attenuate LH development in Wistar rats (Sales et al., Reference Sales, Maciel, Suavinha and Joca2021), exposed to a protocol similar to the one used in the present study (Stanquini et al., Reference Stanquini, Biojone, Guimarães and Joca2017; Ribeiro et al., Reference Ribeiro, Casarotto, Staquini, Silva, Biojone, Wegener and Joca2019). However, unlike other studies that reported the reversal of helplessness behaviour with fluoxetine (Zazpe et al., Reference Zazpe, Artaiz, Labeaga, Lucero and Orjales2007; Reinés et al., Reference Reinés, Cereseto, Ferrero, Sifonios, Podestá and Wikinski2008; Macedo et al., Reference Macedo, Cladouchos, Sifonios, Cassaneli and Wikinski2013), we failed to see any antidepressant effect in WHR submitted to the protocol 1.

In this protocol, we had previously demonstrated that the antidepressant effect in a different protocol, using only the susceptible animals, as previously described (Reinés et al., Reference Reinés, Cereseto, Ferrero, Bonavita and Wikinski2004; Russo et al., Reference Russo, Murrough, Han, Charney and Nestler2012; Drugan et al., Reference Drugan, Christianson, Warner and Kent2013; Wood & Bhatnagar, Reference Wood and Bhatnagar2015; Febbraro et al., Reference Febbraro, Svenningsen, Tran and Wiborg2017). The protocol’s change brings our experiment closer to the clinical setting, in which antidepressant pharmacological treatment is usually performed when behavioural/emotional impairment of individuals is detected (Macedo et al., Reference Macedo, Cladouchos, Sifonios, Cassaneli and Wikinski2013) and is supported by the literature (Reinés et al., Reference Reinés, Cereseto, Ferrero, Bonavita and Wikinski2004; Zazpe et al., Reference Zazpe, Artaiz, Labeaga, Lucero and Orjales2007; Reinés et al., Reference Reinés, Cereseto, Ferrero, Sifonios, Podestá and Wikinski2008). Stress-susceptible animals receiving repeated treatment with fluoxetine (20 mg/kg) did not show improved response when compared to the control group (stressed-susceptible-vehicle).

Previous studies also considered the vulnerability factor to stress when evaluating Wistar rats behaviour in the LH model. In their study, the rodents were exposed on day 1 to a training session (60 inescapable shocks, 0.6 mA, 15 s duration, for 1 h) and 4 days later, they were submitted to a test session (15 shocks, 20 s duration, for 15 min) (Reinés et al., Reference Reinés, Cereseto, Ferrero, Sifonios, Podestá and Wikinski2008). The animals were classified as helpless based on their latency to escape/avoid shocks in T1, which were treated with the drug of interest and re-exposed to the apparatus in test 2. Macedo et al. (Reference Macedo, Cladouchos, Sifonios, Cassaneli and Wikinski2013), Unlike these studies that discarded resilient animals in their analysis, this group was included in our study. Interestingly, we observed that these animals maintained their resilience behaviour at the end of the tests, demonstrating the effectiveness of our model.

In both studies mentioned above, intraperitoneal treatment with fluoxetine 10 mg/kg for 21 days was able to produce an antidepressant effect in Wistar rats submitted to LH, characterised by a decrease in latency time and the number of failures to escape or avoid the shocks (Reinés et al., Reference Reinés, Cereseto, Ferrero, Sifonios, Podestá and Wikinski2008; Macedo et al., Reference Macedo, Cladouchos, Sifonios, Cassaneli and Wikinski2013). In the present work, even with an adapted LH protocol, in which only stress-susceptible rats were treated with fluoxetine 20 mg/kg for 7 days, they continued not responding to the treatment. Once again, our results were contradictory to those in the literature, which could be related to protocol differences (number of shocks, intensity, duration, apparatus and days of treatment) but mainly to the strain used (Reinés et al., Reference Reinés, Cereseto, Ferrero, Bonavita and Wikinski2004, Reference Reinés, Cereseto, Ferrero, Sifonios, Podestá and Wikinski2008; Zazpe et al., Reference Zazpe, Artaiz, Labeaga, Lucero and Orjales2007; Macedo et al., Reference Macedo, Cladouchos, Sifonios, Cassaneli and Wikinski2013; Stanquini et al., Reference Stanquini, Biojone, Guimarães and Joca2017; Ribeiro et al., Reference Ribeiro, Casarotto, Staquini, Silva, Biojone, Wegener and Joca2019). In another attempt, we assessed whether the WHR would be responsive to treatment with imipramine 20 mg/kg for 7 days in the adapted LH protocol 2. Once again, there were two populations of animals, 39.5% of resilient and 60.5% of the susceptible. Similar to what we observed in the experiment with fluoxetine (adapted LH protocol), the resilient animals maintained their resilience until the end of the experiment. Regarding treatment, imipramine in WHR did not induce an antidepressant response. Contrasting with these data, previous works from our group demonstrated that intraperitoneal treatment with imipramine 15 mg/kg for 7 days induced an antidepressant effect in Wistar rats submitted to LH (Stanquini et al., Reference Stanquini, Biojone, Guimarães and Joca2017; Ribeiro et al., Reference Ribeiro, Casarotto, Staquini, Silva, Biojone, Wegener and Joca2019).

