Learning on the basis of feedback and reward has been shown to involve predominantly frontostriatal networks. One major part of these networks is the striatum, which integrates affective, motor and cognitive information and influences goal-directed behaviour. Reference Haber and Knutson1 Several functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies in healthy controls showed a strong bilateral activation of the striatum, mainly in its dorsal part during processing of reinforcement tasks. Reference O'Doherty, Dayan, Schultz, Deichmann, Friston and Dolan2 Apart from the striatum projection regions of the dopaminergic midbrain, such as medial frontal cortex, Reference Kahnt, Park, Cohen, Beck, Heinz and Wrase3 the amygdala-hippocampus complex Reference Cohen, Elger and Weber4 and insula Reference O'Doherty, Dayan, Friston, Critchley and Dolan5 are critically involved in the processing of reinforcement and reward as well as in reward-based learning processes. Of note, midbrain dopaminergic networks have been shown to respond predominantly towards unpredicted rewards, i.e. when a so-called prediction error is committed. Alterations in the dopaminergic reward system, predominantly in frontostriatal networks, constitute core characteristics of the disorder of schizophrenia. Reference Ziauddeen and Murray6 Accordingly, patients have repeatedly been found to be impaired in processes involving this system, such as in reinforcement learning processes. For instance, in a previous study Reference Koch, Schachtzabel, Wagner, Schikora, Schultz and Reichenbach7 we showed that the ability to learn contingencies on the basis of reward-related feedback is altered on a behavioural as well as on a neuronal level in people with schizophrenia. Here, a hypoactivation of the dorsal striatum (i.e. putamen), dorsal cingulate and the superior frontal cortex was found in patients relative to healthy controls during processing of reinforcement and reward. Aberrant activation in the striatum, both in the ventral Reference Esslinger, Englisch, Inta, Rausch, Schirmbeck and Mier8,Reference Morris, Vercammen, Lenroot, Moore, Langton and Short9 and dorsal Reference Gradin, Kumar, Waiter, Ahearn, Stickle and Milders10,Reference Grimm, Vollstadt-Klein, Krebs, Zink and Smolka11 portion, has repeatedly been reported in people with schizophrenia in the context of reward processing. Looking at differences between patients treated with different therapy types, Juckel and colleagues showed a reduced activation in the ventral striatum during response to rewarding stimuli, compared with controls, in people with schizophrenia who were medicated Reference Juckel, Schlagenhauf, Koslowski, Filonov, Wustenberg and Villringer12 and unmedicated. Reference Juckel, Schlagenhauf, Koslowski, Wustenberg, Villringer and Knutson13 Schlagenhauf et al (2008) Reference Schlagenhauf, Juckel, Koslowski, Kahnt, Knutson and Dembler14 found a significantly decreased activation of the ventral striatum in patients treated with typical antipsychotics compared with controls and patients treated with atypical antipsychotics during anticipation of rewarding stimuli. In addition to a disrupted activation in frontostriatal networks alterations in the so-called salience network comprising the insula and the anterior cingulate cortex have been reported in people with schizophrenia during processing of reinforcement and reinforcement-based learning. Reference Dowd and Barch15-Reference Walter, Kammerer, Frasch, Spitzer and Abler17 For instance, using a delayed incentive paradigm with monetary rewards Walter et al Reference Walter, Kammerer, Frasch, Spitzer and Abler17 found disrupted activation in the anterior cingulate in patients, with only healthy controls showing increasing activation with increasing reward. These disruptions within the salience network have been associated with both positive and negative symptoms (i.e. passivity symptoms) of the disorder. Reference Palaniyappan and Liddle18 An inappropriate allocation of salience to internal representations and external events, potentially as a phenomenological consequence of altered neuronal activation in the salience network, is being discussed as the underlying psychopathological mechanism.
In summary, the studies described above show strong evidence for altered activation in networks involved in the processing of reinforcement and reward in patients with schizophrenia. Surprisingly, barely any evidence exists with regard to the functional connectivity between or within these networks in association with reward processing in patients. The first evidence came from Schlagenhauf et al (2009) Reference Schlagenhauf, Sterzer, Schmack, Ballmaier, Rapp and Wrase19 who found a reduced functional connectivity between two brain regions (i.e. ventral striatum and medial frontal cortex) known to be important predominantly in the context of reward delivery. Gradin et al Reference Gradin, Waiter, O'Connor, Romaniuk, Stickle and Matthews20 found not only a reduced activation in, among others, putamen and nucleus accumbens but also a weaker functional connectivity between the dopamine-rich midbrain and the right insula-anterior cingulate cortex salience network that correlated with the severity of psychotic symptoms in patients with schizophrenia. Moreover, it has been hypothesised that structural alterations in regions with a high incidence of dopamine transporters such as the striatum may lead to altered activation and connectivity in the dopamine reward system as well as negative symptoms and passivity as their clinical manifestation. Reference Palaniyappan and Liddle18 In accordance with this assumption, the striatum has been reported to be structurally altered in patients with schizophrenia. The majority of studies reported increases in putaminal volume, Reference Henze, Brunner, Thiemann, Parzer, Richterich and Essig21-Reference Kawasaki, Suzuki, Kherif, Takahashi, Zhou and Nakamura24 which have been linked to long-term antipsychotic treatment, Reference Ho, Andreasen, Ziebell, Pierson and Magnotta25 whereas some studies also found decreased volumes Reference Molina, Galindo, Cortes, de Herrera, Ledo and Sanz26,Reference Ballmaier, Schlagenhauf, Toga, Gallinat, Koslowski and Zoli27 or no volume alterations. Reference Keshavan, Rosenberg, Sweeney and Pettegrew28 Surprisingly, the direct association between structural alterations and functional activation or connectivity of the striatum has barely been investigated in individuals with schizophrenia. Against this background the present study aimed at investigating the potential association between functional connectivity and grey matter structure of the dorsal striatum (i.e. putamen) in patients with schizophrenia during processing of a reward-learning task and to relate potential alterations in connectivity to the degree of negative symptoms and blunted affect (passivity). We expected the connectivity of the dorsal striatum to relevant midbrain and salience network regions to be reduced in patients with schizophrenia, with this reduced connectivity being directly linked to structural characteristics of the striatum.
