Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-lnqnp Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T15:05:44.554Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Childhood adversity and adolescent mental health: Examining cumulative and specificity effects across contexts and developmental timing

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  03 October 2024

Felicia A. Hardi
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI, USA Yale University, New Haven, CT, USA
Melissa K. Peckins
Affiliation:
St. John’s University, New York, NY, USA
Colter Mitchell
Affiliation:
Survey Research Center of the Institute for Social Research, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI, USA Population Studies Center of the Institute for Social Research, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI, USA
Vonnie McLoyd
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI, USA
Jeanne Brooks-Gunn
Affiliation:
Teachers College, Columbia University, New York, NY, USA College of Physicians and Surgeons, Columbia University, New York, NY, USA
Luke W. Hyde
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI, USA Survey Research Center of the Institute for Social Research, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI, USA
Christopher S. Monk*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI, USA Survey Research Center of the Institute for Social Research, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI, USA Neuroscience Graduate Program, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI, USA Department of Psychiatry, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI, USA
*
Corresponding author: Christopher S. Monk; Email: csmonk@umich.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Associations between adversity and youth psychopathology likely vary based on the types and timing of experiences. Major theories suggest that the impact of childhood adversity may either be cumulative in type (the more types of adversity, the worse outcomes) or in timing (the longer exposure, the worse outcomes) or, alternatively, specific concerning the type (e.g., parenting, home, neighborhood) or the timing of adversity (e.g., specific developmental periods). In a longitudinal sample from the Future of Families and Wellbeing Study (N = 4,210), we evaluated these competing hypotheses using a data-driven structured life-course modeling approach using risk factors examined at child age 1 (infancy), 3 (toddlerhood), 5 (early childhood), and 9 (middle childhood). Results showed that exposures to more types of adversity for longer durations (i.e., cumulative in both type and timing) best predicted youth psychopathology. Adversities that occurred at age 9 were better predictors of youth psychopathology as compared to those experienced earlier, except for neglect, which was predictive of internalizing symptoms when experienced at age 3. Throughout childhood (across ages 1–9), aside from the accumulation of all adversities, parental stress and low collective efficacy were the strongest predictors of internalizing symptoms, whereas psychological aggression was predictive of externalizing symptoms.

Type
Regular Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Extensive research has established that adverse experiences during development are associated with a host of poor outcomes, including risk for psychopathology (Cicchetti, Reference Cicchetti2016). Given the high prevalence of adversity during childhood (Kessler et al., Reference Kessler, Davis and Kendler1997), considerable efforts have been made to understand how childhood adversities can serve as antecedents of psychopathology. Over several decades, many studies have focused on disentangling both the additive and distinct contributions of different types of adversities experienced at various developmental periods. Much of this work has focused on internalizing and externalizing symptoms during adolescence, as this is a period when many of the most significant symptoms of mental disorders emerge (Kessler et al., Reference Kessler, Berglund, Demler, Jin, Merikangas and Walters2005). By identifying salient risk factors for youth mental health, this research aims to advance our understanding of the nature of the associations between childhood adversity and youth psychopathology, which can, in turn, inform intervention and prevention efforts.

The accumulation of different types of risk factors

Research on the cumulative risk model of adversity, which posits that mental health risks increase with the number of adversities that children experience (Figure 1), began with early seminal work such as the Isle of Wight (Rutter et al., Reference Rutter, Kent and Rolf1979) and the Rochester Longitudinal Study (Sameroff & Seifer, Reference Sameroff and Seifer1995). These studies found that exposure to a greater number of environmental and family factors (e.g., marital problems, maternal stress and psychopathology, instability) was associated with greater psychopathology in adolescents. Guided by the ecological systems models (Bronfenbrenner & Morris, Reference Bronfenbrenner and Morris2007), subsequent research expanded this work to include a broader array of risk factors across multiple contexts (e.g., parenting, family, neighborhood). These work provide evidence that a wide range of experiences such as harsh parenting, family violence, maternal stress, parental psychopathology, and financial hardship (e.g., Appleyard et al., Reference Appleyard, Egeland, van Dulmen and Alan Sroufe2005; Atkinson et al., Reference Atkinson, Beitchman, Gonzalez, Young, Wilson, Escobar, Chisholm, Brownlie, Khoury, Ludmer, Villani and Eapen2015; Buehler & Gerard, Reference Buehler and Gerard2013; Trentacosta et al., Reference Trentacosta, Hyde, Shaw, Dishion, Gardner and Wilson2008) can have a combined effect on children’s development (see review: Evans et al., Reference Evans, Li and Whipple2013).

Figure 1. Theoretical illustration of the cumulative and specificity models of adversity. The cumulative model (left) suggests that the effects of childhood adversity on socioemotional development would increase with increasing number of adversity and longer exposures to various adverse experiences, up to a specific level whereby more exposures may not yield any additional effect. On the other hand, in the specificity model (right), each adverse experience is hypothesized to have a distinct effect on socioemotional development. The magnitude of influence (i.e., the height of the curves) of a specific adversity may vary across the different types of adverse experiences and when during development they occur (i.e., where they lie on the x-axis). These theoretical models provide a basis for much research on the effect of multiple types of adversity on youth mental health.

The accumulation of risk across multiple developmental periods

Taking a cumulative approach to understanding the effects of childhood adversity can also be extended to the consideration of the timing at which adverse events occur (Figure 1). For instance, many early studies on child maltreatment found that prolonged exposures to adversity (i.e., exposures to risks across multiple developmental periods) predicted greater youth psychopathology (Cicchetti & Toth, Reference Cicchetti and Toth1995; Egeland & Sroufe, Reference Egeland and Sroufe1981). Similar findings have been observed in more recent studies. For example, increased behavioral problems were more frequently observed in children who experienced maltreatment across multiple developmental periods, rather than specific time points (Dunn et al., Reference Dunn, Soare, Raffeld, Busso, Crawford, Davis, Fisher, Slopen, Smith, Tiemeier and Susser2018; Jaffee & Maikovich-Fong, Reference Jaffee and Maikovich-Fong2011; Thompson et al., Reference Thompson, Hannan and Miron2014; Thornberry et al., Reference Thornberry, Ireland and Smith2001). These findings collectively demonstrate that the effects of adversity on mental health may be proportional to the duration of exposure.

Specificity effect of childhood adversity across different types of events

Though understanding the cumulative effect of adversity across experiences and developmental periods is important, cumulative risk research struggles to elucidate the precise mechanisms by which adversity can lead to psychopathology because the risk factors themselves are examined collectively. Thus, in parallel, a host of research has focused on understanding the effect of specific adversities on specific psychopathology outcomes. For example, parenting practices have been found to be a critical factor for youth socioemotional development (Eisenberg et al., Reference Eisenberg, Cumberland and Spinrad1998; Morris et al., Reference Morris, Criss, Silk and Houltberg2017; Rutherford et al., Reference Rutherford, Wallace, Laurent and Mayes2015). Both harsh parenting, characterized by coercion and aggression, and neglectful parenting, characterized by minimal attention and supervision, are robust risk factors for internalizing and externalizing outcomes (Chang et al., Reference Chang, Schwartz, Dodge and McBride-Chang2003; Hoffman-Plotkin & Twentyman, Reference Hoffman-Plotkin and Twentyman1984; Shaw & Bell, Reference Shaw and Bell1993; Trickett & McBride-Chang, Reference Trickett and McBride-Chang1995). These parenting adversities increase the risk of internalizing symptoms by imposing stress and unpredictability, which disrupts the biological stress response (Loman & Gunnar, Reference Loman and Gunnar2010; Repetti et al., Reference Repetti, Taylor and Seeman2002), while increasing the risk of externalizing behaviors by modeling aggression and interfering with the development of emotion regulation (Shaw & Bell, Reference Shaw and Bell1993).

Similarly, other adversities within the home environment can also present risks for later psychopathology by undermining socioemotional development. For instance, witnessing intimate partner violence exposes children to aggression and violence and models maladaptive conflict resolution and poor emotion regulation (Artz et al., Reference Artz, Jackson, Rossiter, Nijdam-Jones, Géczy and Porteous2014; Carpenter & Stacks, Reference Carpenter and Stacks2009). Parental psychological issues, such as maternal depression and parental stress, challenge parents’ ability to be warm and responsive, which undermines the development of secure parent–child relationships (Cummings & Davies, Reference Cummings and Davies1994; Goodman et al., Reference Goodman, Rouse, Connell, Broth, Hall and Heyward2011; McQuillan & Bates, Reference McQuillan, Bates, Deater-Deckard and Panneton2017). Frequent residential changes and material hardship have also been identified as risk factors for youth behavioral problems (Belsky et al., Reference Belsky, Schlomer and Ellis2011; Doom et al., Reference Doom, Vanzomeren-Dohm and Simpson2016; Gershoff et al., Reference Gershoff, Aber, Raver and Lennon2007), potentially because they increase distress for parents and introduce repeated ecological changes that children must adapt to. Thus, multiple adversities within the home can disrupt important child (e.g., development of emotion regulation) and dyadic processes (e.g., establishing a supportive parent–child relationship), which in turn can increase the risk for psychopathology.

In addition to family-level processes, neighborhood risk factors, such as low community support and high neighborhood violence, increase the risk of internalizing symptoms by exposing children to unpredictable threats and violent crimes (Sampson et al., Reference Sampson, Raudenbush and Earls1997) and the risk of externalizing symptoms by providing opportunities for negative peer influence (Cantillon, Reference Cantillon2006). Collectively, prior studies have demonstrated how different types of adverse events can have a specific influence on children’s socioemotional development through multiple pathways and processes, and this knowledge has been key to informing approaches to prevent or treat youth psychopathology (Dodge, Reference Dodge2020).

Changes in children’s environments across different developmental periods

The risks of each type of adversity for mental health may also differ depending on when during development these experiences occur. Childhood is marked by vast changes in the child’s social and biological contexts within a short period of time. While scholars use different terms to describe various developmental periods, in the present study and subsequent discussion, we refer to data collected at age 1 as occurring during infancy, age 3 as toddlerhood, age 5 as early childhood, and age 9 as middle childhood, consistent with previous categorizations of this sample (James et al., Reference James, McLanahan and Brooks-Gunn2021).

During infancy, children are focused on learning basic processes and establishing secure attachments with their caregivers (Greenberg et al., Reference Greenberg, Cicchetti and Cummings1990). From toddlerhood through the early preschool years, there are rapid changes in language, motor skills, and emotion processing. During this period, children begin to move independently and explore with increasing autonomy, while also learning to manage their “big” emotions and desire to maintain closeness with their caregivers. This can create significant challenges for parents as they navigate setting new boundaries, all while ensuring their children’s safety (Gardner & Shaw, Reference Gardner and Shaw2009). Another shift occurs when children begin to transition into formal schooling at around age 5. During this transition to elementary school, children begin spending more time out of the home, in school and neighborhood settings (Hofferth & Sandberg, Reference Hofferth and Sandberg2001; Leventhal & Brooks-Gunn, Reference Leventhal and Brooks-Gunn2000). As children approach the end of elementary school (e.g., around age 9), they prepare for another major contextual change of transitioning into middle school. This period also marks the onset of major biological changes, including puberty (in the present sample, 89% of girls and 81% of boys had begun pubertal development at age 9) and rapid development of brain regions critical to emotion and self-control (Blakemore, Reference Blakemore2012). Socially, parents are more likely to allow children to explore their neighborhoods more independently (Smetana et al., Reference Smetana, Campione-Barr and Metzger2006), and there is an increasing focus on peer groups compared to earlier ages, expanding the focus of social development beyond caregivers (Rubin et al., Reference Rubin, Coplan, Chen, Bowker and McDonald2011).

These shifts across developmental periods have prompted research into how different types of experiences contribute to early risk for psychopathology, depending on the childhood period (i.e., infancy, toddlerhood, early childhood, middle childhood) during which the exposure occurs. Early in childhood, emotional development highly relies on parental influence; thus, disruptions in parent–child relationships during this period can be particularly impactful (Zeanah et al., Reference Zeanah, Boris and Larrieu1997). For instance, childhood maltreatment (Cummings & Davies, Reference Cummings and Davies1994; Goodman et al., Reference Goodman, Rouse, Connell, Broth, Hall and Heyward2011; Hankin, Reference Hankin2005; Lee & Hankin, Reference Lee and Hankin2009), parental stressors (Crnic & Greenberg, Reference Crnic and Greenberg1990), and conflicts (Emery, Reference Emery1982) at early ages can compromise parent–child bonds, foster insecure attachment in children, and produce cascading risks across childhood that could subsequently lead to internalizing and externalizing problems. As children mature from middle childhood into adolescence and begin to spend more time with their peers and engage socially outside of the home, it is hypothesized that neighborhood-level adversities can play a larger role for older children (Kleinepier & van Ham, Reference Kleinepier and van Ham2018).