Compiling our data, the WHR did not respond to different antidepressants known to reverse helpless behaviour, even when tested in an adapted protocol to bring it closer to the clinical setting. Wistar rats comprise several strains derived from a common strain, namely Wistar, Wistar Hannover, Wistar Unielever, Wistar Kyoto and Wistar Furth (McCormick, Reference McCormick2017). Genetic drift and the fact that they grew up in different environments present different behavioural responses, making it difficult to compare studies with different strains (Palm et al., Reference Palm, Hävermark, Meyerson, Nylander and Roman2011). However, a fact that caught our attention is that WKY rats are a valid model of stress susceptibility, exhibiting behaviour, neurochemical and endocrine alterations parallel to clinical depression, being considered resistant to classical antidepressants (Aleksandrova et al., Reference Aleksandrova, Wang and Phillips2019). Corroborating these data, Willner et al. (Reference Willner, Gruca, Lason, Tota-Glowczyk, Litwa, Niemczyk and Papp2019) observed that after chronic stress, WKY did not respond to treatment with different classes of antidepressants (tricyclic, SSRI and serotonin and noradrenaline reuptake inhibitor) in the elevated plus-maze, object recognition test and sucrose intaket. Among the differences that could justify the resistance to antidepressant treatment in WKY are the greater hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis responsiveness (Pardon et al., Reference Pardon, Gould, Garcia, Phillips, Cook, Miller, Mason and Morilak2002; Rittenhouse et al., Reference Rittenhouse, López-Rubalcava, Stanwood and Lucki2002; Morilak et al., Reference Morilak, Barrera, Echevarria, Garcia, Hernandez, Ma and Petre2005), abnormalities in monoaminergic transmission (Tejani-Butt et al., Reference Tejani-Butt, Paré and Yang1994; Durand et al., Reference Durand, Berton, Aguerre, Edno, Combourieu, Mormède and Chaouloff1999; Pollier et al., Reference Pollier, Sarre, Aguerre, Ebinger, Mormède, Michotte and Chaouloff2000; De La Garza & Mahoney, Reference La Garza and Mahoney2004), reduced hippocampus volume relative to Wistar (Tizabi et al., Reference Tizabi, Hauser, Tyler, Getachew, Madani, Sharma and Manaye2010), and lower levels of neurogenesis and cell proliferation in the hippocampus (Kin et al., Reference Kin, Yasuhara, Kameda, Agari, Sasaki, Morimoto, Okazaki, Umakoshi, Kuwahara, Kin, Tajiri and Date2017). Since the WHR is derived from a common lineage to WKY, we speculate that these animals may share similarities in their profile of resistance to antidepressant treatment.

Thus, the absence of acute response to antidepressants in WHR is similar to the delayed antidepressant response observed in patients. Moreover, the responsivity to the tricyclic antidepressant and to ketamine, but not to SSRI class, is another similarity with treatment-resistant patients (Oswald et al., Reference Oswald, Brezinova and Dunleavy1972; Posternak & Zimmerman, Reference Posternak and Zimmerman2005; Bennabi et al., Reference Bennabi, Charpeaud, Yrondi, Genty, Destouches, Lancrenon, Alaïli, Bellivier, Bougerol, Camus, Dorey, Doumy, Haesebaert, Holtzmann, Lançon, Lefebvre, Moliere, Nieto, Rabu, Richieri, Schmitt, Stephan, Vaiva, Walter, Leboyer, El-Hage, Llorca, Courtet and Aouizer2019). However, this conclusion is limited by the lack of investigation with other classes of antidepressants, such as monoamine oxidase inhibitors and serotonin and norepinephrine reuptake inhibitors, or even the combination of different classes of antidepressants. Moreover, we have not investigated more extended treatment periods,the drug effect in female rats and different drug administration routes. Although we have not compared the WHR with other rat strains or from different vendors, our results agree with Theilmann et al. (Reference Theilmann, Kleimann, Rhein, Bleich, Frieling, Löscher and Brandt2016), who demonstrated that WHR from Charles River are more susceptible to stress than WHR from other vendors. Therefore, further studies investigating how different antidepressants affect WHR behaviour in comparison with other rat strains (e.g. Wistar, SD and WKY), along with the evaluation of drug metabolism, neurochemical changes induced by stress, and genomic and proteomic profiles in the brain under stressful and non-stressful conditions, could clarify about the mechanisms subsiding the impaired response to antidepressants in WHR.

Acknowledgements

We are thankful to Flavia Salata and Miriam Melo for the helpful technical assistance.

KMS receives a PhD fellowship from CNPq (141900/2018-0), LV receives a PhD fellowship from FAPESP (2019/04616-2), SFL receives a Young Investigator fellowship from FAPESP (2017/19731-6) and SRLJ receives productivity a fellowship from CNPq (304780/20189). This study was financed in part by CAPES – the Coordenação de Aperfeiçoamento de Pessoal de Nível Superior – Brazil (Finance Code 001), FAPESP (2019/04616-2 and 2017/24304-0) and the Aarhus University Research Foundation.

The main idea of this work was from Samia Joca, Ariandra Sartim, Kennia Silveira and Letícia Vieira. Kennia Silveira, Ariandra Sartim, Letícia Vieira, Sabrina Lisboa, Samia Joca and Gregers Wegener designed this protocol together. Data collection has been done by Kennia Silveira, Ariandra Sartim and Letícia Vieira. Writing of the first drafts of the manuscript have been done by Kennia Silveira and Letícia Vieira. All authors contributed to and have approved the final manuscript.

Conflict of interest

Gregers Wegener is the Editor-in-Chief of Acta Neuropsychiatrica but was not involved and actively withdrew during the review and decision process of this manuscript.

