Introduction
Emotion dysregulation (ED) is defined as a pattern of emotional experience or expression that interferes with appropriate goal-directed behavior (Thompson, Reference Thompson2019). The erratic changes in an individual's emotions can complicate their effective regulation, contributing to the development of ED (Cole, Ramsook, & Ram, Reference Cole, Ramsook and Ram2019), which is a transdiagnostic dimension observed in many mental illnesses (Beauchaine & Cicchetti, Reference Beauchaine and Cicchetti2019) with different degrees of pervasiveness (Aldao, Nolen-Hoeksema, & Schweizer, Reference Aldao, Nolen-Hoeksema and Schweizer2010). For instance, people diagnosed with major depressive disorder (MDD), anxiety and eating disorders (D'Agostino, Covanti, Monti, & Starcevic, Reference D'Agostino, Covanti, Monti and Starcevic2017), or schizophrenia spectrum disorders (Liu, Chan, Chong, Subramaniam, & Mahendran, Reference Liu, Chan, Chong, Subramaniam and Mahendran2020) may present reduced emotional awareness, while people diagnosed with borderline personality disorder (BPD), attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), or bipolar disorder (BD) may display inadequate emotional reactivity or intense emotional expression (D'Agostino et al., Reference D'Agostino, Covanti, Monti and Starcevic2017). Specifically, people diagnosed with BD often show difficulties in facial emotion recognition (Ulusoy, Gülseren, Özkan, & Bilen, Reference Ulusoy, Gülseren, Özkan and Bilen2020) or emotional experience (Carolan & Power, Reference Carolan and Power2011) and are more likely to feel emotions more intensely (Muhtadie, Johnson, Carver, Gotlib, & Ketter, Reference Muhtadie, Johnson, Carver, Gotlib and Ketter2014) compared to the general population. It appears that ED may be influenced by deficits in social cognition, like the ability to correctly perceive emotional information from others or the ability to make predictions about their attitudes or behaviors (Rowland et al., Reference Rowland, Hamilton, Vella, Lino, Mitchell and Green2013b). Emotional intelligence, a domain of social cognition, is also often impaired in people with BD who seem to have more difficulties in perceiving, using, understanding, and managing emotions (Varo et al., Reference Varo, Amoretti, Sparacino, Jiménez, Solé, del Mar Bonnin and Salagre2022). Emotion regulation (ER) is the ability of an individual to monitor, appraise, or adjust emotional reactions (Gross, Reference Gross2015) through the implementation of a range of strategies that can be labeled as adaptive (i.e. cognitive reframing, adaptive coping, and acceptance) or maladaptive (i.e. negative and positive rumination, negative focus, risk-taking behavior, suppression, and dampening) (Dodd, Lockwood, Mansell, & Palmier-Claus, Reference Dodd, Lockwood, Mansell and Palmier-Claus2019). ER requires complex interactions between cortical (i.e. prefrontal and orbitofrontal cortex) and subcortical regions (i.e. amygdala and hypothalamus) (Green & Malhi, Reference Green and Malhi2006) that appear to be altered in patients with BD (Bigot et al., Reference Bigot, Alonso, Houenou, Sarrazin, Dargél, Lledo and Henry2020). Two studies (Kjærstad et al., Reference Kjærstad, Damgaard, Knudsen, Vinberg, Kessing, Macoveanu and Miskowiak2022; Njau et al., Reference Njau, Townsend, Wade, Hellemann, Bookheimer, Narr and Brooks2020) reported a neurobiological heterogeneity in BD during the activation of these neural networks, defining two groups of patients. One of these groups showed higher amygdala activation, while the other exhibited a lower activation of both amygdala and prefrontal cortex during the experience of negative emotions. Depending on these clusters underlining different modalities of neuronal activity, people may preferably rely on specific ER strategies rather than others (Kjærstad et al., Reference Kjærstad, Damgaard, Knudsen, Vinberg, Kessing, Macoveanu and Miskowiak2022), which further contributes to the high heterogeneity of the construct. In our recent meta-analysis (De Prisco et al., Reference De Prisco, Oliva, Fico, Fornaro, de Bartolomeis, Serretti and Murru2022), individuals with BD were shown to adopt more maladaptive ER strategies and fewer adaptive ones than healthy controls and first-degree relatives, with the highest and strongest disparities found in ruminative and risk-taking behaviors. Whether these differences are characteristics of BD or are due to a common psychopathological burden shared by many psychiatric disorders requires further investigation. Indeed, studies focusing on differences in ED between BD and MDD reported mixed results, and higher levels of overall ED were described in both the former (Oymak Yenilmez et al., Reference Oymak Yenilmez, Atagün, Keleş Altun, Tunç, Uzgel, Altınbaş and Oral2021) and the latter (Becerra et al., Reference Becerra, Cruise, Murray, Bassett, Harms, Allan and Hood2013). As ED appears to be a transdiagnostic dimension and a potential target for personalized interventions, it is important to define the extent to which certain features are specific to BD or shared with other psychiatric disorders.