Method
Participants
The study sample consisted of 17 patients (9 male, 8 female) with a DSM-IV diagnosis of schizophrenia (schizophrenia group) and 18 controls (11 male, 7 female; control group). 29 All participants were right-handed. Reference Annett30 On average, the schizophrenia group were 33.1 years old (s.d. = 7.9). Education was measured in years of schooling: German Abitur, 13 years; mittlere Reife, 10 years; Volksschule, 8 years. Mean education in the schizophrenia group was 11.5 years (s.d. = 1.9). In the control group the mean age was 29.3 (s.d. = 6.3) with a mean education of 12.7 years (s.d. = 1.0). There was no significant difference between the groups in terms of age (t(33) = –1.7) but a significant difference regarding education (t(23.5) = 2.2, P<0.04, corrected for unequal variances). Diagnosis was established by the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders (SCID-I) Reference First, Spitzer, Gibbon and Williams31 and confirmed by a clinical psychiatrist (Ch.S.). All participants in the schizophrenia group were free of any concurrent psychiatric diagnosis and had no neurological conditions. They were in remission from an acute psychotic episode. Thirteen were on stable medication with atypical antipsychotics, two were on stable medication with atypical antipsychotics and antidepressant medication (venlafaxine, citalopram) and received typical antipsychotic medication (haloperidol, flupenthixol). Antipsychotic doses were converted to chlorpromazine equivalents according to Woods. Reference Woods32 Mean chlorpromazine equivalents were 544.4 (s.d. = 293.3). The psychopathological status of the patients was assessed with the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS). Reference Kay, Fiszbein and Opler33 Ratings were 16.1 (s.d. = 4.4) on the positive subscale, 21.2 (s.d. = 6.4) on the negative subscale and 37.0 (s.d.= 8.0) on the general psychopathology scale.
The control group were screened by comprehensive assessment procedures for medical, neurological and psychiatric history. Exclusion criteria were current and potentially interfering medical conditions, any current or previous neurological or psychiatric disorder, and first-degree relatives with Axis I psychiatric or neurological disorders. All participants gave written informed consent to the study protocol. The protocol is in accordance with the ethical standards of the Declaration of Helsinki and was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Friedrich-Schiller-University Medical School.
Experimental design
Using the Presentation software package (Neurobehavioral Systems Inc. Albany, CA, USA; see http://www.neurobs.com) stimuli were projected onto a transparent screen inside the scanner tunnel, which could be viewed by the participant through a mirror system mounted on top of the MRI head coil. The participants’ responses were recorded using an MRI-compatible fibre-optic response device (Lightwave Medical Industries, Canada) with four button keypads for the right hand. Participants were informed that they would be presented with a card with a geometrical figure on it (i.e. cross, half-moon, triangle or pentagon) and were asked to guess whether the figure on the card predicted a value higher or lower than the number five. Each figure predicted the respective value with a probability of either 50% or 100%. Each correct guess was followed by a monetary reward (+0.50e) whereas each wrong guess was followed by a punishment (–0.50e). Participants were instructed that the figure predicted the respective value with a certain probability but were not informed about the predictive probabilities of the respective figures. The whole paradigm consisted of a series of 64 interleaved trials with 32 trials for each probability condition distributed across the whole task sequence. Each trial started with the presentation of the probability condition-specific figure, which was shown for 1.5 s. After an interstimulus interval lasting 4.5 s a question mark was presented for 2.5 s during which participants had to answer by a button press. After another interstimulus interval of 4.5 s the correct solution followed by the indication of a reward or punishment appeared for 2.5 s. Participants were compensated according to their performance, although a minimum of e20 was guaranteed for volunteering. Each trial ended with an intertrial interval lasting 3.5 s. In addition, we introduced a temporal jitter by varying the second interstimulus interval between 4.5 and 5.5 s in order to increase sensitivity.
fMRI procedure
Functional data were collected on a 3T whole-body system equipped with a 12-element receive-only head matrix coil (MAGNETOM TIM Trio, Siemens). Foam pads were used for positioning and immobilisation of the participant’s head within the head coil. T * 2-weighted images were obtained using a gradient-echo echo-planar imaging (EPI) sequence (repetition time (TR) = 2040 ms; echo time (TE) = 26 ms; flip angle, 90°) with 40 contiguous transverse slices of 3.3 mm thickness covering the entire brain. Matrix size was 72 × 72 pixels with in-plane resolution of 2.67 × 2.67 mm corresponding to a field of view of 192 × 192 mm. A series of 645 whole-brain volume sets were acquired, with the first three images of each series being discarded. High-resolution anatomical T1-weighted volume scans (MP-RAGE) were obtained in sagittal orientation (TR = 2300 ms; TE = 3.03 ms; inversion time (TI) = 900 ms; flip angle, 9; filed of view (FOV) = 256 mm; matrix 256 × 256; number of sagittal slices, 192; acceleration factor (PAT) = 2; acquisition time (TA) = 5:21 min) with an isotropic resolution of (1 × 1 × 1) mm3.