References

Abelaira, HM, Réus, GZ and Quevedo, J (2013) Animal models as tools to study the pathophysiology of depression. Revista Brasileira De Psiquiatria 35(Suppl 2), S112S120.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aleksandrova, LR, Wang, YT and Phillips, AG (2019) Evaluation of the Wistar-Kyoto rat model of depression and the role of synaptic plasticity in depression and antidepressant response. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 105, 123.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anisman, H and Sklar, LS (1979) Catecholamine depletion in mice upon reexposure to stress: mediation of the escape deficits produced by inescapable shock. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 93(4), 610625.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Avey, MT, Moher, D, Sullivan, KJ, Fergusson, D, Griffin, G, Grimshaw, JM, Hutton, B, Lalu, MM, Macleod, M, Marshall, J, Mei, SHJ, Rudnicki, M, Stewart, DJ, Turgeon, AF, McIntyre, L and Canadian Critical Care Translational Biology Group, The devil is in the details: Incomplete reporting in preclinical animal research. PLoS One 11(11), 113.Google Scholar
Begley, CG and Ioannidis, JPA (2015) Reproducibility in science: improving the standard for basic and preclinical research. Circulation Research 116(1), 116126.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bennabi, D, Charpeaud, T, Yrondi, A, Genty, JB, Destouches, S, Lancrenon, S, Alaïli, N, Bellivier, F, Bougerol, T, Camus, V, Dorey, JM, Doumy, O, Haesebaert, F, Holtzmann, J, Lançon, C, Lefebvre, M, Moliere, F, Nieto, I, Rabu, C, Richieri, R, Schmitt, L, Stephan, F, Vaiva, G, Walter, M, Leboyer, M, El-Hage, W, Llorca, P-M, Courtet, P and Aouizer, B (2019) Clinical guidelines for the management of treatment-resistant depression: French recommendations from experts, the French Association for Biological Psychiatry and Neuropsychopharmacology and the fondation FondaMental. BMC Psychiatry 19(1), 112.Google ScholarPubMed
Berman, RM, Cappiello, A, Anand, A, Oren, DA, Heninger, GR, Charney, DS and Krystal, JH (2000) Antidepressant effects of ketamine in depressed patients. Biological Psychiatry 47(4):351354.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bogdanovaa, OV, Kanekara, S, D’Ancid, KE and Renshawa, PF (2013) Factors influencing behavior in the forced swim test. Physiology & Behavior 118(4–5), 227239.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Borsini, F, Lecci, A, Sessarego, A, Frassine, R and Meli, A (1989) Discovery of antidepressant activity by forced swimming test may depend on pre-exposure of rats to a stressful situation. Psychopharmacology 97(2), 183188.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brand, SJ and Harvey, BH (2017) Exploring a post-traumatic stress disorder paradigm in Flinders sensitive line rats to model treatment-resistant depression I: bio-behavioural validation and response to imipramine. Acta Neuropsychiatrica 29(4), 193206.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brekke, TD, Steele, KA and Mulley, JF (2018) Inbred or outbred? Genetic diversity in laboratory rodent colonies. G3: Genes, Genomes, Genetics 8(2), 679686.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Browne, CA, van, Nest D and Lucki, I (2015) Antidepressant-like effects of buprenorphine in rats are strain dependent. Behavioural Brain Research 176(1), 139148.Google Scholar
Calil, CM and Marcondes, FK (2006) The comparison of immobility time in experimental rat swimming models. Life Sciences 79(18), 17121719.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Charles River Laboratories International I (2011) Wistar Han IGS Rats. https://www.criver.com/sites/default/files/resources/rm_d_Wistar_Han_IGS_Rat.pdf (accessed 27 October 2022).Google Scholar
Charles River Laboratories International I (2015) Outbred rats Outbred rats. https://www.criver.com/products-services/find-model/wistar-han-igs-rat?region=3621 (accessed 27 October 2022).Google Scholar
Chesler, EJ, Wilson, SG, Lariviere, WR, Rodriguez-Zas, SL and Mogil, JS (2002) Influences of laboratory environment on behavior. Nature Neuroscience 5(11), 11011102.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chia, R, Achilli, F, Festing, MFW and Fisher, EMC (2005) The origins and uses of mouse outbred stocks. Nature Genetics 37(11), 11811186.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
CLEA Japan, Inc. (2020) https://www.clea-japan.com/en/products/outbred/item_a0370 (accessed 10 November 2020).Google Scholar
Cryan, JF and Lucki, I (2000) Antidepressant-like behavioral effects mediated by 5-Hydroxytryptamine(2C) receptors. Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics 295(3), 11201126.Google ScholarPubMed
Cryan, JF, Markou, A and Lucki, I (2002) Assessing antidepressant activity in rodents: recent developments and future needs. Trends in Pharmacological Sciences 23(5), 238245.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cryan, JF, Valentino, RJ and Lucki, I (2005) Assessing substrates underlying the behavioral effects of antidepressants using the modified rat forced swimming test. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 29(4-5), 547569.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Detke, MJ, Rickels, M and Lucki, I (1995) Active behaviors in the rat forced swimming test differentially produced by serotonergic and noradrenergic antidepressants. Psychopharmacology 121(1), 6672.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Diniz, CRAF, Casarotto, PC, Fred, SM, Biojone, C, Castrén, E and Joca, SRL (2018) Antidepressant-like effect of losartan involves TRKB transactivation from angiotensin receptor type 2 (AGTR2) and recruitment of FYN. Neuropharmacology 135(10), 163171.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Drugan, RC, Christianson, JP, Warner, TA and Kent, S (2013) Resilience in shock and swim stress models of depression. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience 7, 18.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Durand, M, Berton, O, Aguerre, S, Edno, L, Combourieu, I, Mormède, P and Chaouloff, F (1999) Effects of repeated fluoxetine on anxiety-related behaviours, central serotonergic systems, and the corticotropic axis in SHR and WKY rats. Neuropharmacology 38(6), 893907.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Einat, H, Ezer, I, Kara, NZ and Belzung, C (2018) Individual responses of rodents in modelling of affective disorders and in their treatment: prospective review. Acta Neuropsychiatrica 30(6), 323333.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fanelli, D (2018) Is science really facing a reproducibility crisis, and do we need it to? Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 115(11), 26282631.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Febbraro, F, Svenningsen, K, Tran, TP and Wiborg, O (2017) Neuronal substrates underlying stress resilience and susceptibility in rats. PLoS One 12(6), 119.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fernández-guasti, A, Olivares-nazario, M, Reyes, R and Martínez-mota, L (2017) Pharmacology, biochemistry and behavior sex and age differences in the antidepressant-like effect of fluoxetine in the forced swim test. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior 152, 8189.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