At the same time, other evidence suggests that these factors may have similar influences across different developmental periods. For instance, although it is commonly believed that children’s environments expand beyond the home as they get older, neighborhood factors can be just as critical in early childhood. Affluent neighborhoods, for instance, offer young children access to resources and academic enrichment that promote school readiness (Anderson et al., Reference Anderson, Johnston and Leventhal2019). Similarly, disruptions in caregiving can undermine children’s emotional development during later childhood just as much as during earlier childhood. Children continue to rely on parents for security well beyond the early childhood period while learning to balance their competing desires for independence (Collins et al., Reference Collins, Harris and Susman1995). Environmental instability, such as frequent residential changes or material hardship (e.g., eviction or food insecurity), is proposed to have detrimental effects regardless of whether it occurs in early or late childhood. For example, residential moves in early childhood are particularly impactful on children’s development due to changes within the family context (Anderson et al., Reference Anderson, Leventhal, Newman and Dupéré2014; Gillespie, Reference Gillespie2013; Heinlein & Shinn, Reference Heinlein and Shinn2000). In contrast, moves later in childhood can disrupt children’s routines, especially when they involve changes in multiple environments, such as switching schools or social circles (Anastasio & Leventhal, Reference Anastasio and Leventhal2023; Swanson & Schneider, Reference Swanson and Schneider1999).

Sensitive periods of development

Beyond the idea that specific types of adversities may be more or less salient at different developmental periods, variations in the effects of childhood adversity over time could also be due to changes in the child’s sensitivity to their environment (i.e., sensitive periods), rather than just changes within the child’s environment. Sensitive periods refer to times when environmental influences exert the greatest impact on development, a concept rooted in early neurobiological research using animal models. For instance, Hubel and Wiesel demonstrated that depriving kittens of visual stimuli shortly after birth impairs their long-term visual system, whereas similar deprivation later in development leaves their sensory ability intact (Hubel & Wiesel, Reference Hubel and Wiesel1970). These findings motivated human researchers to better understand whether there are specific sensitive periods during which adversity has a particularly profound impact.

Consistent with this notion, some evidence suggests that early childhood may represent a sensitive period for adversity. For example, the seminal Bucharest Early Intervention Project revealed that removing children from institutional care became markedly less beneficial after the age of 2 (McLaughlin et al., Reference McLaughlin, Sheridan, Tibu, Fox, Zeanah and Nelson2015), underscoring the critical role of early caregiving for long-term socioemotional development. Similarly, in a study of maltreated children, childhood maltreatment, particularly before age 5, was found to predict the greatest risk for depression (Dunn et al., Reference Dunn, McLaughlin, Slopen, Rosand and Smoller2013; Kaplow & Widom, Reference Kaplow and Widom2007). From a neurodevelopmental perspective, early childhood is a period of rapid development for critical neural structures involved in affective processing (Tottenham & Sheridan, Reference Tottenham and Sheridan2010). In one study, for instance, maternal support during preschool (ages 3–6), but not school-aged (4–12), was associated with higher increases in hippocampal volume (Luby et al., Reference Luby, Belden, Harms, Tillman and Barch2016). Similarly, in a subsample of the current sample, harsh parenting at age 3 (but not later ages) was related to differences in amygdala reactivity (Gard et al., Reference Gard, Hein, Mitchell, Brooks-Gunn, McLanahan, Monk and Hyde2021). Taken together, these studies provide support that adversity experienced during early childhood may be particularly important for children’s socioemotional development.

Though theory and these individual studies suggest that early childhood may be a sensitive period for adversity, other studies have questioned this notion. For instance, in one systematic review, Brett and colleagues found limited evidence supporting specific sensitive periods related to child maltreatment, noting that heightened responses to maltreatment have been associated with both early and late exposures (Brett et al., Reference Brett, Humphreys, Fleming, Kraemer and Drury2015). Contrary to the evidence on early childhood sensitive periods, studies have also found that maltreatment experienced later in childhood was more strongly associated with youth psychopathology than maltreatment experienced in early childhood (Dunn et al., Reference Dunn, Busso, Davis, Smith, Mitchell, Tiemeier and Susser2023, Reference Dunn, Soare, Raffeld, Busso, Crawford, Davis, Fisher, Slopen, Smith, Tiemeier and Susser2018; Thornberry et al., Reference Thornberry, Henry, Ireland and Smith2010). Notably, in one study, Dunn and colleagues examined sensitive periods of child maltreatment in the same cohort as the present study. They found that harsh parenting was most strongly associated with parent report of youth internalizing and externalizing symptoms when experienced at age 9 for girls and age 5 for boys (Dunn et al., Reference Dunn, Busso, Davis, Smith, Mitchell, Tiemeier and Susser2023). These findings suggest that heightened vulnerability to adversity may occur later in childhood. However, more research is needed to determine whether these findings extend to different types of adverse experiences and if similar results can be obtained using youth-reported measures of psychopathology.

Theory-informed, data-driven approaches

These various strands of research emphasizing the cumulative or potentially specific type and timing effects of adversity highlight the need to better understand the nature of the associations between adversity and youth psychopathology. While previous studies have examined the differential associations of adversity and psychopathology, as well as their cumulative effects (e.g., Shaw et al., Reference Shaw, Winslow, Owens, Vondra, Cohn and Bell1998), few studies have directly compared these theoretical models within a single framework. For instance, studies have either explored the cumulative effects of multiple types of adverse experiences or the effects of adversity across multiple developmental time points or, in separate analyses, explored the associations of a few adverse experiences at specific time points with mental disorders in children and adolescents. However, to determine if a cumulative risk index could account for the majority of individual differences in internalizing and externalizing outcomes, a direct comparison of these models is needed. This comparison can help to disentangle whether the influence of childhood adversity primarily manifests as cumulative effects or whether certain developmental time points or experiences are especially detrimental.

One novel way to address these competing models is by leveraging a quantitative method called the Structured Life-Course Modelling Approach (SLCMA) (Smith et al., Reference Smith, Hardy, Heron, Joinson, Lawlor, Macdonald-Wallis and Tilling2016, Reference Smith, Heron, Mishra, Gilthorpe, Ben-Shlomo and Tilling2015; Smith et al., Reference Smith, Smith, Dunn and Andersen2022). SLCMA is a theory-informed and data-driven method that leverages the least angle variable selection regression approach to identify the life-course hypothesis (e.g., cumulative, sensitive period) that could be best supported by the available data. In previous investigations, this method was used to examine the developmental impact of maternal depression (Lacey et al., Reference Lacey, Gondek, Smith, Smith, Dunn and Sacker2023) and child maltreatment (Crawford et al., Reference Crawford, Choi, Davis, Zhu, Soare, Smith, Germine and Dunn2022; Dunn et al., Reference Dunn, Busso, Davis, Smith, Mitchell, Tiemeier and Susser2023, Reference Dunn, Soare, Raffeld, Busso, Crawford, Davis, Fisher, Slopen, Smith, Tiemeier and Susser2018, Reference Dunn, Soare, Zhu, Simpkin, Suderman, Klengel, Smith, Ressler and Relton2019) on mental health. These studies directly compared the cumulative effect of repeated exposures to these adversities (across multiple time points) with their impact at specific developmental ages (i.e., early or late childhood sensitive periods). However, this approach has not been used broadly to assess the effect of an array of adversities across a range of developmental periods.

Moreover, further research is needed to compare these models in a large, well-sampled representative cohort. One of the many challenges of studying questions related to the nature of the associations between adversity and mental health is identifying a suitable sample for testing these disparate models. To accomplish this goal, a longitudinal cohort is needed that includes data on a wide range of risks, with continuous distributions of adversity and psychopathology, and a strong sampling frame for generalizability, particularly a cohort with increased representation of individuals exposed to elevated risks (i.e., a population with greater exposure).

A final consideration is the potential “shape” of the association between cumulative adversity over time and type. Originally, two types of cumulative risk models were proposed. Rutter and colleagues found that exposure to more risk factors, up to a certain number, incrementally increased problem behaviors (Rutter et al., Reference Rutter, Kent and Rolf1979). These findings are consistent with the landmark epidemiological study on adverse childhood experiences (Felitti et al., Reference Felitti, Anda, Nordenberg, Williamson, Spitz, Edwards and Marks1998), which suggests that a specific threshold of risk exposures can have the greatest consequences on health. This quadratic cumulative model contrasts with other research that demonstrates a linear pattern, proposing a continuous escalation of maladaptive outcomes with exposure to a greater number of risk factors (Sameroff, Reference Sameroff1998). Thus, investigations into the potentially cumulative effects of adversity also need to test whether models are linear or show ceiling/quadradic effects.

Present investigation

The present study aims to test the cumulative and specificity effects of adversity across a range of childhood periods and contexts to prospectively predict youth internalizing and externalizing problems using a novel theory-informed data-driven approach in a diverse, longitudinal, population-based sample spanning 15 years. We focused on risk factors with previously established and robust links to internalizing and externalizing problems using measures of parenting practices, home environment, and neighborhood environment. In terms of hypotheses, we first anticipated a stronger support for a cumulative risk model across both timing and types, indicating that risk exposures over an extended period, as well as across a greater number of risks, would have the largest impact on youth internalizing and externalizing problems. Additionally, we hypothesized that there would be differential associations relating to timing (sensitive periods) and adversity type (adversities that pose meaningful influence beyond cumulative effect). We posited that adverse experiences occurring in the earlier years of development (ages 1 and/or 3), which are critical for establishing the building blocks of emotion processing, would exert the most substantial influence on mental health compared to adversity experienced at later periods in childhood. Concerning adversity-specific associations, we hypothesized that adversities related to caregiving and the home environment would have a greater influence during early childhood but that neighborhood-related adversities would have a greater influence during later childhood.

Methods

Participants

This study used data from the Future of Families and Child Wellbeing Study (FFCWS), a population-based birth-cohort sample of 4,898 children born in large cities with populations over 200,000 in the United States, with a 3:1 oversampling ratio for nonmarital births (Reichman et al., Reference Reichman, Teitler, Garfinkel and McLanahan2001). Children were followed throughout childhood and adolescence, and data were collected at multiple time points (child’s birth, age 1, 3, 5, 9, and 15). In the current investigation, children who were reported to not live with mother at least half of the time during any waves (n = 290) and those with data collected at fewer than two time points across included waves in any predictors of interest (i.e., adversity measures) were excluded (n = 398), yielding a final sample of N = 4,210 (47% females, 49% Black, 25% Hispanic, 18% White, 8% other/multiracial; $22,500 median household income at child’s birth) for analysis. The included sample (N = 4,210) does not differ from the full sample (N = 4,898) across key demographic variables (Supplemental Table 1).

Measures

Childhood adversity

Ten variables were examined as indicators of childhood adversity experienced across 4 waves within the child’s first 9 years (collected when the child was 1, 3, 5, and 9 years of age). These variables were examined in previous investigations (Gard et al., Reference Gard, Hein, Mitchell, Brooks-Gunn, McLanahan, Monk and Hyde2021; Goetschius, Hein, McLanahan, et al., Reference Goetschius, Hein, McLanahan, Brooks-Gunn, McLoyd, Dotterer, Lopez-Duran, Mitchell, Hyde, Monk and Beltz2020; Goetschius, Hein, Mitchell, et al., Reference Goetschius, Hein, Mitchell, Lopez-Duran, McLoyd, Brooks-Gunn, McLanahan, Hyde and Monk2020; Hardi et al., Reference Hardi, Goetschius, Peckins, Brooks-Gunn, McLanahan, McLoyd, Lopez-Duran, Mitchell and Monk2022, Reference Hardi, Goetschius, Tillem, McLoyd, Brooks-Gunn, Boone, Lopez-Duran, Mitchell, Hyde and Monk2023; Hein et al., Reference Hein, Goetschius, McLoyd, Brooks-Gunn, McLanahan, Mitchell, Lopez-Duran, Hyde and Monk2020; Peckins et al., Reference Peckins, Roberts, Hein, Hyde, Mitchell, Brooks-Gunn, McLanahan, Monk and Lopez-Duran2020) and were selected to represent parenting, home, and neighborhood factors that contribute to youth mental health. Included predictors in the present study were measures of parenting, including (a) physical aggression, (b) psychological aggression, and (c) child neglect; parent or family factors: (d) intimate partner violence, (e) maternal depression, and (f) parental stress; indicators of instability and disadvantage within the home: (g) residential moves and (h) household material hardship; and aspects of the neighborhood: (i) low collective efficacy, and (j) community violence.