Given its complexity, several scales have been developed offering quick and useful clinical instruments to begin exploring this dimension, each one measuring specific aspects of ED. For example, following the operationalization proposed in a previous systematic review (Dodd et al., Reference Dodd, Lockwood, Mansell and Palmier-Claus2019), the Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (CERQ) (Garnefski & Kraaij, Reference Garnefski and Kraaij2007) is used to capture both maladaptive (i.e. rumination and negative focus) and adaptive (i.e. cognitive reframing and acceptance) ER strategies, the Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (ERQ) (Gross & John, Reference Gross and John2003) is employed to measure the levels of suppression and adaptive coping, while the Response Style Questionnaire (RSQ) (Nolen-Hoeksema, Reference Nolen-Hoeksema1991) is designed to measure the degree of negative rumination, risk-taking behavior, and adaptive coping. Other scales focus strictly on maladaptive strategies, such as the Ruminative Response Scale (RRS) (Treynor, Gonzalez, & Nolen-Hoeksema, Reference Treynor, Gonzalez and Nolen-Hoeksema2003) and the Response to Positive Affect (RPA) scale (Feldman, Joormann, & Johnson, Reference Feldman, Joormann and Johnson2008), which specifically assess negative rumination and positive rumination and dampening, respectively. Finally, the Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale (DERS) (Gratz & Roemer, Reference Gratz and Roemer2004) provides an overall assessment of ED while also allowing for a more targeted description of the difficulties in using specific strategies (i.e. risk-taking behavior, adaptive coping, and acceptance).
The present study aims to identify the ED characteristics and ER strategies that discriminate between individuals with BD from individuals diagnosed with any other psychiatric disorder when using these validated measurement scales.
Methods
The present systematic review and meta-analysis was conducted according to the Meta-analysis of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (MOOSE) (Stroup et al., Reference Stroup, Berlin, Morton, Olkin, Williamson, Rennie and Thacker2000) and the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) guidelines (Page et al., Reference Page, McKenzie, Bossuyt, Boutron, Hoffmann, Mulrow and Brennan2021). This systematic review and meta-analysis protocol was registered on the International Prospective Register of Systematic Reviews (PROSPERO) (https://www.crd.york.ac.uk/PROSPERO/; protocol CRD42021293357).
Eligibility criteria and study outcomes
Inclusion criteria were as follows: (i) original studies providing quantitative data on ER and/or ED, (ii) measured with a validated scale, (iii) in people diagnosed with BD, and (iv) compared with clinical groups (people diagnosed with psychiatric disorders other than BD). Psychiatric diagnoses had to be made according to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual for Mental Disorders (DSM) (APA, 1994, 2000, 2013) or the International Classification of Diseases (ICD) (WHO, 2004) diagnostic criteria. No sample size or language restrictions were applied. Both observational and interventional studies were eligible for inclusion, but we only considered baseline data. In the case of overlapping populations across multiple studies, we included the largest one with the most representative data relevant to our objectives. Exclusion criteria were as follows: (i) Studies that assessed ER and/or ED through behavioral measures or cognitive tasks of both explicit (e.g. affective picture viewing task and emotional film clips task) and implicit (e.g. affective go/no-go task and emotional stroop test) types; (ii) reviews; (iii) case reports and case series (not reliable control group); and (iv) studies conducted on animals (population not considered by our criteria).
Search strategy
We systematically searched the PubMed/MEDLINE, EMBASE, Scopus, and PsycINFO databases from inception until April 28th, 2022. Search strings are provided in online Supplementary Materials S1. The references of each included study, textbooks, and other material were hand-searched to identify potential additional studies not captured by the original search string.
Study selection and data extraction
Studies of potential interest were independently screened by two authors (M.D.P. and V.O.). A third author (A.M.) was consulted whenever a consensus could not be reached. Data extraction included (when available): author(s), publication year, geographical region and country, study design, diagnostic criteria, and (semi)structured interview adopted, setting of the study, type of validated scale, outcome type (primary or secondary), type of control group, number of cases and controls, mean and standard deviation (s.d.) of the outcome for cases and controls, mean age of cases and controls, % of females among cases and controls, duration of illness among people with BD, % of people diagnosed with BD-I, % of euthymic, depressed, or (hypo)manic patients, % of patients taking psychotropic medication, psychiatric or other medical comorbidities, and mean score obtained by cases and controls at symptom severity scales. WebPlotDigitizer was used to extract numerical variables from graphs whenever needed (https://automeris.io/WebPlotDigitizer/). In cases where the information was unavailable, we contacted the authors twice to request the necessary data.
Methodological quality appraisal
The risk of bias in the included studies was independently assessed by two authors (M.D.P. and V.O.), and any disagreement was resolved by a third author (A.M.). The Newcastle-Ottawa Scale (NOS) (Stang, Reference Stang2010) was adopted to grade the quality of observational studies, and the scores obtained at the NOS were converted to ‘Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality’ (AHRQ) standards as described elsewhere (Sharmin et al., Reference Sharmin, Kypri, Khanam, Wadolowski, Bruno and Mattick2017).
Statistical analyses
We performed the analyses using RStudio R version 4.1.2 (R Core Team, 2020) and conducted the meta-analysis through the metafor R-package (Viechtbauer & Viechtbauer, Reference Viechtbauer and Viechtbauer2015) using a random-effect model (restricted maximum-likelihood estimator) (Harville, Reference Harville1977). Effect sizes were calculated as standardized mean differences (s.m.d.) with their confidence intervals (CI) and represented by Hedge's g. We conducted sensitivity analyses by removing one study at a time from the analysis (leave-one-out analysis), by considering only good-quality studies according to AHRQ standards (high quality), and by including in the analyses only those studies that compared euthymic-only or depressed-only patients in both groups. Because we ran many statistical tests due to the large number of scales and subscales considered, we also conducted a sensitivity analysis using a more conservative significance level corrected for the number of total tests: this correction was done with the ‘p.adjust()’ function of the stats R-package (R Core Team, 2020) using the Bonferroni method. We performed cumulative analyses to evaluate the repercussions of the studies published over the years on the effect size. We assessed heterogeneity by using the Cochran's Q test (Cochran, Reference Cochran1950) and τ 2 and I 2 statistics (Higgins et al., Reference Higgins, Thomas, Chandler, Cumpston, Li, Page and Welch2019), and this was graphically evaluated by adopting the graphical display of study heterogeneity (GOSH) method (Olkin, Dahabreh, & Trikalinos, Reference Olkin, Dahabreh and Trikalinos2012); additionally, we estimated prediction intervals (Borenstein, Higgins, Hedges, & Rothstein, Reference Borenstein, Higgins, Hedges and Rothstein2017). When the Cochran's Q test presented a p < 0.05 and the I 2 statistic showed a value >50% and the analysis included more than three studies, we conducted a subgroup analysis according to a priori defined subgroups, whenever available (i.e. BD-type, the current mood-state, and the type of outcome according to the original study).