France, C, Li, J-X, Owens, W, Koek, W, Toney, G and Daws, L (2009) Reduced effectiveness of escitalopram in the forced swimming test is associated with increased serotonin clearance rate in food restricted rats. The International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology 12(6), 731736.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gauvin, DV, Dalton, JA, Harter, ML, Holdsworth, D, May, J, Tapp, R, Zimmermann, ZJ, Kilgus, Q and Baird, TJ (2019) Relative equivalence of CNS safety (FOB) assessment outcomes in male and female Wistar-Han and Sprague-Dawley rats. Journal of Pharmacological and Toxicological Methods 95, 211.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Geoffroy, M and Christensen, AV (1993) Psychomotor stimulants versus antidepressants in the learned helplessness model of depression. Drug Development Research 29(1), 4855.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Giknis, ML and Clifford, C (2009) Reproductive parameters and fetal data from reproductive toxicity studies in the Charles River Wistar Hannover [Crl:WI(Han)] rat. Charles River Laboratories technical data 130.Google Scholar
Goepfrich, AA, Gluch, C, Friemel, CM and Schneider, M (2013) Behavioral differences in three Wistar Han rat lines for emotional reactivity, cognitive processing and ethanol intake. Physiology & Behavior 10-1111, 102108.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hayakawa, K, Mimura, Y, Tachibana, S, Furuya, M, Kodama, T, Aoki, T, Hosokawa, S, Fukui, M, Shibata, S, Yoshida, M, Masuyama, T, Narita, T, Kuwagata, M, Hisada, S, Maki, E (2013) Study for collecting background data on Wistar Hannover [Crl: Wi(Han)] rats in general toxicity studies - comparative data to Sprague Dawley rats. Journal of Toxicological Sciences 38(6), 855873.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Honndorf, S, Lindemann, C, Töllner, K and Gernert, M (2011) Female Wistar rats obtained from different breeders vary in anxiety-like behavior and epileptogenesis ଝ. Epilepsy Research 94(1-2), 2638.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hunter, P (2017) The reproducibility “crisis”. Embo Reports 18(9), 14931496.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Du Jardin, KG, Liebenberg, N, Cajina, M, Müller, HK, Elfving, B, Sanchez, C and Wegener, G (2018) S-ketamine mediates its acute and sustained antidepressant-like activity through a 5-HT1B receptor dependent mechanism in a genetic rat model of depression. Frontiers in Pharmacology 8, 111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jin, ZL, Chen, XF, Ran, YH, Li, XR, Xiong, J, Zheng, YY, Gao, NN and Li, YF (2017) Mouse strain differences in SSRI sensitivity correlate with serotonin transporter binding and function. Scientific Reports 7(1), 110.Google ScholarPubMed
Joca, SRL, Padovan, CM and Guimarães, FS (2003) Activation of post-synaptic 5-HT1A receptors in the dorsal hippocampus prevents learned helplessness development. Brain Research 978(1-2), 177184.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Joca, SRL, Zanelati, T and Guimarães, FS (2006) Post-stress facilitation of serotonergic, but not noradrenergic, neurotransmission in the dorsal hippocampus prevents learned helplessness development in rats. Brain Research 1087(2006), 6774.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kara, NZ, Stukalin, Y and Einat, H (2018) Revisiting the validity of the mouse forced swim test: systematic review and meta-analysis of the effects of prototypic antidepressants. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 84, 111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kawai, H, Kodaira, N, Tanaka, C, Ishibashi, T, Kudo, N, Kawashima, Y and Mitsumoto, A (2018) Time of administration of acute or chronic doses of imipramine affects its antidepressant action in rats. Journal of Circadian Rhythms 16(1), 19.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kin, K, Yasuhara, T, Kameda, M, Agari, T, Sasaki, T, Morimoto, J, Okazaki, M, Umakoshi, M, Kuwahara, K, Kin, I, Tajiri, N, Date, I (2017) Hippocampal neurogenesis of Wistar Kyoto rats is congenitally impaired and correlated with stress resistance. Behavioural Brain Research 329, 148156.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
La Garza, RDe and Mahoney, JJ (2004) A distinct neurochemical profile in WKY rats at baseline and in response to acute stress: implications for animal models of anxiety and depression. Brain Research 1021(2), 209218.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lahmame, A, Del, Arco C, Pazos, A, Yritia, M and Armario, A (1997) Are Wistar-Kyoto rats a genetic animal model of depression resistant to antidepressants? European Journal of Pharmacology 337(2-3), 115123.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lahmanme, A and Armario, A (1996) Differential responsiveness of inbred strains of rats to antidepressants in the forced swimming test: are Wistar Kyoto rats an animal model of subsensitivity to antidepressants? Psychopharmacology 123(2), 191198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lohmiller, JJ and Swing, SP (2006) Reproduction and Breeding. Amsterdam: Elsevier.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
López-Rubalcava, C and Lucki, I (2000) Strain differences in the behavioral effects of antidepressant drugs in the rat forced swimming test. Neuropsychopharmacology 22(2), 191199.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lucki, I (1997) The forced swimming test as a model for core and component behavioral effects of antidepressant drugs. Behavioural pharmacology 8, 523532.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lucki, I, Dalvi, A and Mayorga, AJ (2001) Sensitivity to the effects of pharmacologically selective antidepressants in different strains of mice. Psychopharmacology 155(3), 315322.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Macedo, GVF, Cladouchos, ML, Sifonios, L, Cassaneli, PM and Wikinski, S (2013) Effects of fluoxetine on CRF and CRF1 expression in rats exposed to the learned helplessness paradigm. Psychopharmacology 225(3), 647659.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maier, SF and Seligman, MEP (2016) Learned helplessness at fifty: insights from neuroscience. Psychological Review 123(4), 119.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marti, J and Armario, A (1996) Forced swimming behavior is not related to the corticosterone levels achieved in the test: a study with four inbred rat strains. Physiology & Behavior 59(2), 369373.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCormick, DL (2017) Preclinical evaluation of carcinogenicity using standard-bred and genetically engineered rodent models. In A Comprehensive Guide to Toxicology in Nonclinical Drug Development. New York: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Momeni, S, Segerström, L and Roman, E (2015) Supplier-dependent differences in intermittent voluntary alcohol intake and response to naltrexone in Wistar rats. Frontiers in Neuroscience 9, 113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morilak, DA, Barrera, G, Echevarria, DJ, Garcia, AS, Hernandez, A, Ma, S and Petre, CO (2005) Role of brain norepinephrine in the behavioral response to stress. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry 29(8), 12141224.