Parenting behaviors were measured using parent responses on the Parent–Child Conflict Tactics Scale (Straus et al., Reference Straus, Hamby, Finkelhor, Moore and Runyan1998) when the child was 3, 5, and 9 years of age. In accordance with recommendations for the scale (Straus et al., Reference Straus, Hamby, Finkelhor, Moore and Runyan1998), physical aggression, psychological aggression, and neglect were treated as separate constructs. Physical aggression was measured by five questions from the physical assault subscale capturing the frequency that the primary caregiver reported having engaged in behaviors such as “spanked [child] on the bottom with their bare hand” or “hit [child] on the bottom with something like a belt, hairbrush, a stick or some other hard object” in the past year. Psychological aggression was measured using five questions capturing the past year frequency that the caregiver reported having engaged in behaviors such as “shouted, yelled, or screamed at” or “swore or cursed at” the child. Neglect was measured by five questions capturing past year frequency that the caregiver reported having engaged in behaviors such as “had to leave their child home alone, even when they thought some adult should be with him/her” or “was not able to make sure their child got to a doctor or hospital when he/she needed it.” Responses to each question were coded using a dichotomous variable (0 = did not happen, 1 = has happened one or more times) (Straus et al., Reference Straus, Hamby, Finkelhor, Moore and Runyan1998), and scores at each wave were summed to compute the developmental timing-specific measures of physical aggression (age 3 α = .49; age 5 α = .52; age 9 α = .62), psychological aggression (age 3 α = .53; age 5 α = .53; age 9 α = .63), and neglect (age 3 α = .51; age 5 α = .50; age 9 α = .58).

Intimate partner violence (IPV) was measured using the mother’s report on five questions about relationship quality such as “how often does father slap or kick you?” or “how often does father try to isolate you from friends/family?” when the child was 1, 3, 5, and 9 years of age, consistent with prior research (Hunt et al., Reference Hunt, Berger and Slack2017). Responses were coded as 0 = never, 1 = sometimes, 3 = often, where higher scores indicate a greater prevalence of IPV. In cases where the mother was no longer with the biological father of the child, the mother reported information about her current partner instead. IPV was computed at each developmental age by summing all items at each wave (age 1 α = .63; age 3 α = .64; age 5 α = .66; age 9 α = .69).

Two variables, maternal depression and parental stress, were examined as separate factors to represent parental mental health. Maternal depression was measured using the mother’s self-report on the Composite International Diagnostic Interview – Short Form (CIDI-SF) (Kessler et al., Reference Kessler, Andrews, Mroczek, Ustun and Wittchen1998) when the child was 1, 3, 5, and 9 years of age. The CIDI-SF, consistent with the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders – Fourth Edition (Bell, Reference Bell1994), included questions measuring whether the mother had feelings of depressed mood or anhedonia (loss of pleasure or interest in activities that they usually found enjoyable) in the past year that lasted 2 weeks or more. If so, they were asked more detailed questions about losing interest, tiredness, changes in weight, sleep, concentration, worthlessness, and any suicidal ideation. Maternal depression was dichotomously coded (1 = yes, 0 = no), and diagnostic criteria were met if the mother endorsed a depressed mood or anhedonia lasting at least half of the day nearly every day, along with two or more additional symptoms.

Parental stress was measured using four questions adapted from the Child Development Supplement of the Panel of Study of Income Dynamics (Hofferth et al., Reference Hofferth, Davis-Kean, Davis and Finkelstein1997) such as “I often feel tired, worn out, or exhausted from raising a family” and “I feel trapped by my responsibilities as a parent.” These questions measured parenting stress triggered by changes in employment, income, or other factors and were taken from the Parent Stress Inventory (Abidin et al., Reference Abidin, Flens and Austin2006). The scale was administered when the child was 1, 3, 5, and 9 years of age, and the mother’s responses to the questions were coded on a 4-point scale (0 = strongly disagree, 1 = somewhat disagree, 2 = somewhat agree, 3 = strongly agree), where higher scores indicate greater reported parental stress. Parental stress at each developmental age was computed by summing all items at each wave (age 1 α = .62; age 3 α = .64; age 5 α = .67; age 9 α = .67).

Frequent residential moves or household instability have been related to difficulties in adjustments for children (Adam, Reference Adam2004; Leventhal & Newman, Reference Leventhal and Newman2010). When the child was 1, 3, 5, and 9 years of age, mothers or primary caregivers were asked whether the family had moved since the last data collection, and if yes, how many times. Residential instability at each developmental age was measured by the frequency of moves between each wave and was coded as 0 if the family had not moved since the last wave.

Material hardship was measured by eight-item binary questions (1 = yes, 0 = no) when the child was 1, 3, 5, and 9 years of age using parent reports on whether the family experienced housing, utility, food, medical, and financial hardship in the past year. Items were selected from the 1996 Survey of Income and Program Participation; the 1997 and 1999 New York City Social Indicators Survey, and the 1999 Study of Work, Welfare, and Family Well-Being of Iowa families (Bauman, Reference Bauman1999; Mayer & Jencks, Reference Mayer and Jencks1989). Scores were summed to represent material hardship at each age (age 1 α = .61; age 3 α = .62; age 5 α = .62; age 9 α = .61).

Low collective efficacy was measured using questions capturing cohesion and social control within the neighborhood when the child was 3, 5, and 9 years of age. Neighborhood cohesion was measured using four questions taken from the Social Cohesion and Trust Scale (Sampson et al., Reference Sampson, Raudenbush and Earls1997; Sampson, Reference Sampson1997) such as “people around here are willing to help their neighbors” and “this is a close-knit neighborhood”. Responses were coded as 3 = strongly disagree, 2 = disagree, 1 = agree, 0 = strongly agree, where higher scores indicate a greater low neighborhood cohesion. Neighborhood social control was measured using four questions taken from the Informal Social Control Scale (Sampson et al., Reference Sampson, Raudenbush and Earls1997; Sampson, Reference Sampson1997) such as “likely neighbors intervene if children skipping school and hanging on street?” and “how likely neighbors intervene if fight broke out in front of the house?” Responses were coded as 3 = very unlikely, 2 = not very unlikely, 1 = somewhat, 0 = very likely, where higher scores indicate a greater low neighborhood informal social control. Low collective efficacy at each developmental age was measured by summing all items of neighborhood cohesion and social control at each wave (age 3 α = .84; age 5 α = .87; age 9 α = .87).

Exposure to community violence was measured using three questions administered to the primary caregiver when the child was 3, 5, and 9 years of age about neighborhood conditions such as “in the past year, how often did you see person get hit, slapped, punched?” and “in the past year, how often did you see person attacked with weapon?” Responses were coded on a 4-point scale (0 = never, 1 = once, 2 = 2–3 times, 3 = 4–10 times, 4 = more than 10 times). Exposure to community violence at each developmental age was computed by summing all items at each wave (age 3 α = .74; age 5 α = .76; age 9 α = .77).

Adolescent mental health outcomes

Internalizing and externalizing problems at age 15 were modeled separately as latent factors using item-level data of all available youth-report measures of internalizing and externalizing symptoms. Confirmatory factor analysis was conducted using MPlus v8.8 (Muthén & Muthén, Reference Muthén and Muthén2017) with a WLSMV estimator.

Adolescent internalizing symptoms were measured using a latent factor score of youth responses to anxiety and depressive scales collected at age 15: six items from the Brief Symptom Inventory 18 (BSI-18) (Derogatis & Kathryn, Reference Derogatis and Kathryn2000) and five items from the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale (CES-D) (Radloff, Reference Radloff1977) (α = .84). The BSI-18 questions were coded on a 4-point scale (0 = strongly disagree, 1 = somewhat disagree, 2 = somewhat agree, 3 = strongly agree), where higher scores indicate greater self-reported anxiety. The CES-D questions were coded on a 4-point scale (0 = strongly disagree, 1 = somewhat disagree, 2 = somewhat agree, 3 = strongly agree), where higher scores indicate greater self-reported depressive symptoms. Each item was loaded onto a latent factor of overall internalizing symptoms. Model fit indices indicated good model fit (CFI = .970, TLI = .962, RMSEA = .073, SRMR = .034) (Hu & Bentler, Reference Hu and Bentler1999). Internalizing latent factor score was then extracted.

Adolescent externalizing behaviors were measured using a latent factor of youth responses to delinquency and substance use questions collected at age 15, 13 items from the Delinquency scale adopted from the National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health (Add Health) (Harris, Reference Harris2013), and 5 items on youth substance use (α = .81). Youth-reported delinquency was measured by 13 questions, and responses were coded on a 3-point scale (0 = never, 1 = sometimes, 2 = often), where higher scores indicate greater delinquent behavior. Substance use was measured using five binary questions (0 = no, 1 = yes) capturing alcohol use (more than two drinks without parents), tobacco, and other illicit substances (marijuana, illegal or prescription drugs). Each question was loaded onto a latent factor of overall externalizing behavior. Model fit indices indicated good model fit (CFI = .961, TLI = .955, RMSEA = .038, SRMR = .091) (Hu & Bentler, Reference Hu and Bentler1999). Externalizing latent factor score was then extracted.

Covariates

The following covariates were included in subsequent sensitivity analysis models: ethnoracial identity, sex at birth, parental marital status, birth city, child temperament (shyness, emotionality), and pubertal development. Youth self-reported ethnoracial identity (Black/African American only, non-Hispanic; white only, non-Hispanic; Hispanic/Latino; multiracial, non-Hispanic; other only, non-Hispanic) at age 15. Dummy-coded variables of these ethnoracial identity categories were included to account for experiences of race-related adversity such as structural racism that were not measured in the present investigation and could influence mental health (Bailey et al., Reference Bailey, Krieger, Agénor, Graves, Linos and Bassett2017) (multiracial and other groups were combined in analysis). Sex was parent-reported at the child’s birth (0 = female, 1 = male) and was included to account for any sex differences in adolescent mental health (Rutter et al., Reference Rutter, Caspi and Moffitt2003). Parental marital status was parent-reported at child age 1 (0 = unmarried, 1 = married) and was included to account for the FFCWS sampling strategy (Reichman et al., Reference Reichman, Teitler, Garfinkel and McLanahan2001). The birth city was measured by dummy-coded variables representing 20 study sites (see Supplemental Table 1 for a full list of study locations) and was included to account for geographical differences in sampling. Child temperament was measured at age 1 using the sum scores of parent responses to questions about the child’s shyness and emotionality (1 = “not characteristic or typical of your child” and 5 = “very characteristic or typical of your child”) and was included to account for early indicators of psychopathology (Nigg, Reference Nigg2006). Questions on temperament were adapted from the Emotionality, Activity, and Sociability Temperament Survey of Children (Buss & Plomin, Reference Buss and Plomin2013) and were only administered at age 1 in the present sample. Shyness was defined as the tendency for the child to be socially inhibited (e.g., “tends to be shy”), and higher scores represent a greater level of shyness. Emotionality was defined as the tendency for the child to become easily distressed (e.g., “reacts intensely when upset”), and higher scores represent a greater level of emotionality. Pubertal development was measured using parent-reported responses on the Pubertal Development Scale (Petersen et al., Reference Petersen, Crockett, Richards and Boxer1988) at age 9, and was included to account for differences in pubertal development. These questions measured the extent to which specific physical changes (e.g., facial hair, growth spurt, breast development) have occurred (1 = no, 2 = yes, barely, 3 = yes, definitely, 4 = development complete). Scores for females and males were computed separately as there were questions that pertained only to females (e.g., menarche).