Data analysis
Behavioural data
Performance was assessed by the percentage of correct reactions in each probability condition. A two-sample t-test for each probability condition (50%, 100%) was performed to test for differences between the groups. In addition we applied the concept of temporal difference learning to estimate an individual learning rate parameter Reference Sutton and Barto34 (for analysis details see Koch et al Reference Koch, Schachtzabel, Wagner, Schikora, Schultz and Reichenbach7 ). Here, the change in associative strength of stimulus i on each trial j, (ΔV ij), was determined as:
In psychological terms, γ ij can be interpreted as stimulus-reward associability Reference Dayan and Abbott35 and moreover as a discount factor determining the extent to which rewards that arrive earlier are more important for learning than rewards that arrive later. Reference O'Doherty, Dayan, Friston, Critchley and Dolan5,Reference Niv and Schoenbaum36 In case of a correct guess and monetary gain, Aij of a trial takes the value 1, for non-reinforcement trials (i.e. incorrect guess and monetary loss) Aij was coded by 0. illustrates the expected reward or associative strength of each trial j. The learning rate (LR), modelled to assess the individual learning capability under stable learning conditions (i.e. 100 % condition), was then calculated as follows:
where Vij /max reflects the expected reward in case of optimal learning performance. Given 16 trials for each stimulus category gives the mean LR for each condition. Thus, higher learning rate values stand for better learning. A two-sample t-test served for comparing the individual learning rates between the groups. A repeated-measures ANOVA with associative strength (i.e. associative strength values across the 16 trials) as within-participant factor, and group (schizophrenia, control) as between-participant factor served for testing for group differences across the learning process under stable learning conditions. To investigate whether the learning performance was associated with the degree of negative symptoms, a partial correlation between individual learning rate and degree of negative symptoms (corrected for the influence of general psychopathology) was conducted.
fMRI data
Preprocessing and statistical analysis of the fMRI data was performed using SPM8 (http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm). Functional data were corrected for differences in time of acquisition by sinc interpolation, realigned to the first image of the session and linearly and non-linearly normalised to the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) reference brain (MNI 152). Data were spatially smoothed with a Gaussian kernel (8 mm, full-width at half maximum (FWHM)) and high-pass filtered with a 128 s cut-off. All data were inspected for movement artefacts. Participants with movement parameters exceeding 3 mm translation on the x-, y-, or z-axis or 3 rotation were excluded (1 patient and 1 healthy control, which resulted in a final sample size of 17 patients and 18 controls). In addition, individual movement parameters entered the analyses as covariates of no interest.
On the first level, brain activations were then analysed voxel-wise to calculate statistical parametric maps of t-statistics for the 50% probability condition (i.e. activation during responding to triangles and pentagons), the 100% probability condition (i.e. activation during responding to half-moons and crosses) and positive compared with negative reinforcement (i.e. activation during presentation of monetary win v. monetary loss).
Blood oxygenation level-dependent (BOLD) signal changes for the different conditions were modelled as a covariate of variable length boxcar functions and convolved with a canonical haemodynamic response function. These haemodynamic response function were then used as individual regressors within the general linear model.
Psychophysiological interactions
We used psychophysiological interaction (PPI) analysis to investigate our hypotheses of altered striatal (i.e. putamen) connectivity in the schizophrenia group in association with reward processing. The PPI analysis is based on bilinear interaction terms. Reference Friston, Buechel, Fink, Morris, Rolls and Dolan37 Analogous to the use of modulatory bilinear terms in systems engineering the interaction term can be regarded as an expression of the modulatory input of an external factor or signal on the interaction between a target and a source region. Against the background of our hypothesis of an aberrant striatal connectivity in people with schizophrenia, we identified striatal seed regions of interest based on the peak activation from the second-level analysis (i.e. positive v. negative feedback) in the schizophrenia group (left putamen activation maximum: x = –14, y = 10, z = –8; right putamen activation maximum: x = 12, y = 6, z = –7) and the control group (left putamen activation maximum: x = –10, y = 4, z = –12; right putamen activation maximum: x = 8, y = 6, z = –10). Next, individual time series from spheres with 4 mm radius around the individual activation maximum next to the group-specific striatal seed region were extracted for each participant. These individual spheres were all located in the putamen with the distance between individual centre coordinates and centre coordinates of seed regions (as described above) not exceeding a radius of 10 mm for all participants (apart from one healthy control where peak coordinates of left and right putamen were located at x = –24, y = 2, z = –4 and x = 29, y = 0, z = 5 and another healthy control where peak coordinates of right putamen were located at x = 22, y= 4, z= –10). These time series constituted the physiological component of the PPI. The contrast between positive v. negative feedback constituted the psychological component. The interaction (i.e. the element-by-element product) between the psychological component and the physiological component was used as the PPI regressor.
With this implementation of the PPI analysis, significant activations of a particular area would reflect increased functional connectivity between the source area (i.e. left/right putamen) and the activated regions during processing of positive v. negative feedback. One-way ANOVAs served for illustrating regions functionally connected with the striatal seed regions in both the groups as well as in the control group compared with the schizophrenia group. To investigate whether differences in connectivity between the groups are as a result of differences in behavioural performance, we performed additional one-way ANCOVAs with performance (i.e. the learning rate) as covariate. To correct for false positive errors, the double-threshold approach was used, which imposes both an activation threshold and a cluster size threshold. Activation clusters were considered significant if they reached a threshold of P<0.001 on the voxel-level and exceeded an extent of 20 voxels on the cluster-level, which is equivalent to a threshold of P = 0.05, corrected. In addition, we performed small volume corrections (based on 4 mm spheres around maximum-activated voxels) for all significant results of the group comparisons.