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murrough, JW, Iosifescu, DV, Chang, LC, Al Jurdi, RK, Green, CE, Perez, AM, Pillemer, S, Foulkes, A and Mathew, SJ (2013) Antidepressant efficacy of ketamine in treatment-resistant major depression: a two-site randomized controlled trial. The American Journal of Psychiatry 170(10), 11341142.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Noritake, K, Ikeda, T, Ito, K, Miwa, Y and Senuma, M (2013) Study for collecting background data on Wistar Hannover [Crl:WI(Han)] rats in embryo-fetal development studies - comparative data to Sprague Dawley rats. The Journal of Toxicological Sciences 38(6), 847854.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olorisade, BK, Brereton, P and Andras, P (2017) Reproducibility of studies on text mining for citation screening in systematic reviews: evaluation and checklist. Journal of Biomedical Informatics 73(1), 113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oswald, BIAN, Brezinova, V and Dunleavy, DLF (1972) On the slowness of action of tricycic antidepressant drugs. British Journal of Psychiatry 120(559), 673677.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Overstreet, DH, Friedman, E, Mathé, AA and Yadid, G (2005) The Flinders Sensitive Line rat: a selectively bred putative animal model of depression. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 29(4-5), 739759.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Overstreet, DH and Wegener, G (2013) The flinders sensitive line rat model of depression-25 years and still producing. Pharmacological Reviews 65(1), 143155.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Overstreet, DI (1986) Selective breeding for increased cholinergic function: development of a new animal model of depression. Biological Psychiatry 21(1), 4958.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Palm, S, Hävermark, Å., Meyerson, BJ, Nylander, I and Roman, E (2011) When is a Wistar a Wistar ? Behavioral profiling of outbred Wistar rats from five different suppliers using the MCSF test. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 135(1-2), 128137.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pardon, MC, Gould, GG, Garcia, A, Phillips, L, Cook, MC, Miller, SA, Mason, PA and Morilak, DA (2002) Stress reactivity of the brain noradrenergic system in three rat strains differing in their neuroendocrine and behavioral responses to stress: implications for susceptibility to stress-related neuropsychiatric disorders. Neuroscience 115(1), 229242.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paré, WP (1994) Open field, learned helplessness, conditioned defensive burying, and forced-swim tests in WKY rats. Physiology & Behavior 55(3), 433439.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paré, WP and Kluczynski, J (1997) Differences in the stress response of Wistar-Kyoto (WKY) rats from different vendors. Physiology & Behavior 62(3), 643648.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pecoraro, N, Ginsberg, AB, Warne, JP, Gomez, F, la, Fleur SE and Dallman, MF (2006) Diverse basal and stress-related phenotypes of Sprague Dawley rats from three vendors. Physiology & Behavior 89(4), 598610.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petty, F, Kramer, G and Wilson, LeAnn (1992) Prevention of learned helplessness: in vivo correlation with cortical serotonin. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior 43(2), 361367.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pollier, F, Sarre, S, Aguerre, S, Ebinger, G, Mormède, P, Michotte, Y and Chaouloff, F (2000) Serotonin reuptake inhibition by citalopram in rat strains differing for their emotionality. Neuropsychopharmacology 22(1), 6476.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Porsolt, RD, Anton, G, Blavet, N and Jalfre, M (1978a) Behavioural despair in rats: a new model sensitive to antidepressant treatments. European Journal of Pharmacology 47(4), 379391.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Porsolt, RD, Bertin, A and Jalfre, M (1978b) Behavioural despair, in rats and mice: strain differences and the effects of imipramine. European Journal of Pharmacology 51(3), 291294.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Porsolt, RD, Le, Pichon M and Jalfre, M (1977) Depression: a new animal model sensitive to antidepressant treatments. Nature 266(5604), 730732.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Posternak, MA and Zimmerman, M (2005) Is there a delay in the antidepressant effect? A meta-analysis. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 66(2), 148158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pound, P and Ritskes-Hoitinga, M (2018) Is it possible to overcome issues of external validity in preclinical animal research? Why most animal models are bound to fail. Journal of Translational Medicine 16(1), 18.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pryce, CR, Azzinnari, D, Spinelli, S, Seifritz, E, Tegethoff, M and Meinlschmidt, G (2011) Helplessness: a systematic translational review of theory and evidence for its relevance to understanding and treating depression. Pharmacology & Therapeutics 132(3), 242267.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pucilowski, O and Overstreet, DH (1993) Effect of chronic antidepressant treatment on responses to apomorphine in selectively bred rat strains. Brain Research Bulletin 32(5), 471475.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reinés, A, Cereseto, M, Ferrero, A, Bonavita, C and Wikinski, S (2004) Neuronal cytoskeletal alterations in an experimental model of depression. Neuroscience 129(3), 529538.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reinés, A, Cereseto, M, Ferrero, A, Sifonios, L, Podestá, MF and Wikinski, S (2008) Maintenance treatment with fluoxetine is necessary to sustain normal levels of synaptic markers in an experimental model of depression: correlation with behavioral response. Neuropharmacology 33, 18961908.Google Scholar
Ribeiro, A, Ferraz-de-Paula, V, Pinheiro, ML and Palermo-Neto, J (2009) Dose-response effects of systemic anandamide administration in mice sequentially submitted to the open field and elevated plus-maze tests. Brazilian Journal of Medical and Biological Research 42(6), 556560.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ribeiro, DE, Casarotto, PC, Staquini, L, Silva, Pinto E, Biojone, MA, Wegener, C, Joca, G and S (2019) Reduced P2X receptor levels are associated with antidepressant effect in the learned helplessness model. PeerJ 2019(10), 117.Google Scholar
Rittenhouse, PA, López-Rubalcava, C, Stanwood, GD and Lucki, I (2002) Amplified behavioral and endocrine responses to forced swim stress in the Wistar-Kyoto rat. Psychoneuroendocrinology 27(3), 303318.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rockett, JC, Narotsky, MG, Thompson, KE, Thillainadarajah, I, Blystone, CR, Goetz, AK, Ren, H, Best, DS, Murrell, RN, Nichols, HP, Schmid, JE, Wolf, DC, Dix, DJ (2006) Effect of conazole fungicides on reproductive development in the female rat. Reproductive Toxicology 22(4), 647658.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Russel, WMS and Burch, RL (1959) The Principles of Humane Experimental Technique. London: Methuen & Co. Ltd.Google Scholar