Analytic strategy

Structured Life-Course Modeling Approach (SLCMA)

This study used the Structured Life-Course Modeling Approach (SLCMA) (Smith et al., Reference Smith, Hardy, Heron, Joinson, Lawlor, Macdonald-Wallis and Tilling2016) to test the cumulative and specificity hypotheses. SLCMA was implemented using R v4.2.1 and is a variable selection method that selects predictors, among a set of competing predictors, that would best explain the variation in a measured outcome. SLCMA uses the least angle regression variable selection technique, a type of least absolute shrinkage and selection operator regression, which employs a regularization penalty to regressors with low predictability, and is an approach that provides greater statistical power that is robust to multicollinearity (Efron et al., Reference Efron, Hastie, Johnstone and Tibshirani2004; Smith et al., Reference Smith, Hardy, Heron, Joinson, Lawlor, Macdonald-Wallis and Tilling2016, Reference Smith, Smith, Dunn and Andersen2022). This method is particularly suited to compare theoretical models (e.g., cumulative vs. sensitive periods) as it performs a data-driven model selection on a set of competing models that were identified a priori. SLCMA has been applied to study the effects of longitudinal exposure to adversity on psychopathology (Dunn et al., Reference Dunn, Soare, Raffeld, Busso, Crawford, Davis, Fisher, Slopen, Smith, Tiemeier and Susser2018), DNA methylation (Dunn et al., Reference Dunn, Soare, Zhu, Simpkin, Suderman, Klengel, Smith, Ressler and Relton2019), and cognitive performance (Nweze et al., Reference Nweze, Ezenwa, Ajaelu, Hanson and Okoye2023) among others.

In this study, two models of SLCMA were implemented (Table 1). The first tested the cumulative and specificity hypotheses by developmental timing by modeling adversity scores at each wave (ages 1, 3, 5, 9) and their cumulative score (sum of each childhood adversity across time) as separate predictors of internalizing and externalizing problems at age 15. These models were tested separately for each type of adversity; thus 10 models were examined for each behavioral outcome (internalizing and externalizing symptoms). Physical aggression, psychological aggression, neglect, residential moves, low collective efficacy, and community violence were not measured at age 1; thus, only data at ages 3, 5, and 9, as well as their cumulative scores, were included as predictors in those timing-specific SLCMA models. To account for multiple comparisons, statistical significance in the post-inference model selection was Bonferroni corrected for ten models. The second SLCMA model tested for cumulative and specificity hypotheses by types by modeling all adversity scores across childhood (average scores of each childhood adversity across ages 1, 3, 5, 9) and their cumulative score (sum of all examined childhood adversity) as separate predictors of internalizing and externalizing problems (two separate models) at age 15. All models were adjusted for covariates to control for potential confounding factors.

Table 1. Types of SLCMA models tested in the present investigation

Note. No data were collected at age 1 for physical aggression, psychological aggression, neglect, residential moves, low collective efficacy, and community violence.

Steps outlined in previous publications were followed to determine the best-fitting hypothesis (Dunn et al., Reference Dunn, Soare, Raffeld, Busso, Crawford, Davis, Fisher, Slopen, Smith, Tiemeier and Susser2018; Smith et al., Reference Smith, Hardy, Heron, Joinson, Lawlor, Macdonald-Wallis and Tilling2016, Reference Smith, Smith, Dunn and Andersen2022). First, bivariate correlations of all variables were examined to ensure that there were no highly correlated/collinear variables (r > .80) that would limit model selection. Missing data were handled following recommendations on missing data and multiple imputations (Woods et al., Reference Woods, Davis-Kean, Halvorson, King, Logan, Xu, Bainter, Brown, Clay, Cruz, Elsherif, Gerasimova, Joyal-Desmarais, Moreau, Nissen, Schmidt, Uzdavines, Van Dusen and Vasilev2021) using the R MICE package (Buuren & Groothuis-Oudshoorn, Reference Buuren and Groothuis-Oudshoorn2011) (20 imputations with 20 iterations). Then, in the first stage of SLCMA, each resulting elbow plot was examined to select the variables that best explain the outcome in the SLCMA model. In the second stage, selective inference methods (Efron et al., Reference Efron, Hastie, Johnstone and Tibshirani2004) were implemented to determine pooled estimates of effect sizes and confidence intervals for the variables that were selected in the first stage. Selective inference outperforms other inference methods in minimizing bias in effect sizes, confidence intervals, and p-value estimates by adjusting for family-wise error rates based on the number of variables selected (Smith et al., Reference Smith, Smith, Dunn and Andersen2022; Zhu et al., Reference Zhu, Simpkin, Suderman, Lussier, Walton, Dunn and Smith2021).

Results

Models testing timing-specific effects of childhood adversity

Levels and prevalence of each adversity at each developmental age are shown in Table 2.

Table 2. Mean and standard deviation of adversity at each time point

Note. No data were collected at age 1 for physical aggression, psychological aggression, neglect, residential moves, low collective efficacy, and community violence.

At each developmental time point, there were weak to moderate correlations among each childhood adversity exposure (rs = .19 to .59) (Supplemental Table 2). Parental stress had the highest correlations and stability over time (average r = .50), while neglect had the lowest correlations across the different ages (average r = .21). Material hardship was the most correlated with other adversities (average r = .22), while parental stress was the least associated with all other adversities (average r = .13). In terms of associations with internalizing and externalizing symptoms, psychological and physically aggressive parenting, number of residential moves, material hardship, and low collective efficacy were all consistently related to externalizing symptoms (and sometimes related to internalizing symptoms), whereas neglect, maternal depression, intimate partner violence, and low collective efficacy were relatively equally related to both internalizing and externalizing symptoms. Zero-order correlations of all predictors and outcomes at each time point are shown in Supplemental Table 2.

Across all ten timing-specific SLCMA models predicting internalizing symptoms, cumulative was selected 9 times (53%) followed by age 9 (6 times; 35%), age 5 (1 time; 6%), and age 3 (1 time; 6%) in the first stage of model selection (Figure 2). These findings suggest that a cumulative score across all the developmental time points examined explained the most variance in youth internalizing symptoms for almost all SLCMA adversity models, followed by age 9 exposure, and age 5 or 3.

Figure 2. Summary of model selection across all SLCMA models. Cumulative was selected 9 times (53%) in predicting youth internalizing symptoms and 10 times (56%) for externalizing symptoms. Age 9 exposure was selected six times (35%) for internalizing symptoms and six times (33%) for externalizing symptoms. Age 5 exposure was selected one time (6%) for internalizing symptoms and two times (11%) for externalizing symptoms. Finally, age 3 was selected one time (6%) for internalizing symptoms. Selected variables were determined based on the location of the elbow plots in separate timing-specific SLCMA models during the first stage of SLCMA (Figure 3).

Figure 3. Elbow plots of timing-specific and type-specific SLCMA models testing the associations of the childhood adversity and adolescent mental health problems. A full list of variables included in each SLCMA model is outlined in Table 1. (a) Elbow plots of timing-specific SLCMA models predicting youth internalizing symptoms. Of all 10 models, cumulative was selected in nine timing-specific SLCMA models: physical aggression, neglect, intimate partner violence, maternal depression, parental stress, residential moves, material hardship, lack of community support, and neighborhood violence. Age 9 was selected in six timing-specific SLCMA models: physical aggression, psychological aggression, intimate partner violence, parental stress, residential moves, and neighborhood violence. Age 5 was selected for one timing-specific SLCMA model: intimate partner violence. Age 3 was selected for one timing-specific SLCMA model: neglect. (b) Elbow plots of timing-specific SLCMA models predicting externalizing symptoms. Of all 10 timing-specific SLCMA models, cumulative was selected in all models. Age 9 was selected in six timing-specific SLCMA models: physical aggression, psychological aggression, maternal depression, residential moves, material hardship, and lack of community support. Age 5 was selected in two timing-specific SLCMA models: neglect and intimate partner violence. (c) Elbow plots of the type-specific SLCMA models predicting internalizing (left) and externalizing (right) symptoms. Cumulative, parental stress, and lack of community support were selected in the first stage of the type-specific SLCMA model in predicting internalizing symptoms. Post-selection inference showed that all three variables significantly predicted internalizing symptoms. Cumulative, psychological aggression, and material hardship were selected in the first stage of type-specific SLCMA models. Post-selection inference showed that both cumulative and psychological aggression significantly predicted externalizing behaviors.

Effect sizes and confidence intervals were then estimated in the second stage of SLCMA based on the number of variables indicated by each elbow plot (Figure 3a). Results from post-selection inference suggested that the cumulative score significantly predicted internalizing symptoms in three adversity models: maternal depression, material hardship, and low collective efficacy (Table 3). Additionally, parental stress at age 9 and neglect at age 3 also predicted internalizing symptoms (Table 3). These findings suggest that prolonged exposures to maternal depression, material hardship, and low collective efficacy most strongly predicted internalizing symptoms. Additionally, exposure to parental stress at age 9 and early exposure to neglect (at age 3) most strongly predicted internalizing symptoms relative to any other age.

Table 3. Models testing cumulative versus specificity by developmental timing to predict internalizing symptoms

Note. Coefficient and p-values were extracted only for variables that were selected in the first stage of SLCMA (determined by r2 values in corresponding elbow plots). The following predictors were included in each adversity model in the first-stage model specification: adversity at age 1 (for intimate partner violence, maternal depression, parental stress, residential moves, material hardship), age 3, age 5, age 9, cumulative score across all ages, and all covariates (sex at birth, ethnoracial identity, birth city, parental marital status, shyness, emotionality, pubertal development). Alpha values shown were Bonferroni corrected for 10 comparisons.

Across the 10 timing-specific SLCMA models examined to predict externalizing behaviors, cumulative was selected 10 times (56%) followed by age 9 (6 times; 33%) and age 5 (2 times; 11%) (Figure 2). Similar to the SLCMA models predicting internalizing symptoms, these findings suggest that a cumulative score across all the developmental time points best explained youth externalizing behaviors for all SLCMA adversity models examined, followed by age 9 exposures and age 5. After determining the number of selected variables using respective elbow plots (Figure 3b), post-selection inference showed that cumulative effect best predicted externalizing behaviors in six timing-specific SLCMA models: physical aggression, psychological aggression, neglect, parental stress, residential moves, material hardship, and exposure to community violence (Table 4). Additionally, maternal depression at age 9 and intimate partner violence at age 5 also significantly predicted externalizing behaviors (Table 4). These findings suggest that the prolonged childhood exposures to most adversities examined in the present study best predicted youth externalizing symptoms. However, externalizing symptoms were also predicted by maternal depression at age 9 and intimate partner violence at age 5 relative to all other ages examined.

Table 4. Models testing cumulative versus specificity by developmental timing to predict externalizing symptoms

Note. Coefficient and p-values were extracted only for variables that were selected in the first stage of SLCMA (determined by r2 values in corresponding elbow plots). The following predictors were included in each adversity model in the first-stage model specification: adversity at age 1 (for intimate partner violence, maternal depression, parental stress, residential moves, material hardship), age 3, age 5, age 9, cumulative score across all ages, and all covariates (sex at birth, ethnoracial identity, birth city, parental marital status, shyness, emotionality, pubertal development). Alpha values shown were Bonferroni corrected for 10 comparisons.

Models testing type-specific effects of childhood adversity across childhood

There were weak to moderate correlations among childhood adversity exposures across childhood (rs = .05 to .64) (Table 5). Physical and psychological aggression were most correlated at r = .64, while intimate partner violence was least correlated with physical aggression at r = .05.

Table 5. Zero-order correlations of adversity variables (average across 1, 3, 5, 9 years old)

Note. M and SD are used to represent mean and standard deviation, respectively. Zero-order correlations of adversity variables at each developmental wave are in Supplemental Table 2. Internal consistency indices for each measure are in Supplemental Table 3. * indicates p < .05. ** indicates p < .01.

In the type-specific SLCMA model predicting internalizing symptoms, three variables – cumulative, parental stress, and low collective efficacy – were selected in the first stage of SLCMA (Figure 3c), whereby the cumulative score of all adversities across childhood explained the greatest proportion of variance in the model (r 2 = 1.18%) followed by parental stress (r 2 = 0.29%) and low collective efficacy (r 2 = 0.66%) (Table 2). Post-selection inference found that the cumulative effect, parental stress, and low collective efficacy significantly predicted adolescent internalizing symptoms (Table 6). These findings suggest that a cumulative score that captures exposure to all the adversities across ages 1–9 explained the most variance in youth internalizing symptoms. Additionally, parental stress and low collective efficacy explained additional variance in youth internalizing symptoms.