Results
A total of 3239 studies were identified across different sources, and after a semi-automatic duplicate removal, 1813 studies underwent further screening. Of these, 1667 were excluded at the title/abstract level, 112 after the full-text evaluation, and five could not be retrieved. Altogether, 29 studies were included in the present research, and 22 (yielding 145 comparisons) were eligible for performing a meta-analysis. Among the latter, 18 studies (100 comparisons) compared people diagnosed with BD to people diagnosed with MDD, while four studies (45 comparisons) compared patients with BD to patients with BPD. The PRISMA flowchart is shown in Fig. 1. Details on the studies included in the systematic review and meta-analysis are presented in Table 1 and in online Supplementary Materials S1. Details on the excluded studies are presented in online Supplementary Materials S1.
Note: AAQ-II, Acceptance and Action Questionnaire, II; ACS, Affective Control Scale; ACS-1, anger; ACS-2, positive-affect; ACS-3, depression; ACS-4, anxiety; ADHD, attention deficit-hyperactivity disorder; BD, bipolar disorder; BDI, Beck Depression Inventory; BPD, borderline personality disorder; CERQ, Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire; CERQ-1, self-blame; CERQ-2, blaming others; CERQ-3, rumination; CERQ-4, catastrophizing; CERQ-5, putting into perspective; CERQ-6, positive refocus; CERQ-7, positive reappraisal; CERQ-8, acceptance; CERQ-9, focus on replanning; DASS, Depression Anxiety Stress Scales; DERS, Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale; DERS-1, nonacceptance; DERS-2, goals; DERS-3, impulse; DERS-4, awareness; DERS-5, strategies; DERS-6, clarity; DERS-TOT, total score; DIGS, diagnostic interview for genetic studies; DSM, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders; ERQ, Emotion Regulation Questionnaire; ERQ-1, reappraisal; ERQ-2, suppression; HDRS, Hamilton Depression Rating Scale; HC, healthy controls; IDS-C, Inventory of Depressive Symptomatology; LESS, Leahy Emotional Schema Scale; LESS-1, adaptive emotional schemas; LESS-2, rigid emotional schemas; LESS-3, negative beliefs about emotions; MADRS, Montgomery-Asberg Depression Rating Scale; MADRS, Montgomery–Åsberg Depression Rating Scale; MDD, major depressive disorder; MINI, The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview; MRI, magnetic resonance imaging; NOS, Newcastle-Ottawa Scale; PHQ-9, Patient Health Questionnaire-9; PTSD, post-traumatic stress disorder; QIDS-SR, Quick Inventory of Depressive Symptoms-Self Report; RIPoSt-40, Reactivity, Intensity, Polarity and Stability Questionnaire; RIPoSt-40-1, affective instability; RIPoSt-40-2, emotional impulsivity; RIPoSt-40-3, negative emotionality; RIPoSt-40-4, positive emotionality; RIPoSt-Y, Reactivity, Intensity, Polarity and Stability Questionnaire, Youth version; RIPoSt-Y-1, affective instability; RIPoSt-Y-2, positive emotionality; RIPoSt-Y-3, emotional reactivity; RIPoSt-Y-4, interpersonal sensitivity; RPA, response to positive affect; RPA-1, emotion focus; RPA-2, dampening; RPA-3, self-focus; RRS, Ruminative Response Scale; RRS-1, reflective pondering; RRS-2, brooming; RSQ, Response Styles Questionnaire; RSQ-1, rumination; RSQ-2, adaptive; RSQ-3, risk-taking; SADS, schedule for affective disorders and schizophrenia; SCID, Structured Clinical Interview for DSM Disorders; YMRS, Young Mania Rating Scale.
Main analyses
The main results of the meta-analyses conducted are presented in Table 2 and Fig. 2, organized according to overall ED measures, maladaptive ER strategies, and adaptive ER strategies, as in previous systematic reviews (De Prisco et al., Reference De Prisco, Oliva, Fico, Fornaro, de Bartolomeis, Serretti and Murru2022; Dodd et al., Reference Dodd, Lockwood, Mansell and Palmier-Claus2019). Overall, people with BD significantly differed from people with MDD in adopting more positive rumination and risk-taking behaviors. On the other hand, people with BPD relied less on adaptive ER strategies and exhibited higher levels of self-blaming and catastrophic thinking than people with BD.
Note: BD, Bipolar Disorder; BPD, Borderline Personality Disorder; CERQ, Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire; CI, confidence intervals; DERS, Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale; ERQ, Emotion Regulation Questionnaire; MDD, Major Depressive Disorder; PI, prediction intervals; RPA, response to positive affect; RRS, Rumination Response Scale; RSQ, Response Style Questionnaire; s.m.d., standardized mean difference (represented as Hedge's). Significant results are shown in bold and the exact p-value is given in the relative column. When the prediction intervals are the same as the confidence interval, the p-value is the same. In the two cases where the significant prediction intervals are different from the confidence intervals, their p-value is ≤ 0.01.
Additional details on the main analyses are provided in online Supplementary Materials S1.