Russo, SJ, Murrough, JW, Han, MH, Charney, DS and Nestler, EJ (2012) Neurobiology of resilience. Nature Neuroscience 15(11), 14751484.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sales, AJ, Fogaça, MV, Sartim, AG, Pereira, VS, Wegener, G, Guimarães, FS and Joca, SRL (2018) Cannabidiol induces rapid and sustained antidepressant-like effects through increased BDNF signaling and synaptogenesis in the prefrontal cortex. Molecular Neurobiology 56(2), 10701081.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sales, AJ, Maciel, IS, Suavinha, ACDR and Joca, SRL (2021) Modulation of DNA methylation and gene expression in rodent cortical neuroplasticity pathways exerts rapid antidepressant-like effects. Molecular Neurobiology 58(2), 777794.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sartim, AG, Moreira, FA and Joca, SRL (2017) Involvement of CB1 and TRPV1 receptors located in the ventral medial prefrontal cortex in the modulation of stress coping behavior. Neuroscience 340, 126134.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schiller, GD, Pucilowski, O, Wienicke, C and Overstreet, DH (1992a) Immobility-reducing effects of antidepressants in a genetic animal model of depression. Brain Research Bulletin 28(5), 821823.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schiller, GD, Pucilowski, O, Wienicke, C and Overstreet, DH (1992b) Immobility-reducing effects of antidepressants in a genetic animal model of depression. Brain Research Bulletin 28(5), 821823.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sherman, AD and Petty, F (1980) Neurochemical basis of the action of antidepressants on learned helplessness. Behavioral and Neural Biology 30(2), 119134.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sherman, AD, Sacquitne, JL and Petty, F (1982) Specificity of the learned helplessness model of depression. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior 16(3), 449454.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Slattery, DA and Cryan, JF (2014) The ups and downs of modelling mood disorders in rodents. ILAR Journal 55(2), 297309.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Slattery, DA, Desrayaud, S and Cryan, JF (2005) GABAB receptor antagonist-mediated antidepressant-like behavior is serotonin-dependent. Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics 312(1), 290296.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Soares-Cunha, C, Coimbra, B, Borges, S, Domingues, AV, Silva, D, Sousa, N and Rodrigues, AJ (2018) Mild prenatal stress causes emotional and brain structural modifications in rats of both sexes. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience 12, 115.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stanquini, LA, Biojone, C, Guimarães, FS and Joca, SR (2017) Repeated treatment with nitric oxide synthase inhibitor attenuates learned helplessness development in rats and increases hippocampal BDNF expression. Acta Neuropsychiatrica 30(3), 127136.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tejani-Butt, SM, Paré, WP and Yang, J (1994) Effect of repeated novel stressors on depressive behavior and brain norepinephrine receptor system in Sprague-Dawley and Wistar Kyoto (WKY) rats. Brain Research 649(1-2), 2735.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Theilmann, W, Kleimann, A, Rhein, M, Bleich, S, Frieling, H, Löscher, W and Brandt, C (2016) Behavioral differences of male Wistar rats from different vendors in vulnerability and resilience to chronic mild stress are reflected in epigenetic regulation and expression of p11. Brain Research 1642, 505515.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tillmann, S (2017) Mind the gap – towards complete and transparent reporting of animal research. Medical Writing 26(4), 2427.Google Scholar
Tizabi, Y, Bhatti, BH, Manaye, KF, Das, JR and Akinfiresoye, L (2012) Antidepressant-like effects of low ketamine dose is associated with increased hippocampal AMPA/NMDA receptor density ratio in female Wistar-Kyoto rats. Neuroscience 213, 7280.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tizabi, Y, Hauser, SR, Tyler, KY, Getachew, B, Madani, R, Sharma, Y and Manaye, KF (2010) Effects of nicotine on depressive-like behavior and hippocampal volume of female WKY rats. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry 34(1), 6269.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vollmayr, B and Gass, P (2013) Learned helplessness: unique features and translational value of a cognitive depression model. Cell and Tissue Research 354(1), 171178.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wang, Q, Timberlake, MA, Prall, K and Dwivedi, Y (2017) The recent progress in animal models of depression. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry 77(3), 99109.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wieland, S, Boren, JL, Consroe, PF and Martin, A (1986) Stock differences in the susceptibility of rats to learned helplessness training. Life Sciences 39(10), 937944.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Will, CC, Aird, F and Redei, EE (2003) Selectively bred Wistar-Kyoto rats: an animal model of depression and hyper-responsiveness to antidepressants. Molecular Psychiatry 8(11), 925932.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Willner, P, Gruca, P, Lason, M, Tota-Glowczyk, K, Litwa, E, Niemczyk, M and Papp, M (2019) Validation of chronic mild stress in the Wistar-Kyoto rat as an animal model of treatment-resistant depression. Behavioural Pharmacology 30(2 and 3), 239250.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wood, CM, Nicolas, CS, Choi, SL, Roman, E, Nylander, I, Fernandez-Teruel, A, Kiianmaa, K, Bienkowski, P, de Jong, TR, Colombo, G, Chastagnier, D, Wafford, KA, Collingridge, GL, Wildt, SJ, Conway-Campbell, BL, Robinson, ESJ, Lodge, D (2017) Prevalence and influence of cys407* Grm2 mutation in Hannover-derived Wistar rats: mGlu2 receptor loss links to alcohol intake, risk taking and emotional behaviour. Neuropharmacology 115(3), 128138.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wood, SK and Bhatnagar, S (2015) Resilience to the effects of social stress: evidence from clinical and preclinical studies on the role of coping strategies. Neurobiology of Stress 1(1), 164173.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zazpe, A, Artaiz, I, Labeaga, L, Lucero, ML and Orjales, A (2007) Reversal of learned helplessness by selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors in rats is not dependent on 5-HT availability. Neuropharmacology 52(3), 975984.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Fig. 1. Effect of imipramine, fluoxetine and escitalopram in WHR submitted to forced swim test with pre-swim (b, d) and open field test (OFT) (c, e). (a) Experimental design of forced swim test with pre-swim: the animals were submitted to the pre-test (PT) section of forced swim and, immediately after, received the first i.p. injection (0 h), the second and third injection were administrated 5 h and 23 h after PT, respectively. One hour after the last injection, the WHR were submitted to OFT and FST. The bars represent mean ± standard error (one-way ANOVA followed by Dunnett’s post hoc test or Student’s t-test, *p < 0.05 compared to the vehicle group (Veh), n = 6–10 per group).