Table 6. Models testing cumulative versus specificity by adversity type to predict youth internalizing and externalizing

Note. Coefficient and p-values were extracted only for variables that were selected in the first stage of SLCMA (determined by r 2 values in corresponding elbow plots). In the first-stage model specification for testing cumulative versus specificity by adversity type, average scores across all time points of all adversity were included as predictors: physical aggression, psychological aggression, neglect, intimate partner violence, maternal depression, parental stress, residential moves, material hardship, low collective efficacy, community violence, cumulative score across all adversity, and all covariates (sex at birth, ethnoracial identity, birth city, parental marital status, shyness, emotionality, pubertal development).

In the type-specific SLCMA model predicting externalizing behaviors, three variables – cumulative (r 2 = 1.16%), psychological aggression (r 2 = 2.48%), and material hardship (r 2 = 0.50%) were selected in the first stage of SLCMA (Figure 3c). Post-selection inference found that the cumulative effect of all types of adversity as well as psychological aggression significantly predicted youth externalizing behaviors (Table 6). These findings suggest that a cumulative score that captures exposure to all the adversities across ages 1–9 explained the most variance in youth externalizing symptoms. Additionally, psychological aggression explained additional variance in externalizing symptoms.

Discussion

Using a structured theory-informed and data-driven approach, this study compared different life-course hypotheses (cumulative, timing specificity), as well as the accumulation and unique contributions of different types of adverse experiences (cumulative, adversity-specificity) on adolescent internalizing and externalizing symptoms. We examined these questions in a prospective, diverse, population-based sample of youth from whom data were collected at multiple time points spanning 15 years. Consistent with our hypotheses, results suggested that internalizing and externalizing symptoms in youth were best explained by the cumulative effect of adversity across multiple types of adversity and over time. However, our results also demonstrated some adversity- and timing-specific effects through the distinct contributions of certain types of adversity at certain developmental periods. In particular, in addition to cumulative effects, most adversities experienced at age 9 better explained youth internalizing and externalizing symptoms than any other time points (age 1, 3, or 5). There were a few notable exceptions. The association between neglectful parenting and youth internalizing symptoms was better explained by exposures earlier in development (age 3), and the association between intimate partner violence and youth externalizing behaviors was better explained by experience at age 5. Moreover, in addition to cumulative effects across the types of adversities, parental stress and low collective efficacy predicted internalizing symptoms, whereas parental psychological aggression predicted externalizing behaviors in youth. Importantly, our analyses adjusted for pubertal development and early child temperament risk. Thus, the contributions of these separate adversities, as well as their cumulative effects on youth internalizing and externalizing problems, were over and above early childhood risk for psychopathology or differences in pubertal development. Collectively, these results suggest that, although childhood adversity is most likely to contribute prospectively to youth internalizing and externalizing symptoms in a cumulative fashion, there are specific developmental periods and types of adversity that have unique effects above the broad cumulative impact of adversity.

These findings support the cumulative risk model as particularly powerful in explaining the associations between childhood adversity and youth internalizing and externalizing symptoms. These findings align with other research suggesting that the number of different types of childhood adversity as well as across multiple developmental periods may be most detrimental to children’s mental health compared to the distinct contributions of any specific type of adversity at any developmental point (Evans et al., Reference Evans, Li and Whipple2013). The cumulative model of adversity converges with the principle of the allostatic load model (McEwen, Reference McEwen1998), which describes the process through which stressful experiences can increase the “wear and tear” of the body, to the detriment of health (Evans, Reference Evans2003; McEwen & Stellar, Reference McEwen and Stellar1993). The term “allostasis” refers to the neurobiological response to stressors in order to regain homeostasis or stability. When stress is experienced acutely, this response is adaptive and necessary for survival; however, repeated efforts to maintain homeostasis in response to the cumulative burden of stressful life events can lead to the detrimental effects of “allostatic load”. These cumulative findings can also reflect a model of adversity in which the risks for maladaptive behaviors increase with more exposures and longer durations of adversity but require a specific threshold to be met for maladaptive behaviors to manifest. Consequently, the specific type or timing of adversity may be less critical than the total number of adversities or the duration that they are experienced – a key finding to consider in models and studies of psychopathology, which often attempt to isolate a singular adversity at a particular time point and relate it to a singular outcome. Though this result may seem obvious in some ways – that the more adversity a child experiences, the greater the risk for psychopathology – it is important to consider for translational efforts. If our interventions focus only on one factor, we may have less success at promoting positive health for youth who are experiencing high cumulative risk. Thus, interventions that address risks across multiple domains (e.g., multisystemic therapy; Henggeler, Reference Henggeler1999) or those that can be tailored to specific needs and risk profiles of youth or families (e.g., Gill & Shaw, Reference Gill and Shaw2020) may be more effective in settings with high exposure to adversity.

Though the SLCMA models examined in the present study consistently favored cumulative effects, there were also timing-specific and type-specific associations that warrant attention. In particular, adverse experiences later in childhood (age 9) were more likely to be selected in the first stage of SLCMA to explain both youth internalizing and externalizing symptoms compared to adversities earlier in childhood (ages 1, 3, or 5). This pattern was evident across most factors examined (parenting, home, neighborhood), highlighting the importance of later childhood experiences for youth mental health. Age 9 is also a time when numerous social and biological changes are beginning. Emerging evidence on pubertal stress recalibration (Gunnar et al., Reference Gunnar, DePasquale, Reid and Donzella2019) indicates that this period leading up to puberty may hold greater significance for the development of stress response mechanisms than previously believed. Thus, the present findings suggest that adversity experienced during this developmental period is particularly critical for youth mental health. Additionally, it is also possible that developmental adaptation may be more difficult to accomplish when children have already established patterns and routines in their existing environment (Chetty et al., Reference Chetty, Hendren and Katz2016). Thus, environmental changes due to adverse events occurring during later childhood could present more challenges than currently understood. More research is needed to test these hypotheses and examine the differential mechanisms underlying adversity later in development.

The importance of experiences later in childhood and the lack of specificity in the developmental timing across contexts (e.g., home vs. neighborhood) in the present study are contrary to other studies that have established the significance of early childhood experiences. However, our pattern of results may diverge from previous studies for several reasons. First, the present sample contains a large representation of low-income families, which contrasts with some of the previous literature that were focused on more advantaged communities or clinical samples. It could be that timing effects are modulated by the total overall exposure to adversity or the relative disadvantage of the context surrounding the exposures. Second, FFCWS data collection at age 9 coincides with the Great Recession (Garfinkel et al., Reference Garfinkel, McLanahan and Wimer2016), suggesting that this period of instability may have contributed additional stressors beyond the measures we examined, with downstream implications for children as they mature into adolescence (Schneider et al., Reference Schneider, Waldfogel and Brooks-Gunn2015). More research is needed to examine the influence of acute economic downturns and other stressors (e.g., the global pandemic) that produce a widespread adverse effect on families. Future studies are also needed to replicate these findings in other diverse samples to test the generalizability of the present results.

There were two notable exceptions where experiences earlier in childhood (ages 3 or 5) were favored over the cumulative effect or age 9 exposures. First, exposure to neglectful parenting at age 3 was more likely to predict internalizing symptoms than any other developmental periods or cumulative effect. These findings echo other studies on early sensitive periods associated with previously institutionalized children (e.g., McLaughlin et al., Reference McLaughlin, Sheridan, Tibu, Fox, Zeanah and Nelson2015) who experienced severe neglect. It is possible that neglect more directly represents deprivation of an expected environment compared to other types of adversity (e.g., violence or neighborhood factors) (Brett et al., Reference Brett, Humphreys, Fleming, Kraemer and Drury2015). Consequently, neglect can lead to biological changes and subsequently internalizing symptoms in more children. Second, intimate partner violence at age 5 better predicted externalizing symptoms than cumulative effect or violence during any other developmental periods. This significance of intimate partner violence is consistent with prior research (Moss et al., Reference Moss, Loxton and Mishra2023) showing that children begin to develop a more nuanced understanding of conflict during this developmental period. Thus, witnessing violence at home may be more dysregulating for the child at age 5 than earlier (when they have limited contextual understanding) or later (when they have better emotion regulation skills or have developed other coping skills from school contexts).

When examining the relative contributions of specific adversities across childhood, in addition to cumulative effects, parental stress and low collective efficacy predicted internalizing symptoms above other types of adverse experiences. This is notable, particularly in light of the well-established evidence supporting the family stress model (Conger et al., Reference Conger, Wallace, Sun, Simons, McLoyd and Brody2002; Masarik & Conger, Reference Masarik and Conger2017). Within this framework, parents’ psychological distress is recognized as a critical factor in explaining how socioeconomic disadvantage can result in children’s maladaptive behaviors through increased caregivers’ emotional distress, strained parental interpersonal relationships, and disrupted parenting practices (Masarik & Conger, Reference Masarik and Conger2017). A supportive neighborhood can buffer against within-home stress (Browning et al., Reference Browning, Cagney, Boettner, Shanahan, Mortimer and Kirkpatrick Johnson2016; Silk et al., Reference Silk, Sessa, Sheffield Morris, Steinberg and Avenevoli2004). Therefore, the present findings demonstrate that the presence of both parental stress and low collective efficacy can have a significant impact on youth internalizing symptoms over and above other adverse experiences.

The meaningful role of parent psychological aggression across childhood, in addition to the cumulative risk, in predicting adolescent externalizing behaviors aligns with an extensive body of research investigating risk factors for externalizing outcomes (Hinshaw & Lee, Reference Hinshaw and Lee2003). These findings could indicate genetic risks through personality traits associated with externalizing behaviors (e.g., impulsivity, interpersonal aggression) that are passed down from parents to children (Gard et al., Reference Gard, Dotterer and Hyde2019). Alternatively, harsh parenting could also predict later externalizing behaviors through environmental mechanisms (Burt et al., Reference Burt, Clark, Gershoff, Klump and Hyde2021). These associations likely reflect influences of both genetic and environmental processes and their interactions. For example, according to multiple developmental models of youth externalizing, early childhood difficult temperament interacts with parent psychological aggression via disrupted empathy development (Eisenberg, Reference Eisenberg2005; Kochanska, Reference Kochanska1997) and the modeling of aggressive interactions (Granic & Patterson, Reference Granic and Patterson2006), leading to a cycle of coercive interactions over time that spill out into other settings (e.g., schools, peers; Patterson et al., Reference Patterson, DeBaryshe and Ramsey1989). This work highlights the importance of preventing harsh parenting as a key public health measure to reduce the risk of later externalizing behaviors (Dodge et al., Reference Dodge, Murphy, O’Donnell and Christopoulos2009; Dodge, Reference Dodge2001).

There are several limitations to our study. First, we selected risk factors that the current literature indicates as particularly salient for both internalizing and externalizing symptoms. Although the present study examined 10 types of childhood adversity across 4 waves in the first 9 years of childhood, there are other risk factors that could have been included, but not measured in the present sample. Moreover, given that adverse experiences are often interconnected and overlapping, there could be interactions between these experiences that have important implications. Second, given the large-scale survey approach of FFCWS, most of the measures capturing adversities were brief and potentially less robust than other studies (e.g., many were represented only by a few items, leading to lower internal consistencies). This brief measurement may introduce errors and fail to capture each construct deeply. Moreover, although the maternal report can capture an important perspective on adversity exposure, especially when collected prospectively during childhood (Tajima et al., Reference Tajima, Herrenkohl, Huang and Whitney2004), the childhood adversities examined here were measured only using parental responses, which may capture only one slice of the context in which a child is growing up in (De Los Reyes et al., Reference De Los Reyes, Ohannessian and Racz2019). Third, data collection in this cohort was focused primarily on childhood ages (i.e., 1–9) with outcomes measured in adolescence (age 15), and we had no data during the critical developmental period between 9 and 15 or the prenatal period. Furthermore, many timing-specific measures were examined using data from only one time point (e.g., no data were collected for neglect at age 1). More studies are thus needed to examine these questions at additional time points, including the in utero period, infancy, and pre-adolescence. Fourth, SLCMA presently is unable to accommodate time-varying covariates. Thus, we are unable to account for changes in factors that could vary across time. Fifth, although the current models provide a statistical method to tease apart developmental timing differences in adversity exposure, it is important to acknowledge that experiences during later development inherently include temporal information from earlier development. More research is thus needed to further distinguish the influence of exposures at age 9 from chronic experiences or cascading effects. Lastly, although this study utilized prospective, longitudinal data spanning 15 years, as with all longitudinal studies, causal relationships between these experiences and behavioral outcomes cannot be determined (Rutter, Reference Rutter2003). There are also numerous other biological and contextual factors, such as the interactions of genetic and environmental influences, that are critical in explaining the antecedents of psychopathology (Manuck & McCaffery, Reference Manuck and McCaffery2014).