Voxel-based morphometry - PPI
To investigate whether alterations in grey matter structure (i.e. grey matter volume) in patients with schizophrenia underlie altered functional connectivity we performed voxel-based morphometry (VBM) applying the VBM-toolbox (http://dbm.neuro.uni-jena.de/) implemented in SPM8 using default parameters. Images were bias-corrected, tissue classified, and linearly (i.e. 12-parameter affine registration) and non-linearly (i.e. warping regularisation) registered. Subsequently, grey matter and white matter segments were modulated by multiplication with the non-linear components derived from the normalisation matrix in order to preserve actual grey matter and white matter values locally. Images were smoothed with a Gaussian kernel of 8 mm FWHM). Finally, to investigate the hypothesised association between altered striatal connectivity and grey matter structure patients’ age-corrected grey matter values were extracted from the left and right putamen (i.e. 4 mm sphere at x = 9, y = 6, z = –11 and x = –14, y = 11, z = –8) and correlated with task-related functional connectivity of the left and right putamen (i.e. seed regions as described above). As before, thresholding was performed according to the double-threshold approach, i.e. activation clusters were considered significant if they reached or exceeded an extent of 20 voxels, which is equivalent to a threshold of P = 0.05, corrected. Potential group differences in putaminal grey matter volume (i.e. extracted grey matter values from left and right putamen) were investigated by two-sample t-tests. Although the reliability of chlorpromazine equivalents is not uncontroversial, Reference Rijcken, Monster, Brouwers and de Jong-van den Berg38 extracted grey matter values from left and right putamen were correlated with chlorpromazine equivalents Reference Woods32 to explore the effects of antipsychotic medication on putamen volume.
Psychopathology - PPI
Finally, to investigate the hypothesised association between altered striatal connectivity and negative symptoms negative PANSS scores and passivity scores (i.e. N1 blunted affect) were correlated with task-related functional connectivity of the left and right putamen (i.e. seed regions as described above). To adjust for the confounding effects of general psychopathology PANSS general scores were added as a covariate-of-no-interest. Again, thresholding was based on the double-threshold approach, i.e. correlation between functional connectivity and psychopathology was considered significant if clusters reached or exceeded an extent of 20 voxels corresponding to a threshold of P = 0.05, corrected.
Results
Behavioural data
In the 50% condition where prediction or learning was impossible both the schizophrenia and the control group showed a comparable percentage of correct responses (controls: 50.3 (s.d. = 11.4), patients: 46.5 (s.d. = 8.8), t(33) = 1.1, not significant). In the 100% condition the schizophrenia group showed a lower percentage of correct responses compared with controls (control group: 88.7 (s.d. = 15.9), schizophrenia group: 78.7 (s.d. = 13.7), t(33) = 2.0, P<0.054). The assessment of the individual learning capability under stable learning conditions yielded a mean learning rate of 0.8 (s.d. = 0.1) in the schizophrenia group and 0.9 (s.d. = 0.2) in the control group. The independent two-sample t-test testing for differences in the individual learning rates between the groups yielded a trend significance (t(33) = 1.9, P<0.063). The ANOVA testing for group differences in associative strength across the learning process under stable learning conditions yielded a trend significance for group (F(1,33) = 3.7, P<0.063), a main effect of associative strength (F(1,37) = 49.9, P<0.001) and a borderline significant interaction (F(15,33) = 1.7, P = 0.057) indicating lower overall performance as well as a worse learning performance across time in the schizophrenia group compared with the control group.
PPI
The one-way ANOVA investigating functional connectivity of the left putamen in association with processing of positive v. negative feedback yielded a network consisting of mainly bilateral occipital and frontal regions, the left caudate and the right insula in the control group (Table 1, online Fig. DS1) and a network comprising predominantly bilateral occipital and frontal regions and the left temporal cortex in the schizophrenia group (Table 1, online Fig. DS1).
The one-way ANOVA investigating functional connectivity of the right putamen in association with processing of positive v. negative feedback yielded a network consisting of mainly bilateral occipital, frontal and temporal regions, the right caudate and the insula bilaterally in the control group (Table 2) and a network comprising predominantly bilateral occipital and frontal regions in the schizophrenia group (Table 3).
The one-way ANOVA comparing functional connectivity of the left putamen between the groups yielded a significantly lower connectivity in the schizophrenia group in a network containing the right middle temporal gyrus, the occipital lobe bilaterally, the right insula and the precuneus (Table 4, online Fig. DS1). The one-way ANCOVA comparing functional connectivity of the left putamen between the groups corrected for performance (i.e. learning rate) yielded a significantly lower connectivity in the schizophrenia group in a network containing the right middle temporal gyrus, the occipital lobe bilaterally and the right insula (Table 4). The opposite contrast showed no significantly increased connectivity in the control group, neither with nor without correcting for performance.
The one-way ANOVA comparing functional connectivity of the right putamen between the groups yielded a significantly lower connectivity in the schizophrenia group in the left precuneus (x = –30, y = –74, z = 40, Brodmann’s area (BA) 19, k = 69, T = 4.40). The one-way ANCOVA comparing functional connectivity of the right putamen between the groups corrected for performance (i.e. learning rate) yielded no significant results. The opposite contrast showed a significantly increased connectivity in the schizophrenia group in the left inferior frontal gyrus/BA 45 (ANOVA: x = 50, y = 16, z = 4, k = 18, T = 4.88, ANCOVA corrected for performance: x = 50, y = 16, z = 4, k = 53, T = 5.82). Finally, all regions showing significant group differences remained significant when analysed with a small volume correction (i.e. P<0.006 or smaller).
VBM - PPI
The positive correlation between grey matter volume of the left putamen and functional connectivity of the left putamen seed region yielded significant results in a network containing mainly the right middle temporal gyrus, the occipital lobe bilaterally, the left insula, the left precuneus and the frontal cortex bilaterally (Table 5, online Fig. DS2). The negative correlation yielded no significant effects. There were also no significant effects for the right putamen. The two-sample t-test comparing grey matter volumes between the groups yielded a significant result both for the left (schizophrenia group: 0.56 (s.d. = 0.07), control group: 0.64 (s.d. = 0.06), t(33) = –3.4, P<0.002) and the right (schizophrenia group 0.61 (s.d. = 0.06), control group: 0.68 (s.d. = 0.05), t(33) = –3.3, P<0.003) putamen. There was no significant correlation between chlorpromazine equivalent dosage and volume of the right or left putamen.