Figure 1

Fig. 2. Effects of the acute and repeated administration of fluoxetine (20 mg/kg), imipramine (20 mg/kg) and s-ketamine (10 mg/kg) in rats submitted to forced swimming test (FST) without pre-swim and open field test (OFT). (a) Experimental design of forced swim test without pre-swim: the animals were treated systematically during 7 days with antidepressant or vehicle (subchronic treatment) or received the vehicle for 6 days and received the drugs only on the last day (acute treatment). On the seventh day, the OFT and FST were performed 1 h after the last treatment. (b) Immobility time during 10 min of FST; (c) locomotor activity during 10 min of OFT; (d) immobility time in the first 5 min of FST; and (e) immobility time in the last 5 min of FST. The bars represent mean ± standard error (one-way ANOVA followed by Dunnett’s post hoc test, *p < 0.05 compared to the vehicle group (Veh), n = 8–10 per group).

Figure 2

Fig. 3. Effects of the acute and repeated administration of escitalopram (20 mg/kg) and s-ketamine (10 mg/kg) in rats submitted to forced swim test (FST) without pre-swim and open field test (OFT). (a) Experimental design of FST without pre-swim; (b) immobility time during 10 min of FST; (c) locomotor activity during 10 min of OFT; (d) immobility time in the first 5 min of FST; and (e) immobility time in the last 5 min of FST. The bars represent mean ± standard error (one-way ANOVA followed by Dunnett’s post hoc test, *p < 0.05 compared to the vehicle group (Veh), n = 9–10 per group).