Subgroup analyses
Subgroup analyses were conducted only for comparisons with high heterogeneity and according to (i) the BD-type; (ii) the current mood-state; and (iii) the type of outcome according to the original study.
Studies comparing BD and MDD and exploring differences in the total DERS score as a primary outcome presented a significant and opposite effect size with reduced heterogeneity (test for subgroup difference: Q M = 7.26; p value = 0.01).
Studies comparing BD and BPD and exploring differences in the ‘clarity’ subscale of the DERS as a primary outcome presented a significantly lower effect size and reduced heterogeneity (test for subgroup difference: Q M = 5.59; p value = 0.02).
No significant differences were observed according to the BD-type or current mood state.
Additional details on the subgroup analyses are provided in online Supplementary Materials S1.
Sensitivity analyses
Sensitivity analyses were conducted (i) removing one study at a time; (ii) considering good-quality studies only; (iii) considering only those studies that compared euthymic-only or depressed-only patients in both groups; (iv) considering a more conservative significance level. GOSH plots were graphically inspected to further evaluate outliers and heterogeneity.
In studies comparing BD and MDD, the following comparisons became significant after the leave-one-out sensitivity analysis: (i) ‘reflective pondering’ subscale of the RRS, by removing the study (Aslan & Baldwin, Reference Aslan and Baldwin2021), (ii) ‘putting into perspective’ subscale of the CERQ, by removing the study (Wolkenstein, Zwick, Hautzinger, & Joormann, Reference Wolkenstein, Zwick, Hautzinger and Joormann2014), and (iii) ‘goals’ subscale of the DERS, by removing the study (Oymak Yenilmez et al., Reference Oymak Yenilmez, Atagün, Keleş Altun, Tunç, Uzgel, Altınbaş and Oral2021), all passing from a small to a small-to-moderate effect size; (iv) ‘awareness’ subscale of the DERS, by removing the study (Musket, Hansen, Welker, Gilbert, & Gruber, Reference Musket, Hansen, Welker, Gilbert and Gruber2021), passing from a small-to-moderate to a moderate effect size. On the contrary, by removing the studies ([Becerra et al., Reference Becerra, Cruise, Murray, Bassett, Harms, Allan and Hood2013], [Becerra, Bassett, & Harms, Reference Becerra, Bassett and Harms2016], or [Weintraub, Van de Loo, Gitlin, & Miklowitz, Reference Weintraub, Van de Loo, Gitlin and Miklowitz2017]) in the comparison relative to the ‘clarity’ subscale of the DERS, the overall effect became nonsignificant with an effect size comparable in magnitude.
In studies comparing BD and BPD and exploring differences of the ‘clarity’ subscale of the DERS, the overall effect became insignificant by removing the study (Bayes, Parker, & McClure, Reference Bayes, Parker and McClure2016), with an effect size comparable in magnitude.
In good-quality studies comparing BD and MDD and exploring differences of the ‘clarity’ subscale of the DERS, the overall effect became nonsignificant. In good-quality studies comparing BD and MDD and exploring differences of the ‘reflective pondering’ subscale of the RRS, the overall effect became significant.
In studies comparing BD and MDD and exploring differences in the ‘clarity’ subscale of the DERS and by solely considering only those studies that compared euthymic-only or depressed-only patients in both groups, the overall effect was not significant.
In studies comparing BD and MDD, differences found in the ‘clarity’ subscale of the DERS were no longer significant when using a more conservative significance level.
In studies comparing BD and BPD, differences found in the ‘positive refocusing’, ‘positive reappraisal’, and ‘refocus on planning’ subscales of the CERQ and ‘clarity’ subscale of the DERS were no longer significant when using a more conservative significance level.
Inspecting the GOSH plots in studies comparing BD and MDD, the following results were found (each referring to the comparisons related to each mentioned subscale): (i) in the ‘putting into perspective’ subscale of the CERQ, subsets including the study (Wolkenstein et al., Reference Wolkenstein, Zwick, Hautzinger and Joormann2014) presented higher heterogeneity and lower effect size; (ii) in the ‘positive refocusing’ subscale of the CERQ, subsets including the study (Ariana Kia & Hasani, Reference Ariana Kia and Hasani2014) presented higher heterogeneity and effect size; (iii) in the ‘refocus on planning’ subscale of the CERQ, subsets including the study (Kjærstad et al., Reference Kjærstad, Vinberg, Goldin, Køster, Støttrup, Knorr and Miskowiak2016) presented higher heterogeneity and effect size; (iv) in the ‘goals’ subscale of the DERS, subsets including the study (Oymak Yenilmez et al., Reference Oymak Yenilmez, Atagün, Keleş Altun, Tunç, Uzgel, Altınbaş and Oral2021) presented higher heterogeneity and opposite effect size; (v) in the ‘non acceptance’ subscale of the DERS, subsets including the study (Oymak Yenilmez et al., Reference Oymak Yenilmez, Atagün, Keleş Altun, Tunç, Uzgel, Altınbaş and Oral2021) presented higher heterogeneity and opposite effect size; and (vi) in the ‘awareness’ subscale of the DERS, subsets including the study (Musket et al., Reference Musket, Hansen, Welker, Gilbert and Gruber2021) presented higher heterogeneity and lower effects size.
By inspecting the GOSH plots in the studies comparing BD and BPD and exploring differences in the total DERS score, subsets including the study (Das, Calhoun, & Malhi, Reference Das, Calhoun and Malhi2014) presented higher heterogeneity and effect size.
Additional details on the sensitivity analyses and the GOSH plots are provided in online Supplementary Materials S1.