Conclusion

In a well-sampled cohort of youth at an increased risk for exposure to adversity, this investigation provides empirical evidence supporting the longitudinal associations between childhood adversity and youth mental health outcomes, using a theory-informed and data-driven method. These findings underscore the profound impact of childhood adversity and carry direct policy implications.

First, considering the evidence that cumulative effects were the strongest in predicting adolescent mental health, it is unsurprising that interventions targeting only a single form of adversity might yield less consistent or substantial mental health improvements, especially for individuals living in poverty. For instance, Shelleby and Shaw (Reference Shelleby and Shaw2014) found that while some parenting interventions led to significant improvements in child outcomes, children from lower socioeconomic backgrounds benefited less. This may be due to the exposure to a myriad of risks and increased barriers that many disadvantaged families face when trying to access care (Staudt, Reference Staudt2007). Therefore, interventions targeting multiple exposures or personalizing their approach based on the family’s unique constellation of risks may prove to be more effective (Shaw et al., Reference Shaw, Bell and Gilliom2000; Shaw et al. Reference Shaw, Dishion, Supplee, Gardner and Arnds2006). Moreover, policies and interventions that target systemic structures (e.g., profound wealth inequality, concentrated poverty, structural racism) can be a more efficient and effective way to allocate resources (Braveman & Gottlieb, Reference Braveman and Gottlieb2014), as they prevent youth from encountering an array of risks across an extended period of development.

Second, while research on early childhood has provided valuable insights into the etiology of mental disorders, more efforts are needed to understand the mechanisms of prepubertal experiences identified as key predictors for mental health in this sample. Evidence supporting a potentially sensitive period during late childhood underscores the importance of understanding the biological shifts leading up to puberty and how these changes could create opportunities for prevention and intervention.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579424001512.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank individuals and families who made this study possible. This work was supported by the National Institute of Health: CSM, R01MH103761; CSM, CM, LWH, R01MH121079; and (CSM, VM, T32HD007109).

Competing interests

None.