Psychopathology - PPI
The positive correlation between degree of negative symptoms and functional connectivity of the left putamen seed region yielded a small cluster in the left cerebellum (x = –22, y = –40, z = –32, k = 20, T = 4.51). The negative correlation yielded significant results in a network comprising the occipital lobe bilaterally, the right insula, the anterior cingulate and the frontal cortex bilaterally (Table 6). Similar results were detectable for the correlation with passivity (i.e. N1 blunted affect). The positive correlation between degree of negative symptoms and functional connectivity of the right putamen seed region yielded no significant results. The same applied to the negative correlation with passivity symptoms. The negative correlation showed significant activation in the anterior cingulate and the middle frontal gyrus bilaterally for the degree of negative symptoms (Table 6) and in the left insula and bilateral middle frontal gyrus for the degree of passivity.
Side | Brodmann’s area | k | T | x, y, z | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Control group | |||||
Occipital lobe, superior parietal lobe, cerebellum | Left/right | 18 | 29 390 | 9.25 | –30, –86, –10 |
Anterior insula, middle frontal gyrus | Right | 13 | 2547 | 5.92 | 42, 14, 16 |
Caudate | Left | 178 | 5.89 | –10, 12, –6 | |
Frontal lobe, medial frontal gyrus | 9 | 781 | 5.76 | 6, 40, 30 | |
Frontal lobe, inferior frontal gyrus | Left | 47 | 66 | 5.39 | –28, 16, –20 |
Frontal lobe, middle frontal gyrus | Left | 9 | 489 | 5.06 | –42, 10, 30 |
Frontal lobe, middle frontal gyrus | Right | 8 | 215 | 4.6 | 26, 26, 46 |
Frontal lobe, precentral gyrus | Right | 6 | 171 | 4.36 | 40, –12, 36 |
Cingulate gyrus | 24 | 27 | 4.12 | 4, –4, 34 | |
Frontal lobe, precentral gyrus | Left | 4 | 43 | 4.09 | –62, –12, 30 |
Cingulate gyrus | Left | 31 | 22 | 3.92 | –24, –34, 38 |
Frontal lobe, middle frontal gyrus | Left | 6 | 46 | 3.87 | –30, 14, 48 |
Parahippocampal gyrus | Left | 34 | 22 | 3.82 | –14, –4, –14 |
Frontal lobe, inferior frontal gyrus | Left | 47 | 24 | 3.77 | –34, 26, –4 |
Schizophrenia group | |||||
Frontal lobe, inferior frontal gyrus | Left | 47 | 225 | 7.33 | –28, 16, –18 |
Temporal lobe, fusiform gyrus | Right | 37 | 1222 | 7.11 | 42, –54, –16 |
Temporo-occipito-parietal lobe, precuneus Left | 31 | 2374 | 6.45 | –24, –72, 26 | |
Occipital lobe, middle occipital gyrus | Right | 19 | 765 | 5.25 | 38, –82, 16 |
Frontal lobe, medial frontal gyrus | Right | 9 | 129 | 5.11 | 6, 42, 20 |
Temporal lobe, superior temporal gyrus | Left | 22 | 161 | 4.96 | –52, –40, 6 |
Frontal lobe, inferior frontal gyrus | Right | 47 | 56 | 4.77 | 26, 12, –16 |
Frontal lobe, middle frontal gyrus | Right | 46 | 47 | 4.66 | 54, 26, 24 |
Parahippocampal gyrus | Left | 34 | 59 | 4.58 | –14, 0, –14 |
Frontal lobe, medial frontal gyrus | 9 | 80 | 4.11 | 2, 50, 38 | |
Parahippocampal gyrus | Right | 34 | 27 | 4.08 | 16, 0, –12 |
Frontal lobe, inferior frontal gyrus | Right | 9 | 140 | 3.95 | 42, 8, 30 |
a. One-way ANOVA at P<0.05, corrected.
Side | Brodmann’s area | k | T | x, y, z | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Occipital lobe | Left/right | 18/19 | 17 132 | 8 | –20, –84, –6 |
Anterior insula | Right | 13 | 375 | 6.68 | 36, 24, 0 |
Frontal lobe, medial frontal gyrus | Right | 6 | 1095 | 6.47 | 6, 40, 34 |
Parietal lobe, postcentral gyrus | Right | 40 | 124 | 5.83 | 56, –30, 50 |
Frontal lobe, middle frontal gyrus | Right | 46 | 485 | 5.53 | 46, 28, 24 |
Frontal lobe, middle frontal gyrus | Left | 6 | 125 | 5.41 | –34, 14, 54 |
Temporal lobe, middle temporal gyrus | Left | 22 | 144 | 5.16 | –52, –38, 4 |
Cingulate gyrus | Left | 31 | 201 | 4.79 | –12, –40, 40 |
Frontal lobe, middle frontal gyrus | Right | 9 | 290 | 4.75 | 40, 10, 40 |
Caudate | Right | 347 | 4.7 | 8, 14, –2 | |
Frontal lobe, inferior frontal gyrus | Left | 9 | 490 | 4.69 | –50, 16, 24 |
Anterior insula | Left | 13 | 146 | 4.64 | –36, 22, –6 |
Cerebellum | Left | 54 | 4.6 | –8, –56, –18 | |
Frontal lobe, precentral gyrus | Left | 4 | 61 | 4.43 | –46, –10, 46 |
Temporal lobe, middle temporal gyrus | Right | 39 | 41 | 4.31 | 42, –64, 24 |
Parietal lobe, postcentral gyrus | Left | 40 | 44 | 4.29 | –52, –38, 44 |
Frontal lobe, middle frontal gyrus | Right | 6 | 59 | 4.27 | 32, 2, 48 |
Posterior cingulate | Right | 30 | 36 | 4.21 | 6, –48, 18 |
Parahippocampal gyrus | Left | 35 | 35 | 4.19 | –16, –24, –8 |
Frontal lobe, middle frontal gyrus | Right | 10 | 44 | 4.19 | 42, 56, 8 |
Temporal lobe, superior temporal gyrus | Right | 39 | 122 | 4.11 | 50, –50, 28 |
Frontal lobe, superior frontal gyrus | Right | 10 | 26 | 4.01 | 26, 52, 0 |
Frontal lobe, middle frontal gyrus | Left | 6 | 35 | 3.93 | –34, 4, 60 |
Substantia nigra | Right | 39 | 3.83 | 8, –14, –8 | |
Frontal lobe, paracentral lobule | Left | 6 | 23 | 3.81 | –4, –32, 58 |
a. One-way ANOVA at P<0.05, corrected.