Characteristics of the studies included in the qualitative synthesis
The following studies reported information about the comparison of BD with other psychiatric conditions; however, it was not possible to perform a meta-analysis on these data. One study (Rowland et al., Reference Rowland, Hamilton, Lino, Ly, Denny, Hwang and Green2013a) compared BD with schizophrenia using the CERQ. Three studies (Kim, Kim, & Kim, Reference Kim, Kim and Kim2022; Masi et al., Reference Masi, Sesso, Pfanner, Valente, Molesti, Placini and Montesanto2021; Sesso et al., Reference Sesso, Milone, Drago, Viglione, Berloffa, Boldrini and Placini2021) compared BD with ADHD using the ERQ (Kim et al., Reference Kim, Kim and Kim2022), the Reactivity, Intensity, Polarity and Stability questionnaire (RIPoSt-40) (Masi et al., Reference Masi, Sesso, Pfanner, Valente, Molesti, Placini and Montesanto2021), or the RIPoSt youth version (Sesso et al., Reference Sesso, Milone, Drago, Viglione, Berloffa, Boldrini and Placini2021). Two studies (Becerra et al., Reference Becerra, Cruise, Murray, Bassett, Harms, Allan and Hood2013; Kim, Yu, Lee, & Kim, Reference Kim, Yu, Lee and Kim2012) compared BD with anxiety disorders adopting the DERS (Becerra et al., Reference Becerra, Cruise, Murray, Bassett, Harms, Allan and Hood2013) or the RRS (Kim et al., Reference Kim, Yu, Lee and Kim2012). Finally, one study (Kim et al., Reference Kim, Yu, Lee and Kim2012) compared BD with post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) using the ERQ (Kim et al., Reference Kim, Kim and Kim2022).
Detailed characteristics of these studies are provided in Table 1 and in online Supplementary Materials S1.
Discussion
The present systematic review and meta-analysis aimed at exploring the differences in terms of adopted ER strategies and ED characteristics among people diagnosed with BD compared with people diagnosed with other psychiatric disorders. Overall, BD patients were quite similar to patients with MDD in both overall ED and adopted ER strategies, although they showed more positive rumination and risk-taking behaviors. Patients with BD exhibited a lower degree of ED when compared with BPD patients, as they adopted more adaptive ER strategies and fewer maladaptive ones.
Specifically, no significant differences were found in almost all BD-MDD comparisons. Previous studies focusing on neuroimaging correlates of ER and ED found that patients with MDD compared to healthy controls presented altered connections between the amygdala and both lateral (Johnstone, Van Reekum, Urry, Kalin, & Davidson, Reference Johnstone, Van Reekum, Urry, Kalin and Davidson2007) and medial (Perlman et al., Reference Perlman, Simmons, Wu, Hahn, Tapert, Max and Campbell-Sills2012) prefrontal cortex during reappraisal, and similar changes were also outlined in studies comparing BD patients with healthy subjects (Bigot et al., Reference Bigot, Alonso, Houenou, Sarrazin, Dargél, Lledo and Henry2020; Townsend & Altshuler, Reference Townsend and Altshuler2012). However, when people diagnosed with BD and MDD were directly compared, they displayed significant differences in activating those brain areas during ER tasks (Rai et al., Reference Rai, Griffiths, Breukelaar, Barreiros, Chen, Boyce and Bryant2021; Rive et al., Reference Rive, Mocking, Koeter, van Wingen, de Wit, van den Heuvel and Schene2015). Taking these data together, the overall impaired connections found in these patients could support the shared difficulties in ER, while the dissimilarities might point toward psychopathological differences that are too subtle to be detected by self-administered tests used in the included studies and also a misdiagnosis of BD as MDD appears to be frequent (Daveney, Panagioti, Waheed, & Esmail, Reference Daveney, Panagioti, Waheed and Esmail2019), so diagnostic biases might dilute a possibly existing difference in terms of ED. Although most of the included studies supported their diagnoses with the adoption of structured or semistructured interview tools, BD patients may be very aware of their depressive symptoms while ignoring or minimizing hypomanic ones (Vieta et al., Reference Vieta, Berk, Schulze, Carvalho, Suppes, Calabrese and Grande2018) or experience several depressive episodes before the onset of mania or hypomania (O'Donovan & Alda, Reference O'Donovan and Alda2020), impeding the proper diagnostic orientation. Moreover, the high heterogeneity observed in some CERQ-related comparisons and in all DERS-related comparisons, which is not explained by consideration of the BD type or affective state, could also be related to other aspects interacting with ER. One study (Varo et al., Reference Varo, Kjærstad, Poulsen, Meluken, Vieta, Kessing and Miskowiak2021) explored the heterogeneity of emotional cognition in a sample of people diagnosed with BD or MDD, finding that people with mood disorders can be grouped into different clusters based on their emotional reactivity, adopted ER strategies, or correct recognition of facial expressions. Due to the unavailability of this information, it has not been possible to know how this result may have affected our population. Nevertheless, additional features beyond those related to the diagnosis alone are expected to help us better understand the phenomenon.