References

Abidin, R., Flens, J. R., & Austin, W. G. (2006). The parenting stress index. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.Google Scholar
Adam, E. K. (2004). Beyond quality: Parental and residential stability and children’s adjustment. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13(5), 210213. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.00310.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anastasio, R. J., & Leventhal, T. (2023). Elementary disruptions? Exploring residential move type, timing, and duration of implications during middle childhood. Child Development, 94(5), e264e278. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13939 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anderson, S., Johnston, W., & Leventhal, T. (2019). When neighborhoods matter: Developmental timing and youth reading achievement and problem behaviors. Social Science Research, 81, 111. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ssresearch.2019.02.010 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anderson, S., Leventhal, T., Newman, S., & Dupéré, V. (2014). Residential mobility among children: A framework for child and family policy. Cityscape, 16(1), 536.Google Scholar
Appleyard, K., Egeland, B., van Dulmen, M. H. M., & Alan Sroufe, L. (2005). When more is not better: The role of cumulative risk in child behavior outcomes. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46(3), 235245. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00351.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Artz, S., Jackson, M. A., Rossiter, K. R., Nijdam-Jones, A., Géczy, I., & Porteous, S. (2014). A comprehensive review of the literature on the impact of exposure to intimate partner violence on children and youth. Sociology-the Journal of The British Sociological Association, 5(4), 493587. https://doi.org/10.18357/ijcyfs.artzsGoogle Scholar
Atkinson, L., Beitchman, J., Gonzalez, A., Young, A., Wilson, B., Escobar, M., Chisholm, V., Brownlie, E., Khoury, J. E., Ludmer, J., Villani, V., Eapen, V. (2015). Cumulative risk, cumulative outcome: A 20-year longitudinal study. PLOS ONE, 10(6), e0127650. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0127650 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bailey, Z. D., Krieger, N., Agénor, M., Graves, J., Linos, N., & Bassett, M. T. (2017). Structural racism and health inequities in the USA: Evidence and interventions. The Lancet, 389(10077), 14531463. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(17)30569-X CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bauman, K. J. (1999). Shifting family definitions: The effect of cohabitation and other nonfamily household relationships on measures of poverty. Demography, 36(3), 315325. https://doi.org/10.2307/2648055 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bell, C. C. (1994). DSM-IV: Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. JAMA, 272(10), 828829.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Belsky, J., Schlomer, G., & Ellis, B. (2011). Beyond cumulative risk: Distinguishing harshness and unpredictability as determinants of parenting and early life history strategy. Developmental Psychology, 48(3), 662673. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0024454 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blakemore, S.-J. (2012). Imaging brain development: The adolescent brain. NeuroImage, 61(2), 397406. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.11.080 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Braveman, P., & Gottlieb, L. (2014). The social determinants of health: It’s time to consider the causes of the causes. Public Health Reports, 129(1_suppl2), 1931. https://doi.org/10.1177/00333549141291S206 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brett, Z. H., Humphreys, K. L., Fleming, A. S., Kraemer, G. W., & Drury, S. S. (2015). Using cross-species comparisons and a neurobiological framework to understand early social deprivation effects on behavioral development. Development and Psychopathology, 27(2), 347367. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579415000036 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bronfenbrenner, U., & Morris, P. A. (2007). The bioecological model of human development. Handbook of Child Psychology, 1, 793–828.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Browning, C. R., Cagney, K. A., & Boettner, B. (2016). Neighborhood, place, and the life course. In Shanahan, M. J., Mortimer, J. T., & Kirkpatrick Johnson, M. (Eds.), Handbook of the life course. vol. II, p. 597620). Springer International Publishing, https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-20880-0_26 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buehler, C., & Gerard, J. M. (2013). Cumulative family risk predicts increases in adjustment difficulties across early adolescence. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 42(6), 905920. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-012-9806-3 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burt, S. A., Clark, D. A., Gershoff, E. T., Klump, K. L., & Hyde, L. W. (2021). Twin differences in harsh parenting predict youth’s antisocial behavior. Psychological Science, 32(3), 395409. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797620968532 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buss, A. H., & Plomin, R. (2013). The EAS approach to temperament. In The study of temperament (pp. 6779). Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Buuren, S.van, & Groothuis-Oudshoorn, K. (2011). mice: Multivariate imputation by chained equations in R. Journal of Statistical Software, 45(3), 167. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v045.i03 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cantillon, D. (2006). Community social organization, parents, and peers as mediators of perceived neighborhood block characteristics on delinquent and prosocial activities. American Journal of Community Psychology, 37(1), 111127. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10464-005-9000-9 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carpenter, G. L., & Stacks, A. M. (2009). Developmental effects of exposure to Intimate Partner Violence in early childhood: A review of the literature. Children and Youth Services Review, 31(8), 831839. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.childyouth.2009.03.005 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chang, L., Schwartz, D., Dodge, K. A., & McBride-Chang, C. (2003). Harsh parenting in relation to child emotion regulation and aggression. Journal of Family Psychology, 17(4), 598606. https://doi.org/10.1037/0893-3200.17.4.598 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chetty, R., Hendren, N., & Katz, L. F. (2016). The effects of exposure to better neighborhoods on children: New evidence from the moving to opportunity experiment. American Economic Review, 106(4), 855902. https://doi.org/10.1257/aer.20150572 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cicchetti, D. (2016). Developmental psychopathology, risk, resilience, and intervention, vol. 4. John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Cicchetti, D., & Toth, S. L. (1995). A developmental psychopathology perspective on child abuse and neglect. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 34(5), 541565. https://doi.org/10.1097/00004583-199505000-00008 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Collins, W. A., Harris, M. L., & Susman, A. (1995). Parenting during middle childhood. In Handbook of parenting, Vol. 1: Children and parenting. (pp. 6589). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.Google Scholar
Conger, R. D., Wallace, L. E., Sun, Y., Simons, R. L., McLoyd, V. C., & Brody, G. H. (2002). Economic pressure in African American families: A replication and extension of the family stress model. Developmental Psychology, 38(2), 179193.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crawford, K. M., Choi, K., Davis, K. A., Zhu, Y., Soare, T. W., Smith, A. D. A. C., Germine, L., Dunn, E. C. (2022). Exposure to early childhood maltreatment and its effect over time on social cognition. Development and Psychopathology, 34(1), 409419. https://doi.org/10.1017/S095457942000139X CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crnic, K. A., & Greenberg, M. T. (1990). Minor parenting stresses with young children. Child Development, 61(5), 16281637. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1990.tb02889.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cummings, E. M., & Davies, P. T. (1994). Maternal depression and child development. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 35(1), 73112.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Los Reyes, A., Ohannessian, C. M., & Racz, S. J. (2019). Discrepancies between adolescent and parent reports about family relationships. Child Development Perspectives, 13(1), 5358. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12306 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Derogatis, L. R., & Kathryn, L. (2000). The SCL-90-R and Brief Symptom Inventory (BSI) in primary care. In Handbook of psychological assessment in primary care settings (pp. 310347). Routledge.Google Scholar
Dodge, K. A. (2001). The science of youth violence prevention: Progressing from developmental epidemiology to efficacy to effectiveness to public policy. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 20(1, Supplement 1), 6370. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0749-3797(00)00275-0 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dodge, K. A. (2020). Annual research review: Universal and targeted strategies for assigning interventions to achieve population impact. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 61(3), 255267. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.13141 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dodge, K. A., Murphy, R., O’Donnell, K., & Christopoulos, C. (2009). Community-level prevention of child maltreatment. Preventing Child Maltreatment: Community Approaches, 68, 68–81.Google Scholar
Doom, J. R., Vanzomeren-Dohm, A. A., & Simpson, J. A. (2016). Early unpredictability predicts increased adolescent externalizing behaviors and substance use: A life history perspective. Development and Psychopathology, 28(4 Pt 2), 15051516. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579415001169 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dunn, E. C., Busso, D. S., Davis, K. A., Smith, A. D. A. C., Mitchell, C., Tiemeier, H., & Susser, E. S. (2023). Sensitive periods for the effect of child maltreatment on psychopathology symptoms in adolescence. Complex Psychiatry, 1(1-4), 145153. https://doi.org/10.1159/000530120 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunn, E. C., McLaughlin, K. A., Slopen, N., Rosand, J., & Smoller, J. W. (2013). Developmental timing of child maltreatment and symptoms of depression and suicidal ideation in young adulthood: Results from the national longitudinal study of adolescent health. Depression and Anxiety, 30(10), 955964. https://doi.org/10.1002/da.22102 Google ScholarPubMed
Dunn, E. C., Soare, T. W., Raffeld, M. R., Busso, D. S., Crawford, K. M., Davis, K. A., Fisher, V. A., Slopen, N., Smith, A. D. A. C., Tiemeier, H., Susser, E. S. (2018). What life course theoretical models best explain the relationship between exposure to childhood adversity and psychopathology symptoms: Recency, accumulation, or sensitive periods? Psychological Medicine, 48(15), 25622572. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291718000181 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dunn, E. C., Soare, T. W., Zhu, Y., Simpkin, A. J., Suderman, M. J., Klengel, T., Smith, A. D. A. C., Ressler, K. J., Relton, C. L. (2019). Sensitive periods for the effect of childhood adversity on DNA methylation: Results from a prospective, longitudinal study. Biological Psychiatry, 85(10), 838849. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2018.12.023 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Efron, B., Hastie, T., Johnstone, I., & Tibshirani, R. (2004). Least angle regression. The Annals of Statistics, 32(2), 407499. https://doi.org/10.1214/009053604000000067 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Egeland, B., & Sroufe, L. A. (1981). Development sequelae of maltreatment in infancy. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 1981(11), 7792. https://doi.org/10.1002/cd.23219811106 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eisenberg, N. (2005). The development of empathy-related responding. In Volume 51 of the Nebraska Symposium on Motivation. Moral motivation through the life span (pp. 73117). University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Eisenberg, N., Cumberland, A., & Spinrad, T. L. (1998). Parental socialization of emotion. Psychological Inquiry, 9(4), 241273. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327965pli0904_1 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Emery, R. E. (1982). Interparental conflict and the children of discord and divorce. Psychological Bulletin, 92(2), 310330.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Evans, G. W. (2003). A multimethodological analysis of cumulative risk and allostatic load among rural children. Developmental Psychology, 39(5), 924933. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.39.5.924 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Evans, G. W., Li, D., & Whipple, S. S. (2013). Cumulative risk and child development. Psychological Bulletin, 139(6), 13421396. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0031808 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Felitti, V. J., Anda, R. F., Nordenberg, D., Williamson, D. F., Spitz, A. M., Edwards, V., & Marks, J. S. (1998). Relationship of childhood abuse and household dysfunction to many of the leading causes of death in adults: The Adverse Childhood Experiences (ACE) Study. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 14(4), 245258.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gard, A. M., Dotterer, H. L., & Hyde, L. W. (2019). Genetic influences on antisocial behavior: Recent advances and future directions. Current Opinion in Psychology, 27, 4655. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2018.07.013 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gard, A. M., Hein, T. C., Mitchell, C., Brooks-Gunn, J., McLanahan, S. S., Monk, C. S., & Hyde, L. W. (2021). Prospective longitudinal associations between harsh parenting and corticolimbic function during adolescence. Development and Psychopathology, 1-16(3), 981996. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579420001583 Google Scholar
Gardner, F., & Shaw, D. (2009). Behavioral problems of infancy and preschool children (0-5). In Rutter’s child and adolescent psychiatry (5th ed. pp. 882893). https://doi.org/10.1002/9781444300895.ch53 Google Scholar
Garfinkel, I., McLanahan, S. S., & Wimer, C. (2016). Children of the Great Recession, . Russell Sage Foundation.Google Scholar
Gershoff, E. T., Aber, J. L., Raver, C. C., & Lennon, M. C. (2007). Income is not enough: Incorporating material hardship into models of income associations with parenting and child development. Child Development, 78(1), 7095. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.00986.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gill, A. M., & Shaw, D. S. (2020). The family check-up: Building on family strengths to promote child wellbeing. In Handbook of research on emotional and behavioral disorders. Routledge.Google Scholar
Gillespie, B. J. (2013). Adolescent behavior and achievement, social capital, and the timing of geographic mobility. Advances in Life Course Research, 18(3), 223233. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.alcr.2013.07.001 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goetschius, L. G., Hein, T. C., McLanahan, S. S., Brooks-Gunn, J., McLoyd, V. C., Dotterer, H. L., Lopez-Duran, N., Mitchell, C., Hyde, L. W., Monk, C. S., Beltz, A. M. (2020). Association of childhood violence exposure with adolescent neural network density. JAMA Network Open, 3(9), e2017850. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2020.17850 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goetschius, L. G., Hein, T. C., Mitchell, C., Lopez-Duran, N. L., McLoyd, V. C., Brooks-Gunn, J., McLanahan, S. S., Hyde, L. W., Monk, C. S. (2020). Childhood violence exposure and social deprivation predict adolescent amygdala-orbitofrontal cortex white matter connectivity. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 45, 100849. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2020.100849 Google ScholarPubMed
Goodman, S. H., Rouse, M. H., Connell, A. M., Broth, M. R., Hall, C. M., & Heyward, D. (2011). Maternal depression and child psychopathology: A meta-analytic review. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 14(1), 127. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10567-010-0080-1 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Granic, I., & Patterson, G. R. (2006). Toward a comprehensive model of antisocial development: A dynamic systems approach. Psychological Review, 113(1), 101131. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.113.1.101 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greenberg, M. T., Cicchetti, D., & Cummings, E. M. (1990). Attachment in the preschool years: Theory, research, and intervention. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Gunnar, M. R., DePasquale, C. E., Reid, B. M., & Donzella, B. (2019). Pubertal stress recalibration reverses the effects of early life stress in postinstitutionalized children. Proceedings of The National Academy of Sciences of The United States of America, 116(48), 2398423988. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1909699116 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hankin, B. L. (2005). Childhood maltreatment and psychopathology: Prospective tests of attachment, cognitive vulnerability, and stress as mediating processes. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 29(6), 645671. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-005-9631-z CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hardi, F. A., Goetschius, L. G., Peckins, M. K., Brooks-Gunn, J., McLanahan, S. S., McLoyd, V., Lopez-Duran, N. L., Mitchell, C. & Monk, C. S. (2022). Differential developmental associations of material hardship exposure and adolescent amygdala-prefrontal cortex white matter connectivity. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 34(10), 18661891. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_01801 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hardi, F. A., Goetschius, L. G., Tillem, S., McLoyd, V., Brooks-Gunn, J., Boone, M., Lopez-Duran, N., Mitchell, C., Hyde, L. W., Monk, C. S. (2023). Early childhood household instability, adolescent structural neural network architecture, and young adulthood depression: A 21-year longitudinal study. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 61, 101253. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2023.101253 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harris, K. M. (2013). The add health study: Design and accomplishments (vol. 1, pp.122). Carolina Population Center, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill.Google Scholar
Hein, T. C., Goetschius, L. G., McLoyd, V. C., Brooks-Gunn, J., McLanahan, S. S., Mitchell, C., Lopez-Duran, N. L., Hyde, L. W., Monk, C. S. (2020). Childhood violence exposure and social deprivation are linked to adolescent threat and reward neural function. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 15(11), 12521259. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsaa144 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heinlein, L. M., & Shinn, M. (2000). School mobility and student achievement in an urban setting. Psychology in the Schools, 37(4), 349357. https://doi.org/10.1002/1520-6807(200007)37: 3.0.CO;2-1>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henggeler, S. W. (1999). Multisystemic therapy: An overview of clinical procedures, outcomes, and policy implications. Child Psychology and Psychiatry Review, 4(1), 210. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1360641798001786 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hinshaw, S. P., & Lee, S. S. (2003). Conduct and oppositional defiant disorders. Child Psychopathology, 2(1), 144198.Google Scholar
Hofferth, S., Davis-Kean, P. E., Davis, J., & Finkelstein, J. (1997). The child development supplement to the Panel Study of Income Dynamics: 1997 user guide. Survey Research Center, Institute for Social Research, University of Michigan.Google Scholar
Hofferth, S. L., & Sandberg, J. F. (2001). How American children spend their time. Journal of Marriage and Family, 63(2), 295308. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1741-3737.2001.00295.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoffman-Plotkin, D., & Twentyman, C. T. (1984). A multimodal assessment of behavioral and cognitive deficits in abused and neglected preschoolers. Child Development, 55(3), 794802. https://doi.org/10.2307/1130130 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6(1), 155. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705519909540118 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hubel, D. H., & Wiesel, T. N. (1970). The period of susceptibility to the physiological effects of unilateral eye closure in kittens. The Journal of Physiology, 206(2), 419436. https://doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.1970.sp009022 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hunt, T. K. A., Berger, L. M., & Slack, K. S. (2017). Adverse childhood experiences and behavioral problems in middle childhood. Child Abuse & Neglect, 67, 391402. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chiabu.2016.11.005 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jaffee, S. R., & Maikovich-Fong, A. K. (2011). Effects of chronic maltreatment and maltreatment timing on children’s behavior and cognitive abilities. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 52(2), 184194. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02304.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
James, S., McLanahan, S., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (2021). Contributions of the fragile families and child wellbeing study to child development. Annual Review of Developmental Psychology, 3(1), 187206. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-devpsych-050620-113832 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaplow, J. B., & Widom, C. S. (2007). Age of onset of child maltreatment predicts long-term mental health outcomes. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 116(1), 176187. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.116.1.176 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kessler, R. C., Davis, C. G., & Kendler, K. S. (1997). Childhood adversity and adult psychiatric disorder in the US National Comorbidity Survey. Psychological Medicine, 27(5), 11011119. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291797005588 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kessler, R. C., Andrews, G., Mroczek, D., Ustun, B., & Wittchen, H. (1998). The World Health Organization composite international diagnostic interview short-form (CIDI-SF). International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research, 7(4), 171185.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kessler, R. C., Berglund, P., Demler, O., Jin, R., Merikangas, K. R., & Walters, E. E. (2005). Lifetime prevalence and age-of-onset distributions of DSM-IV disorders in the national comorbidity survey replication. Archives of General Psychiatry, 62(6), 593. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.62.6.593 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kleinepier, T., & van Ham, M. (2018). The temporal dynamics of neighborhood disadvantage in childhood and subsequent problem behavior in adolescence. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 47(8), 16111628.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kochanska, G. (1997). Mutually responsive orientation between mothers and their young children: Implications for early socialization. Child Development, 68(1), 94112. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1997.tb01928.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lacey, R. E., Gondek, D., Smith, B. J., Smith, A. D. A. C., Dunn, E. C., & Sacker, A. (2023). Testing lifecourse theories characterising associations between maternal depression and offspring depression in emerging adulthood: The Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 64(8), 11491158. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.13699 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, A., & Hankin, B. L. (2009). Insecure attachment, dysfunctional attitudes, and low self-esteem predicting prospective symptoms of depression and anxiety during adolescence. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 38(2), 219231. https://doi.org/10.1080/15374410802698396 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leventhal, T., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (2000). The neighborhoods they live in: The effects of neighborhood residence on child and adolescent outcomes. Psychological Bulletin, 126(2), 309337. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.126.2.309 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leventhal, T., & Newman, S. (2010). Housing and child development. Children and Youth Services Review, 32(9), 11651174. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.childyouth.2010.03.008 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Loman, M. M., & Gunnar, M. R. (2010). Early experience and the development of stress reactivity and regulation in children. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 34(6), 867876.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luby, J. L., Belden, A., Harms, M. P., Tillman, R., & Barch, D. M. (2016). Preschool is a sensitive period for the influence of maternal support on the trajectory of hippocampal development. Proceedings of The National Academy of Sciences of The United States of America, 113(20), 57425747. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1601443113 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Manuck, S. B., & McCaffery, J. M. (2014). Gene-environment interaction. Annual Review of Psychology, 65(1), 4170. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115100 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Masarik, A. S., & Conger, R. D. (2017). Stress and child development: A review of the family stress model. Current Opinion in Psychology, 13, 8590. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2016.05.008 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mayer, S. E., & Jencks, C. (1989). Poverty and the distribution of material hardship. The Journal of Human Resources, 24(1), 88114. JSTOR. https://doi.org/10.2307/145934CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McEwen, B. S. (1998). Stress, adaptation, and disease. Allostasis and allostatic load. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 840(1), 3344. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.1998.tb09546.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McEwen, B. S., & Stellar, E. (1993). Stress and the individual: Mechanisms leading to disease. Archives of Internal Medicine, 153(18), 20932101. https://doi.org/10.1001/archinte.1993.00410180039004 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McLaughlin, K. A., Sheridan, M. A., Tibu, F., Fox, N. A., Zeanah, C. H., & Nelson, C. A. (2015). Causal effects of the early caregiving environment on development of stress response systems in children. Proceedings of The National Academy of Sciences of The United States of America, 112(18), 56375642. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1423363112 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McQuillan, M. E., & Bates, J. E. (2017). Parental stress and child temperament. In Deater-Deckard, K., & Panneton, R. (Eds.), Parental stress and early child development: Adaptive and maladaptive outcomes (pp. 75106). Springer International Publishing. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-55376-4_4 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morris, A. S., Criss, M. M., Silk, J. S., & Houltberg, B. J. (2017). The impact of parenting on emotion regulation during childhood and adolescence. Child Development Perspectives, 11(4), 233238. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12238 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moss, K. M., Loxton, D., & Mishra, G. D. (2023). Does timing matter? Associations between intimate partner violence across the early life course and internalizing and externalizing behavior in children. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 38(19-20), 1056610587. https://doi.org/10.1177/08862605231174505 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muthén, B., & Muthén, L. (2017). Mplus. In Handbook of item response theory (pp. 507518). Chapman and Hall/CRC.Google Scholar
Nigg, J. T. (2006). Temperament and developmental psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47(3-4), 395422. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01612.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nweze, T., Ezenwa, M., Ajaelu, C., Hanson, J. L., & Okoye, C. (2023). Cognitive variations following exposure to childhood adversity: Evidence from a pre-registered, longitudinal study. eClinicalMedicine, 56, 101784. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eclinm.2022.101784 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Patterson, G. R., DeBaryshe, B. D., & Ramsey, E. (1989). A developmental perspective on antisocial behavior. The American Psychologist, 44(2), 329335. https://doi.org/10.1037//0003-066x.44.2.329 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peckins, M. K., Roberts, A. G., Hein, T. C., Hyde, L. W., Mitchell, C., Brooks-Gunn, J., McLanahan, S. S., Monk, C. S., Lopez-Duran, N. L. (2020). Violence exposure and social deprivation is associated with cortisol reactivity in urban adolescents. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 111, 104426. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2019.104426 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petersen, A. C., Crockett, L., Richards, M., & Boxer, A. (1988). A self-report measure of pubertal status: Reliability, validity, and initial norms. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 17(2), 117133. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01537962 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Radloff, L. S. (1977). The CES-D scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement, 1(3), 385401.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reichman, N. E., Teitler, J. O., Garfinkel, I., & McLanahan, S. (2001). Fragile families: Sample and design. Children and Youth Services Review, 23(4-5), 303326. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0190-7409(01)00141-4 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Repetti, R. L., Taylor, S. E., & Seeman, T. E. (2002). Risky families: Family social environments and the mental and physical health of offspring. Psychological Bulletin, 128(2), 330366. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.128.2.330 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rubin, K. H., Coplan, R., Chen, X., Bowker, J., & McDonald, K. L. (2011). Peer relationships in childhood. In Social and personality development. Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Rutherford, H. J. V., Wallace, N. S., Laurent, H. K., & Mayes, L. C. (2015). Emotion regulation in parenthood. Developmental Review, 36, 114. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2014.12.008 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rutter, M. (2003). Poverty and child mental health natural experiments and social causation. JAMA, 290(15), 20632064. https://doi.org/10.1001/jama.290.15.2063 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rutter, M., Caspi, A., & Moffitt, T. E. (2003). Using sex differences in psychopathology to study causal mechanisms: Unifying issues and research strategies. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44(8), 10921115. https://doi.org/10.1111/1469-7610.00194 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rutter, M., Kent, M. W., & Rolf, J. (1979). Primary prevention of psychopathology, vol. 3: Social competence in children. Psychiatric Services, 3(4), 279280.Google Scholar
Sameroff, A. J. (1998). Environmental risk factors in infancy. Pediatrics, 102(5), 12871292.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sameroff, A. J., & Seifer, R. (1995). Accumulation of environmental risk and child mental health. In Children of poverty. Routledge.Google Scholar
Sampson, R. J. (1997). Collective regulation of adolescent misbehavior: Validation results from eighty Chicago neighborhoods. Journal of Adolescent Research, 12(2), 227244.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sampson, R. J., Raudenbush, S. W., & Earls, F. (1997). Neighborhoods and violent crime: A multilevel study of collective efficacy. Science, 277(5328), 918924. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.277.5328.918 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schneider, W., Waldfogel, J., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (2015). The great recession and behavior problems in 9-year old children. Developmental Psychology, 51(11), 16151629. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000038 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shaw, D. S., & Bell, R. Q. (1993). Developmental theories of parental contributors to antisocial behavior. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 21(5), 493518. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00916316 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shaw, D. S., Bell, R. Q., & Gilliom, M. (2000). A truly early starter model of antisocial behavior revisited. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 3(3), 155172.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shaw, D. S., Dishion, T. J., Supplee, L., Gardner, F., & Arnds, K. (2006). Randomized trial of a family-centered approach to the prevention of early conduct problems: 2-year effects of the family check-up in early childhood. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 74(1), 19. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.74.1.1 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shaw, D. S., Winslow, E. B., Owens, E. B., Vondra, J. I., Cohn, J. F., & Bell, R. Q. (1998). The development of early externalizing problems among children from low-income families: A transformational perspective. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 26(2), 95107. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1022665704584 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shelleby, E. C., & Shaw, D. S. (2014). Outcomes of parenting interventions for child conduct problems: A review of differential effectiveness. Child Psychiatry & Human Development, 45(5), 628645. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10578-013-0431-5 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Silk, J. S., Sessa, F. M., Sheffield Morris, A., Steinberg, L., & Avenevoli, S. (2004). Neighborhood cohesion as a buffer against hostile maternal parenting. Journal of Family Psychology, 18(1), 135146.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smetana, J. G., Campione-Barr, N., & Metzger, A. (2006). Adolescent development in interpersonal and societal contexts. Annual Review of Psychology, 57(1), 255284. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.57.102904.190124 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, A. D., Hardy, R., Heron, J., Joinson, C. J., Lawlor, D. A., Macdonald-Wallis, C., & Tilling, K. (2016). A structured approach to hypotheses involving continuous exposures over the life course. International Journal of Epidemiology, 45(4), 12711279. https://doi.org/10.1093/ije/dyw164 Google ScholarPubMed
Smith, A. D., Heron, J., Mishra, G., Gilthorpe, M. S., Ben-Shlomo, Y., & Tilling, K. (2015). Model selection of the effect of binary exposures over the life course. Epidemiology (Cambridge, Mass.), 26(5), 719726. https://doi.org/10.1097/EDE.0000000000000348 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, B. J., Smith, A. D. A. C., & Dunn, E. C. (2022). Statistical modeling of sensitive period effects using the structured life course modeling approach (SLCMA). In Andersen, S. L. (Ed.), Sensitive periods of brain development and preventive interventions (pp. 215234). Springer International Publishing. https://doi.org/10.1007/7854_2021_280 Google Scholar
Staudt, M. (2007). Treatment engagement with caregivers of at-risk children: Gaps in research and conceptualization. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 16(2), 183196. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10826-006-9077-2 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Straus, M. A., Hamby, S. L., Finkelhor, D., Moore, D. W., & Runyan, D. (1998). Identification of child maltreatment with the parent-child conflict tactics scales: Development and psychometric data for a national sample of American parents. Child Abuse & Neglect, 22(4), 249270. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0145-2134(97)00174-9 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swanson, C. B., & Schneider, B. (1999). Students on the move: Residential and educational mobility in America’s schools. Sociology of Education, 72(1), 5467. https://doi.org/10.2307/2673186 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tajima, E. A., Herrenkohl, T. I., Huang, B., & Whitney, S. D. (2004). Measuring child maltreatment: A comparison of prospective parent reports and retrospective adolescent reports. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 74(4), 424435. https://doi.org/10.1037/0002-9432.74.4.424 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thompson, K. L., Hannan, S. M., & Miron, L. R. (2014). Fight, flight, and freeze: Threat sensitivity and emotion dysregulation in survivors of chronic childhood maltreatment. Personality and Individual Differences, 69, 2832. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.05.005 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thornberry, T. P., Henry, K. L., Ireland, T. O., & Smith, C. A. (2010). The causal impact of childhood-limited maltreatment and adolescent maltreatment on early adult adjustment. The Journal of Adolescent Health: Official Publication of the Society for Adolescent Medicine, 46(4), 359365. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2009.09.011 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thornberry, T. P., Ireland, T. O., & Smith, C. A. (2001). The importance of timing: The varying impact of childhood and adolescent maltreatment on multiple problem outcomes. Development and Psychopathology, 13(4), 957979. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579401004114 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tottenham, N., & Sheridan, M. A. (2010). A review of adversity, the amygdala and the hippocampus: A consideration of developmental timing. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 3, 1019. https://doi.org/10.3389/neuro.09.068.2009 Google ScholarPubMed
Trentacosta, C. J., Hyde, L. W., Shaw, D. S., Dishion, T. J., Gardner, F., & Wilson, M. (2008). The relations among cumulative risk, parenting, and behavior problems during early childhood. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49(11), 12111219. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.01941.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trickett, P. K., & McBride-Chang, C. (1995). The developmental impact of different forms of child abuse and neglect. Developmental Review, 15(3), 311337. https://doi.org/10.1006/drev.1995.1012 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Woods, A. D., Davis-Kean, P., Halvorson, M. A., King, K., Logan, J., Xu, M., Bainter, S., Brown, D., Clay, J. M., Cruz, R. A., Elsherif, D., Gerasimova, D., Joyal-Desmarais, K., Moreau, D., Nissen, J., Schmidt, K., Uzdavines, A., Van Dusen, B. & Vasilev, M. Missing data and multiple imputation decision tree. PsyArXiv 2021.Google Scholar
Zeanah, C. H., Boris, N. W., & Larrieu, J. A. (1997). Infant development and developmental risk: A review of the past 10 years. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 36(2), 165178. https://doi.org/10.1097/00004583-199702000-00007 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zhu, Y., Simpkin, A. J., Suderman, M. J., Lussier, A. A., Walton, E., Dunn, E. C., & Smith, A. D. A. C. (2021). A structured approach to evaluating life-course hypotheses: Moving beyond analyses of exposed versus unexposed in the -omics context. American Journal of Epidemiology, 190(6), 11011112. https://doi.org/10.1093/aje/kwaa246 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Figure 1. Theoretical illustration of the cumulative and specificity models of adversity. The cumulative model (left) suggests that the effects of childhood adversity on socioemotional development would increase with increasing number of adversity and longer exposures to various adverse experiences, up to a specific level whereby more exposures may not yield any additional effect. On the other hand, in the specificity model (right), each adverse experience is hypothesized to have a distinct effect on socioemotional development. The magnitude of influence (i.e., the height of the curves) of a specific adversity may vary across the different types of adverse experiences and when during development they occur (i.e., where they lie on the x-axis). These theoretical models provide a basis for much research on the effect of multiple types of adversity on youth mental health.