Side | Brodmann’s area | k | T | x, y, z | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Frontal lobe, precentral gyrus | Right | 44 | 28 | 5.16 | 50, 16, 4 |
Frontal lobe, subcallosal gyrus | Left | 34 | 31 | 5.12 | –14, 0, –10 |
Occipital lobe | Right | 19 | 393 | 4.75 | 32, –72, 28 |
Cerebellum | Right | 93 | 4.7 | 40, –48, –20 | |
Frontal lobe, inferior frontal gyrus | Right | 9 | 150 | 4.66 | 44, 6, 30 |
Occipital lobe, lingual gyrus | Left | 18 | 368 | 4.5 | –26, –74, –8 |
Occipital lobe, cuneus | Left | 18 | 33 | 4.18 | –26, –72, 26 |
Cerebellum | Right | 180 | 4.16 | 32, –72, –12 | |
Occipital lobe, middle occipital gyrus | Left | 18 | 66 | 4.09 | –22, –96, 8 |
Frontal lobe, superior frontal gyrus | Left | 6 | 21 | 4.03 | –8, 20, 56 |
Occipital lobe, lingual gyrus | Right | 17 | 44 | 4.01 | 16, –94, –6 |
Frontal lobe, middle frontal gyrus | Right | 46 | 52 | 3.94 | 54, 30, 24 |
Frontal lobe, middle frontal gyrus | Left | 6 | 33 | 3.79 | –38, 2, 54 |
Frontal lobe, medial frontal gyrus | Right | 6 | 22 | 3.79 | 4, 42, 36 |
a. One-way ANOVA at P<0.05, corrected.
Side | Brodmann’s area | k | T | x, y, z | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Uncorrected for performance | |||||
Temporal lobe, middle temporal gyrus | Right | 22 | 43 | 4.65 | 48, –46, 0 |
Occipital lobe, fusiform gyrus | Left | 18 | 35 | 4.28 | –24, –96, –10 |
Anterior insulaFootnote b | Right | 13 | 19 | 4.05 | 30, 4, 14 |
Parietal lobe, precuneus | Left | 7 | 21 | 3.94 | –14, –60, 40 |
Occipital lobe, inferior occipital gyrus | Right | 18 | 31 | 3.94 | 34, –90, –10 |
Parietal lobe, precuneus | 7 | 48 | 3.78 | 0, –50, 44 | |
Corrected for performance | |||||
Temporal lobe, middle temporal gyrus | Right | 22 | 21 | 4.20 | 48, –46, 0 |
Occipital lobe, fusiform gyrus | Left | 18 | 38 | 4.49 | –24, –94, –10 |
Anterior insula | Right | 13 | 23 | 4.41 | 28, 4, 16 |
Occipital lobe, lingual gyrus | Right | 19 | 53 | 3.87 | 14, –48, –4 |
a. One-way ANOVA at P<0.05, corrected, if not otherwise indicated.
b. P<0.001 uncorrected.
Side | Brodmann’s area | k | T | x, y, z | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Parietal lobe, precuneus | Left | 19 | 79 | 8.61 | –20, –82, 40 |
Cerebellum | Right | 154 | 7.11 | 4, –68, –24 | |
Frontal lobe, superior frontal gyrus | Right | 6 | 32 | 5.42 | 4, 30, 58 |
Frontal lobe, medial frontal gyrus | Left | 9 | 56 | 5.37 | –16, 40, 14 |
Temporal lobe, middle temporal gyrusFootnote b | Right | 41 | 16 | 5.25 | 42, –38, 16 |
Frontal lobe, superior frontal gyrus | Left | 6 | 77 | 5.21 | –2, 2, 66 |
Inferior parietal lobe | Left | 40 | 71 | 5.14 | –64, –32, 30 |
Posterior cingulate | Right | 31 | 79 | 5.1 | 24, –64, 14 |
Frontal lobe, precentral gyrus | Left | 6 | 151 | 4.99 | –42, –4, 54 |
Occipital lobe, cuneus | Right | 19 | 77 | 4.92 | 14, –80, 38 |
Middle insula | Left | 13 | 23 | 4.73 | –42, –10, 24 |
Parietal lobe, postcentral gyrus | Left | 3 | 42 | 4.67 | –34, –30, 66 |
Frontal lobe, precentral gyrus | Left | 6 | 23 | 4.18 | –52, 0, 44 |
a. Multiple regression at P<0.05, corrected, if not otherwise indicated.
b. P<0.001 uncorrected.
Discussion
Performance
Present results indicate that the schizophrenia group showed a worse performance in reward-related probabilistic trial-and-error learning as compared with the healthy control group. This was indicated by differences in percentage of correct responses as well as learning rates under fully predictable learning conditions. Results are roughly in line with previous studies revealing impaired performance for reward-related learning in schizophrenia. Reference Koch, Schachtzabel, Wagner, Schikora, Schultz and Reichenbach7,Reference Yilmaz, Simsek and Gonul39 It should be noted, however, that not only the magnitude of impairment in our previous study was larger, most likely because of a more difficult version of the task that was based on three instead of only two learning conditions, but also that results in the present study showed only a trend towards significance. As expected, for the condition under which learning was not possible (i.e. 50% probability), both groups revealed a similar percentage of correct responses with no significant differences between groups.