In our meta-analysis, patients with BD significantly differed from patients with MDD regarding positive rumination, as confirmed by the small effect sizes of the comparisons of the ‘emotion focus’ and ‘self-focus’ subscales of the RPA, and this difference remained when analyzing the prediction intervals. BD is often linked to increased expression of positive emotions in response to or in expectation of positive or rewarding stimuli (Gruber, Reference Gruber2011). The persistence of positive emotions may be related to high-risk and impulsive behavior in this population (Muhtadie et al., Reference Muhtadie, Johnson, Carver, Gotlib and Ketter2014). This type of conduct is often later associated with feelings of regret (Rydahl et al., Reference Rydahl, Brund, Medici, Straarup, Straszek and Østergaard2022), and for this reason, we expected that people with BD could have been more involved in dampening their positive emotions when compared with MDD patients. However, there was no difference in the ‘dampening’ subscale of the RPA between these groups, even when looking at the sensitivity analyses. Still, specific features of this subscale may partially explain this finding. Some of the items of the RPA contributing to the ‘dampening’ subscale score (e.g. ‘Thinking: My streak of luck is going to end soon’ or ‘Thinking: I don't deserve this’) (Feldman et al., Reference Feldman, Joormann and Johnson2008) may be related to pessimistic thinking, which is a core MDD characteristic (Katayama et al., Reference Katayama, Nakagawa, Umeda, Terasawa, Kurata, Tabuchi and Mimura2019), contributing to overall high scores in this population rather than the need to soothe positive emotions.
Patients with BD and MDD did not significantly differ in ruminating on negative emotions. Considering the comparison relative to the ‘reflective pondering’ subscale of the RRS, significance was achieved after removing one study (Aslan & Baldwin, Reference Aslan and Baldwin2021) that included only currently depressed patients. In contrast, other studies (Kim et al., Reference Kim, Yu, Lee and Kim2012; Shapero et al., Reference Shapero, Stange, Goldstein, Black, Molz, Hamlat and Alloy2015) included both symptomatic and euthymic patients in their sample, suggesting that the current affective symptomatology may influence the differences in terms of negative rumination. In addition, acutely depressed patients with MDD scored significantly higher than acutely depressed BD patients when looking at more absorbing and negative emotion-focused form of ruminations, although this comparison was not significant after conducting a sensitivity analysis. A study comparing MDD patients presenting different levels of rumination (Tsuchiyagaito et al., Reference Tsuchiyagaito, Sánchez, Misaki, Kuplicki, Park, Paulus and Guinjoan2022) observed that higher degrees of rumination were associated with greater connectivity between brain areas dedicated to language processing and areas involved in ER: the heterogeneity found at the level of brain connectivity may further influence the differences observed in our analyses.
BD and MDD patients also significantly differed at the ‘risk-taking’ subscale of the RSQ. BD is related to higher risk-taking behavior (Hıdıroğlu et al., Reference Hıdıroğlu, Esen, Tunca, Yalçìn, Lombardo, Glahn and Özerdem2013), which may be followed by dramatic social and health consequences, especially in the case of comorbid substance abuse (Meade, Graff, Griffin, & Weiss, Reference Meade, Graff, Griffin and Weiss2008). No differences were found relative to the ‘impulse’ subscale of the DERS. Most of the participants included in these studies were euthymic, and our findings corroborate with existing literature that failed to discriminate euthymic people with BD and MDD based on their trait-impulsivity (Henna et al., Reference Henna, Hatch, Nicoletti, Swann, Zunta-Soares and Soares2013; Ozten & Erol, Reference Ozten and Erol2019). This finding remained even when a subgroup analysis based on mood states was conducted, but more studies focused on acutely ill patients are lacking.
In this meta-analysis, BD patients presented a significantly lower level of overall difficulties in ER compared to BPD patients. Among maladaptive ER strategies, they significantly differ from patients with BPD in negative focus, scoring lower at the ‘self-blame’ and ‘catastrophizing’ subscales of the CERQ. Usually, patients with BPD display monocausal reasoning when explaining positive or negative situations, with the tendency to catastrophize as they only consider a little explanation (Schilling, Moritz, Schneider, Bierbrodt, & Nagel, Reference Schilling, Moritz, Schneider, Bierbrodt and Nagel2015). Additionally, self-blame seems to be a core feature of BPD (Sorgi-Wilson & McCloskey, Reference Sorgi-Wilson and McCloskey2022); it could represent a significant clinical target due to its role in internalizing symptoms like anxiety, depression, or emotional withdrawal (Tanzer, Salaminios, Morosan, Campbell, & Debbané, Reference Tanzer, Salaminios, Morosan, Campbell and Debbané2021). Despite these elements' importance in BD, their predominance in BPD may be suggested by different clinical characteristics. Indeed, unlike BD, BPD presents a more continuous and trait-like course, a lower age at onset, and fewer available treatment strategies (Leichsenring, Leibing, Kruse, New, & Leweke, Reference Leichsenring, Leibing, Kruse, New and Leweke2011), which may contribute to the more pervasive adoption of maladaptive ER strategies. For the same reasons, these groups may differ in their impulsive response to negative emotions. This finding is in line with previous reports underlining BPD's higher broad and trait-impulsiveness compared with BD patients (Bøen et al., Reference Bøen, Hummelen, Elvsåshagen, Boye, Andersson, Karterud and Malt2015).
No differences were found in the ‘rumination’ subscale of the CERQ. Since negative rumination can be defined as a form of thinking that involves excessive and repetitive negative thoughts or themes that interfere with other forms of mental activity, several types of ruminations may be considered depending on their object (e.g. depression, worry, and anger). However, the items composing the ‘rumination’ subscale of the CERQ may be too general and lacking in specificity, failing to capture the various aspects and types of rumination. Therefore, although both BD and BPD patients may share the same propensity to ruminate, they may differ in their content, considering the association of depressive and anger rumination with borderline personality features (Baer & Sauer, Reference Baer and Sauer2011).