Figure 1

Table 1. Types of SLCMA models tested in the present investigation

Figure 2

Table 2. Mean and standard deviation of adversity at each time point

Figure 3

Figure 2. Summary of model selection across all SLCMA models. Cumulative was selected 9 times (53%) in predicting youth internalizing symptoms and 10 times (56%) for externalizing symptoms. Age 9 exposure was selected six times (35%) for internalizing symptoms and six times (33%) for externalizing symptoms. Age 5 exposure was selected one time (6%) for internalizing symptoms and two times (11%) for externalizing symptoms. Finally, age 3 was selected one time (6%) for internalizing symptoms. Selected variables were determined based on the location of the elbow plots in separate timing-specific SLCMA models during the first stage of SLCMA (Figure 3).

Figure 4

Figure 3. Elbow plots of timing-specific and type-specific SLCMA models testing the associations of the childhood adversity and adolescent mental health problems. A full list of variables included in each SLCMA model is outlined in Table 1. (a) Elbow plots of timing-specific SLCMA models predicting youth internalizing symptoms. Of all 10 models, cumulative was selected in nine timing-specific SLCMA models: physical aggression, neglect, intimate partner violence, maternal depression, parental stress, residential moves, material hardship, lack of community support, and neighborhood violence. Age 9 was selected in six timing-specific SLCMA models: physical aggression, psychological aggression, intimate partner violence, parental stress, residential moves, and neighborhood violence. Age 5 was selected for one timing-specific SLCMA model: intimate partner violence. Age 3 was selected for one timing-specific SLCMA model: neglect. (b) Elbow plots of timing-specific SLCMA models predicting externalizing symptoms. Of all 10 timing-specific SLCMA models, cumulative was selected in all models. Age 9 was selected in six timing-specific SLCMA models: physical aggression, psychological aggression, maternal depression, residential moves, material hardship, and lack of community support. Age 5 was selected in two timing-specific SLCMA models: neglect and intimate partner violence. (c) Elbow plots of the type-specific SLCMA models predicting internalizing (left) and externalizing (right) symptoms. Cumulative, parental stress, and lack of community support were selected in the first stage of the type-specific SLCMA model in predicting internalizing symptoms. Post-selection inference showed that all three variables significantly predicted internalizing symptoms. Cumulative, psychological aggression, and material hardship were selected in the first stage of type-specific SLCMA models. Post-selection inference showed that both cumulative and psychological aggression significantly predicted externalizing behaviors.

Figure 5

Table 3. Models testing cumulative versus specificity by developmental timing to predict internalizing symptoms

Figure 6

Table 4. Models testing cumulative versus specificity by developmental timing to predict externalizing symptoms

Figure 7

Table 5. Zero-order correlations of adversity variables (average across 1, 3, 5, 9 years old)

Figure 8

Table 6. Models testing cumulative versus specificity by adversity type to predict youth internalizing and externalizing

Supplementary material: File

Hardi et al. supplementary material

Hardi et al. supplementary material
Download Hardi et al. supplementary material(File)
File 59.2 KB