Figure 3

Fig. 4. Effects of fluoxetine (20 mg/kg) at number of failures, avoidances, escapes and intertrial crossings of Wistar Hannover rats subjected to learned helplessness (traditional protocol). (a) Experimental design used for LH traditional protocol. The animals were divided into non-stressed and stressed. On the first day of the experiment (pre-test), the non-stressed animals were just exposed to the shuttle box for 40 min without shocks, while the stressed ones were subjected to 40 inescapable shocks (0.6 mA, 10 s duration, 30–90 s interval). The animals were treated once daily for 7 days with vehicle (sterile saline) or fluoxetine 20 mg/kg/mL and 1 h after the last injection was tested with 30 escape shocks (0.6 mA, 10 s duration, 30–90 s interval) preceded by a warning tone (60 dB, 670 Hz, 5 s). (b) Number of failures. (c) Number of avoidance. (d) Number of escape. (e) Number of intertrial crossings. NST-VEH (non-stressed vehicle), ST-VEH (stressed-vehicle), ST-FLX (stressed-fluoxetine 20 mg/kg/mL). Values are expressed as mean ± SEM. Kruskal–Wallis test followed by Dunn’s test indicated that there was no statistical difference between the groups.

Figure 4

Fig. 5. Effects of fluoxetine (20 mg/kg) at number of failures, avoidances, escapes and intertrial crossings of Wistar Hannover rats subjected to learned helplessness (adapted protocol). (a) Experimental design used for adapted LH protocol. The animals were divided into non-stressed and stressed. On the first day of the experiment (pre-test), the non-stressed animals were just exposed to the shuttle box for 40 min without shocks, while the stressed ones were subjected to 40 inescapable shocks (0.6 mA, 10 s duration, 30–90 s interval). Twenty-four hours later, the stressed animals were subjected to test 1 (T1) (0.6 mA, 10 s duration, 30–90 s interval) and classified according to their number of failures in resilient (0–10 failures) or susceptible (15–30 failures). The non-stressed group was again just exposed to the shuttle box without shocks. The animals groups were treated once daily for 7 days with: non-stressed (sterile saline); stressed-resilient (sterile saline), stressed-susceptible-vehicle (sterile saline) and stressed-susceptible-fluoxetine (20 mg/kg/mL). One hour after the last administration, the animals were exposed to test 2 (0.6 mA, 10 s duration, 30–90 s interval) preceded by a warning tone (60 dB, 670 Hz, 5 s). (b) Number of failures after T1. (c) Number of avoidance after T1. (d) Number of escapes after T1. (e) Number of intertrial crossings after T1. Data are expressed as mean ± SEM. The Mann–Whitney test indicated a significant difference in the number of failures, avoidances and escapes between resilient (n = 6) and susceptible (n = 22) animals. *p < 0.05. (f) Number of failures after T2. (g) Number of avoidance after T2. (h) Number of escapes after T2. (i) Number of intertrial crossings after T2. NST-VEH (non-stressed vehicle), ST-RESIL (stressed-resilient), ST-SUSC-VEH (stressed-susceptible-vehicle), ST-SUSC FLX (stressed-susceptible-fluoxetine). Data are expressed as mean ± SEM. The Kruskal–Wallis test followed by Dunn’s test indicated that there was no significant difference in the number of failures between the ST-SUSC VEH × ST-SUSC FLX groups. *p < 0.05. n = 6–11 per group.

Figure 5

Fig. 6. Effects of imipramine (20 mg/kg) at number of failures, avoidances, escapes and intertrial crossings of Wistar Hannover rats subjected to learned helplessness (adapted protocol). (a) Experimental design used for adapted LH protocol. The animals were divided into non-stressed and stressed. On the first day of the experiment (pre-test), the non-stressed animals were just exposed to the shuttle box for 40 min without shocks, while the stressed ones were subjected to 40 inescapable shocks (0.6 mA, 10 s duration, 30–90 s interval). Twenty-four hours later, the stressed animals were subjected to test 1 (T1) (0.6 mA, 10 s duration, 30–90 s interval) and classified according to their number of failures in resilient (0–10 failures) or susceptible (15–30 failures). The non-stressed group was again just exposed to the shuttle box without shocks. The animals groups were treated once daily for 7 days with: non-stressed (sterile saline); stressed-resilient (sterile saline), stressed-susceptible-vehicle (sterile saline) and stressed-susceptible-fluoxetine (20 mg/kg/mL). One hour after the last administration, the animals were exposed to test 2 (0.6 mA, 10 s duration, 30–90 s interval) preceded by a warning tone (60 dB, 670 Hz, 5 s). (b) Number of failures after T1. (c) Number of avoidance after T1. (d) Number of escapes after T1. (e) Number of intertrial crossings after T1. Data are expressed as mean ± SEM. The Mann– Whitney test indicated a significant difference in the number of failures, avoidances, escapes and crossings between resilient (n = 19) and susceptible (n = 29) animals. *p < 0.05. (f) Number of failures after T2. (g) Number of avoidance after T2. (h) Number of escape after T2. (i) Number of intertrial crossings after T2. NST-VEH (non-stressed vehicle), ST-RESIL (stressed-resilient), ST-SUSC-VEH (stressed-susceptible-vehicle), ST-SUSC IMIP (stressed-susceptible-imipramine). Data are expressed as mean ± SEM. The Kruskal–Wallis test followed by Dunn’s test indicated that there was no significant difference in the number of failures between the ST-SUSC VEH × ST-SUSC IMIP groups. *p < 0.05. n = 10–19 per group.