Interestingly, in our meta-analysis, people with BD rely on more adaptive ER strategies than BPD patients. Adopting specific ER strategies is possibly influenced by the family environment and the relationships experienced during childhood (Thompson, Reference Thompson2019). Importantly, early-life abuse and childhood maltreatment are associated with higher levels of ED (Gruhn & Compas, Reference Gruhn and Compas2020). Childhood abuse is commonly reported by patients with BD (Fisher & Hosang, Reference Fisher and Hosang2010), but rates of emotional, physical, or sexual trauma seem higher in the BPD population (Merza, Papp, & Kuritárné Szabó, Reference Merza, Papp and Kuritárné Szabó2015; Temes et al., Reference Temes, Magni, Fitzmaurice, Aguirre, Goodman and Zanarini2017), suggesting that previously learned strategies, usefully adopted in response to a real or perceived threat, turned into dysfunctional or inappropriate coping mechanisms. However, some of these observed differences are not significant using a more conservative significance level, suggesting the need for further investigation in this direction.
It is important to note that all studies included in the meta-analyses adopted structured or semistructured interview tools to confirm their diagnosis and exclude comorbid cases. However, the prevalence of BPD in BD is reported to be around 21%, rising up to 37% in BD type II (Fornaro et al., Reference Fornaro, Orsolini, Marini, De Berardis, Perna, Valchera and Stubbs2016), and real-world patients may have more severe ED characteristics than those described in this systematic review and meta-analysis, as suggested by studies specifically focusing on this population (Bayes et al., Reference Bayes, Parker and McClure2016). Hence, the clinical question on the intensity of ED in BD comorbid to BPD requires additional research.
A quantitative approach was not viable to study the differences in ER-ED between BD and other psychiatric conditions. The higher scores at the ‘blaming the others’ subscale of the CERQ reported by patients diagnosed with schizophrenia may be due to the higher rate of self-serving bias or broader social cognition deficits described in this population (Müller, Betz, & Bechdolf, Reference Müller, Betz and Bechdolf2021). People with BD compared to young patients with ADHD showed higher levels of ED relative to negative emotions. Despite no available evidence allowing us to explore this comparison in adult life, varying clinical course and high rates of psychiatric comorbidities (Franke et al., Reference Franke, Michelini, Asherson, Banaschewski, Bilbow, Buitelaar and Haavik2018) could worsen the intensity and presentation of ED in ADHD patients. Studies comparing BD to anxiety disorders reported contrasting results and this may be due to the broad diagnostic category considered. Anxiety disorders are in fact conditions with widely heterogeneous clinical presentations and severity, so future studies should consider these entities separately to assess ED.
To the best of our knowledge, this is the first systematic review and meta-analysis focusing on different aspects of ED in BD compared with other psychiatric disorders. In this meta-analysis, patients with BD displayed fewer differences than those found in our previous work involving healthy controls and first-degree relatives (De Prisco et al., Reference De Prisco, Oliva, Fico, Fornaro, de Bartolomeis, Serretti and Murru2022). ED is a transdiagnostic construct, present throughout different disorders, albeit with different pervasiveness. Our results can be useful in outlining features that are particularly relevant for understanding BD, although the extent of divergences between conditions is smaller. As suggested for mood instability (Kessing & Faurholt-Jepsen, Reference Kessing and Faurholt-Jepsen2022), ED assessment in clinical trials may be more valuable than conventional outcomes in estimating disease severity or patient functioning. In general, addressing ED in clinical practice can be helpful in accelerating the transition from categorical to quantitative nosology. In fact, this dimension lies above the classical diagnostic categories and could guide future genetic research better than the latter (Kotov et al., Reference Kotov, Krueger, Watson, Cicero, Conway, DeYoung and Latzman2021) as well as to assist the development of treatments aimed at these transdiagnostic features (Capitão et al., Reference Capitão, Chapman, Filippini, Wright, Murphy, James and Harmer2022; Solmi et al., Reference Solmi, Bodini, Cocozza, Seeman, Vieta, Dragioti and Fusar-Poli2020).
The present study has some limitations. First, the available evidence precluded us from making meta-analytic comparisons with other psychiatric disorders like schizophrenia, ADHD, anxiety disorders, or PTSD, limiting the understanding of differential ED features between BD and those conditions. As described in other ED-related constructs (Miskowiak & Varo, Reference Miskowiak and Varo2021), there is no specific consensus on which tools are more appropriate to assess ED, with the risk that the same aspects are measured by different tools, making a meta-analytic approach difficult. Second, the paucity of studies did not permit us to perform meta-regressions to study the impact of continuous variables on the overall effect size or to explore publication bias. Meta-regressions on affective symptom severity scales would have given us a better understanding of their role on different effect sizes, especially considering that in some studies the two groups appear to differ significantly, although often within a similar range of severity. Third, precise data about medical and psychiatric comorbidities or psychopharmacological treatments were seldom reported, limiting our understanding of their influence on ED. Additionally, we did not have data on patients' previous psychotherapy experience or whether they suffered from subthreshold symptomatology that is concerning, given their role on ED (Crockett, Martínez, & Jiménez-Molina, Reference Crockett, Martínez and Jiménez-Molina2020; Sloan et al., Reference Sloan, Hall, Moulding, Bryce, Mildred and Staiger2017). Fourth, sample sizes were small in general, and only a few studies contributed to any comparison, suggesting the need for further studies on the matter. Fifth, most of the included studies did not provide sufficient information on sample recruitment or the statistical methods used to calculate sample size, and the study populations were not always adequately matched. As a result, less than a third of them were considered to be of good quality, limiting the strength of the observations made in this review. Sixth, the exclusion from this review of studies assessing ED (or related aspects) through behavioral measures limits the generalizability of our findings. Although our goal was to focus on instruments that are quick to administer and easy to use in clinical settings, the inclusion of tasks designed to explore explicit or implicit emotional regulation may have allowed for the identification of more subtle differences in the populations studied. Seventh, some studies included populations without specifying their affective status or enrolled patients at different phases of illness. In the latter cases, however, most of the patients in the two groups belonged to the same affective polarity.