Functional connectivity
In the control group, within-group analysis revealed processing of positive reinforcement in terms of monetary reward to be associated with an increased connectivity between dorsal striatum (i.e. putamen) and fronto-occipital areas as well as caudate and anterior insula. In contrast, in the schizophrenia group, an increase in connectivity was mainly restricted to fronto-occipital areas. Comparison between groups revealed a reduced connectivity in the schizophrenia group between dorsal striatum and temporo-occipital areas as well as precuneus and insula. The findings suggest that a deficient interplay between the striatum and the outlined networks may be a mechanism underlying the accumulating evidence of reduced striatal activation in association with reward processing in people with schizophrenia. Reference Esslinger, Englisch, Inta, Rausch, Schirmbeck and Mier8-Reference Juckel, Schlagenhauf, Koslowski, Filonov, Wustenberg and Villringer12,Reference Schlagenhauf, Juckel, Koslowski, Kahnt, Knutson and Dembler14,Reference Schlagenhauf, Sterzer, Schmack, Ballmaier, Rapp and Wrase19,Reference Murray, Corlett, Clark, Pessiglione, Blackwell and Honey40 The results confirm recent initial findings of reduced functional connectivity in individuals with schizophrenia in the context of reward processing. In these studies, however, partly different networks were affected by reduced connectivity, namely ventral striatum and medial frontal cortex networks Reference Schlagenhauf, Sterzer, Schmack, Ballmaier, Rapp and Wrase19 as well as midbrain and the insula-anterior cingulate cortex salience network. Reference Gradin, Kumar, Waiter, Ahearn, Stickle and Milders10 Our results suggest that a reduced connectivity between dorsal striatum and temporo-occipital as well as insular areas may be of relevance. There is mounting evidence showing that it is predominantly the dorsal part of the striatum which is critically involved in response-related functions.
Side | Brodmann’s area | k | T | x, y, z | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Left putamen | |||||
Negative symptoms | |||||
Anterior cingulate | Left | 32 | 147 | 6.67 | 12, 42, –2 |
Anterior insula, inferior frontal gyrus | Right | 13/47 | 112 | 5.95 | 32, 20, –6 |
Occipital lobe, lingual gyrus | Right | 18 | 194 | 5.52 | 18, –78, 0 |
Frontal lobe, medial frontal gyrus | Left | 10 | 33 | 5.0 | –18, 48, –4 |
Anterior cingulate | Right | 24 | 81 | 4.92 | –8, 36, 4 |
Occipital lobe, inferior occipital gyrus | Left | 18 | 34 | 4.83 | 42, –84, –2 |
Blunted affect | |||||
Anterior cingulate | Right | 32 | 31 | 5.85 | 12, 42, –2 |
Posterior cingulate | Right | 31 | 51 | 5.23 | 8, –22, 40 |
Occipital lobe, inferior occipital gyrus | Left | 18 | 34 | 5.18 | –38, –86, –4 |
Anterior insula, inferior frontal gyrus | Right | 13/47 | 36 | 5.14 | 38, 16, –8 |
Anterior cingulate | Left | 24 | 112 | 4.96 | –12, 34, 4 |
Frontal lobe, superior frontal gyrus | Right | 10 | 20 | 4.95 | 24, 54, 0 |
Occipital lobe, inferior occipital gyrus | Right | 18 | 60 | 4.71 | 26, –84, –2 |
Right putamen | |||||
Negative symptoms | |||||
Anterior cingulate | Left | 24 | 147 | 6.67 | –2, 36, 8 |
Frontal lobe, middle frontal gyrus | Right | 9 | 112 | 5.95 | 34, 26, 34 |
Anterior cingulate | Right | 24 | 194 | 5.52 | 12, 34, 4 |
Frontal lobe, middle frontal gyrus | Left | 9 | 33 | 5.0 | –36, 20, 30 |
Blunted affect | |||||
Insula, inferior frontal gyrus | Left | 47 | 53 | 6.34 | –28, 24, 4 |
Frontal lobe, middle frontal gyrus | Left | 9 | 40 | 6.08 | –36, 20, 28 |
Frontal lobe, middle frontal gyrus | Right | 9 | 48 | 5.23 | 34, 28, 36 |
a. Multiple regression at P<0.05, corrected.
This evidence is supported by the fact that there are strong anatomical connections between the putamen and primary, premotor and supplementary motor cortices. Reference Seger41 Studies illustrating putamen activity in association with reward delivery and response towards receipt of reward are in line with the putative relevance of the putamen in association with behavioural response towards reward delivery. As indicated by earlier findings the ventral striatum, on the other hand, may be predominantly involved in the anticipation of rewards not necessarily going along with a direct behavioural response. Reference Cooper and Knutson42 Thus, our results showing a decreased connectivity between dorsal striatum and temporo-occipital as well as insular areas in people with schizophrenia in association with reward presentation and response towards it, fit the picture and indicate that a disruption within this behaviourally relevant dorsal striatal loop may constitute the basis for altered reward learning in people with schizophrenia.
Anatomically, vast connections between insula and dorsal as well as ventral portions of the striatum have been revealed in non-human primates and humans alike. Reference Chikama, McFarland, Amaral and Haber43 Converging evidence from resting state fMRI (rs-fMRI) in humans displayed networks linking insular and putamen. Reference Cauda, D'Agata, Sacco, Duca, Geminiani and Vercelli44 Functionally, the insula plays a role for a broad range of tasks as diverse as emotional processing, interoception, self-recognition, perceptual decision-making and others. A large body of evidence points towards a functional subdivision of insula in an anterior region, mainly engaged in emotional processing and interoception and a posterior region mainly engaged in multimodal sensory processing. Reference Wylie and Tregellas45 In a recent meta-analysis, Duerden et al Reference Duerden, Arsalidou, Lee and Taylor46 examined data from more than 140 experiments reporting insula activation connected to emotional processing. Highest activation was found in bilateral anterior insula, regardless of emotional valence. The decrease in functional connectivity between striatum and anterior insula found in the schizophrenia group in the present study may thus affect the responsivity to affective stimulation normally caused by positive reinforcement or reward and may explain characteristic symptoms of the disorder such as flat affect or lack of motivation.