In conclusion, ED is present in different degrees among psychiatric disorders. This construct appears as a transdiagnostic feature, and small to moderate differences were found when comparing BD with other conditions. Patients diagnosed with BD did not differ from those diagnosed with MDD in adopting many of the adaptive and maladaptive ER strategies. However, positive rumination and risk-taking behaviors were more common in the former. Conversely, people with BPD displayed a higher degree of ED compared with patients diagnosed with BD. This suggests a potential clue to clinically discriminate between these two populations and a potential treatment target.
Supplementary material
The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S003329172300243X
Acknowledgements
G.F. received the support of a fellowship from ‘La Caixa’ Foundation (ID 100010434 – fellowship code LCF/BQ/DR21/11880019). J.R. acknowledges the support of the Spanish Ministry of Science and Innovation (PI19/00394, CPII19/00009) integrated into the Plan Nacional de I + D + I and co-financed by the ISCIII-Subdirección General de Evaluación and the Fondo Europeo de Desarrollo Regional (FEDER) and the Instituto de Salud Carlos III. I.G. acknowledges the support of the Spanish Ministry of Science and Innovation (MCIN) (PI19/00954) integrated into the Plan Nacional de I + D + I and cofinanced by the ISCIII-Subdirección General de Evaluación y el Fondos Europeos de la Unión Europea (FEDER, FSE, Next Generation EU/Plan de Recuperación Transformación y Resiliencia_PRTR); the Instituto de Salud Carlos III; the CIBER of Mental Health (CIBERSAM); and the the Secretaria d'Universitats i Recerca del Departament d'Economia i Coneixement 2021-SGR-01358), CERCA Programme/Generalitat de Catalunya as well as the Fundació Clínic per la Recerca Biomèdica (Pons Bartran 2022-FRCB_PB1_2022). G.A. was supported by a Rio Hortega 2021 Grant (CM21/00017) from the Spanish Ministry of Health financed by the Instituto de Salud Carlos III (ISCIII) and co-financed by the Fondo Social Europeo Plus (FSE+). DHM's research was supported by a Juan Rodés JR18/00021 granted by the Instituto de Salud Carlos III (ISCIII). E.V. acknowledges the support of the Spanish Ministry of Science and Innovation (PI18/00805, PI21/00787) integrated into the Plan Nacional de I + D + I and co-financed by the ISCIII-Subdirección General de Evaluación and the Fondo Europeo de Desarrollo Regional (FEDER); the Instituto de Salud Carlos III; the CIBER of Mental Health (CIBERSAM); the Secretaria d'Universitats i Recerca del Departament d'Economia i Coneixement (2021-SGR-01358), the CERCA Programme, and the Departament de Salut de la Generalitat de Catalunya for the PERIS Grant SLT006/17/00357. Thanks the support of the European Union Horizon 2020 research and innovation program (EU.3.1.1. Understanding health, wellbeing and disease: Grant No. 754907 and EU.3.1.3. Treating and managing disease: Grant No. 945151). A.M. acknowledges the support of the Spanish Ministry of Science and Innovation (PI19/00672) integrated into the Plan Nacional de I + D + I and co-financed by the ISCIII-Subdirección General de Evaluación and the Fondo Europeo de Desarrollo Regional (FEDER). All authors are grateful to Derek Clougher for carefully reviewing the language throughout the text.
Funding statement
This research received no specific grant from any funding agency, commercial or not-for-profit sectors, other than those mentioned in the acknowledgements.
Competing interests
G.F. has received CME-related honoraria or consulting fees from Angelini, Janssen-Cilag, and Lundbeck; I.G. has received grants and served as a consultant, advisor, or CME speaker for the following identities: ADAMED, Angelini, Casen Recordati, Ferrer, Janssen Cilag, and Lundbeck, Lundbeck-Otsuka, Luye, SEI Healthcare outside the submitted work;
G.A. has received CME-related honoraria or consulting fees from Janssen-Cilag, Lundbeck, Lundbeck/Otsuka, and Angelini, with no financial or other relationship relevant to the subject of this article.
A.d.B. has received research support from Janssen, Lundbeck, and Otsuka and lecture fees for educational meeting from Chiesi, Lundbeck, Roche, Sunovion, Vitria, Recordati, Angelini, and Takeda; he has served on advisory boards for Eli Lilly, Jansen, Lundbeck, Otsuka, Roche, and Takeda, Chiesi, Recordati, Angelini, and Vitria; A.S. is or has been a consultant/speaker for Abbott, Abbvie, Angelini, AstraZeneca, Clinical Data, Boehringer, Bristol-Myers Squibb, Eli Lilly, GlaxoSmithKline, Innovapharma, Italfarmaco, Janssen, Lundbeck, Naurex, Pfizer, Polifarma, Sanofi, Servier, and Taliaz; E.V. has received grants and served as a consultant, advisor, or CME speaker for the following entities: AB-Biotics, AbbVie, Angelini, Biogen, Boehringer-Ingelheim, Celon Pharma, Dainippon Sumitomo Pharma, Ethypharm, Ferrer, Gedeon Richter, GH Research, Glaxo-Smith Kline, Janssen, Lundbeck, Medincell, Novartis, Orion Corporation, Organon, Otsuka, Rovi, Sage, Sanofi-Aventis, Sunovion, Takeda, and Viatris, outside the submitted work; A.M. has received grants and served as a consultant, advisor, or CME speaker for the following entities: Angelini, Idorsia, Lundbeck, Pfizer, and Takeda, outside of the submitted work. All the other authors have no conflict to declare.
Open practices statement
The datasets and the codes used for this research are available on request.