Apart from extensive connections to the insula the putamen has strong connections to the precuneus as demonstrated in monkey studies. Reference Leichnetz47 Functionally, the precuneus has been linked to a variety of cognitive functions, such as episodic memory-retrieval, visuospatial imagery, consciousness and self-processing. A decrease in connectivity between striatum and precuneus may be a substrate of impaired cognitive processing. This assumption could be corroborated by correcting the connectivity differences for performance. After removing the influence of performance, the decreased connectivity between striatum and precuneus was no longer detectable. Taken together, results suggest that the impaired connectivity between striatum and anterior insula (mainly subserving functions such as emotion processing) as well as between striatum and precuneus (involved in cognitive processing) may have a direct influence on reinforcement-based learning as both, emotional and cognitive processing, are fundamental components of this kind of learning.
Association between connectivity and grey matter structure
A positive correlation between putamen grey matter volume and connectivity (i.e. the lower the grey matter volume, the lower the connectivity between putamen and fronto-occipital areas, precuneus as well as insula) was found in the schizophrenia group in largely those regions showing a decreased striatal connectivity in this group relative to the control group (see Fig. DS2). Hence, present results suggest that a disrupted functional interplay or connectivity between putamen and fronto-occipital areas, precuneus and insula is directly linked or may even constitute the consequence of an alteration in putaminal grey matter structure.
The results thus corroborate first evidence showing an association between functional connectivity and grey or white matter structural connectivity in partly overlapping regions in people with schizophrenia. As indicated by the group comparison of grey matter values extracted from the striatal seed region individuals with schizophrenia in the present study had significantly lower putaminal grey matter volumes relative to the control group. Previous studies on structural alterations of putamen are rather heterogeneous. The majority of studies reported increases in putaminal volume, Reference Henze, Brunner, Thiemann, Parzer, Richterich and Essig21-Reference Kawasaki, Suzuki, Kherif, Takahashi, Zhou and Nakamura24 which have been linked to long-term antipsychotic treatment Reference Ho, Andreasen, Ziebell, Pierson and Magnotta25 whereas some studies also found decreased volumes Reference Molina, Galindo, Cortes, de Herrera, Ledo and Sanz26,Reference Ballmaier, Schlagenhauf, Toga, Gallinat, Koslowski and Zoli27 or no volume alterations. Reference Keshavan, Rosenberg, Sweeney and Pettegrew28 A recent meta-analysis of 77 studies on schizophrenia, Reference Bora, Fornito, Radua, Walterfang, Seal and Wood48 in which most of the included patients were treated with atypical antipsychotics, revealed no significant effect of antipsychotic treatment on global grey volume. Although we found no correlation between chlorpromazine equivalent dosage and striatal grey matter volume, effects of (previous) long-term antipsychotic treatment on striatal grey matter volume cannot be ruled out. However, independently of the influence of current or previous antipsychotic treatment on striatal grey matter structure, present data suggest a close linkage between striatal functional connectivity and striatal grey matter volume.
Association between connectivity and psychopathology
In addition, a negative correlation was found between the magnitude of negative symptoms as well as blunted affect and functional connectivity, i.e. the higher the magnitude of negative symptoms and blunted affect, the lower the connectivity between left putamen, anterior cingulate, insula and (ventromedial) fronto-occipital areas as well as right putamen, anterior cingulate, insula and middle frontal areas. Here, the significant result in the insula and the anterior cingulate, which together constitute the so-called salience network, may be of major psychopathological relevance given the hypothesis that a disruption within the salience network may constitute the neuronal substrate of blunted affect and passivity in schizophrenia. Reference Palaniyappan and Liddle18
Gradin and colleagues’ study has recently demonstrated the relevance of functional interplay of midbrain-insular networks with regard to psychopathology. Reference Gradin, Waiter, O'Connor, Romaniuk, Stickle and Matthews20 They found a reduced functional connectivity between midbrain and insula in schizophrenia to correlate with the psychopathological status of the patients. As opposed to our findings, however, Gradin et al detected a significant correlation with severity of psychotic symptoms but no correlation with negative symptoms. More in line with the present findings are recent results by Manoliu et al Reference Manoliu, Riedl, Doll, Bauml, Muhlau and Schwerthoffer49 that showed, among others, that a decreased functional connectivity of the left anterior insula correlated with severity of negative symptoms in a sample of people with remitted schizophrenia.
Orliac and colleagues Reference Orliac, Naveau, Joliot, Delcroix, Razafimandimby and Brazo50 revealed a decreased functional connectivity in the left and right striatum in a sample of patients with schizophrenia. Somewhat in agreement with our findings they discovered a significant association between connectivity decrease in the left striatum and delusion as well as depression scores. Hence, there is increasing evidence that a disrupted connectivity within reward-related or salience networks is closely linked to characteristic psychopathological symptoms of the disorder.
Whether the impairment in connectivity represents a psychopathological state marker or is to be interpreted in light of an underlying cause of pathology is, however, difficult to determine. Although the significant correlation with the current status or degree of negative symptoms seems to speak in favour of a state marker, the hypothesis that negative symptoms, as a result of an inappropriate allocation of salience to internal representations and external events, constitute the phenomenological consequence of altered neuronal activation in the salience network, would support the latter interpretation. As these questions are difficult to answer given the design of the present study, further, ideally longitudinal studies focusing on the outlined networks should make an attempt to illuminate the significance of altered connectivity within these networks for the psychopathology of schizophrenia.
Limitations
Given the potential effects of antipsychotic treatment on striatal connectivity and structure the fact that the schizophrenia group were taking medication limits the explanatory power of the present study to some degree. In summary, present results suggest a disruption in the functional connectivity of the striatum that is closely linked to striatal volume in people with schizophrenia and that may explain the emergence of negative symptoms.
eLetters
No eLetters have been published for this article.