Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-8ctnn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-25T18:55:19.439Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Emotion dysregulation in bipolar disorder compared to other mental illnesses: a systematic review and meta-analysis

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  16 October 2023

Michele De Prisco
Affiliation:
Departament de Medicina, Facultat de Medicina i Ciències de la Salut, Universitat de Barcelona (UB), c. Casanova, 143, 08036 Barcelona, Spain Bipolar and Depressive Disorders Unit, Hospìtal Clinic de Barcelona. c. Villarroel, 170, 08036 Barcelona, Spain Institut d'Investigacions Biomèdiques August Pi i Sunyer (IDIBAPS), c. Villarroel, 170, 08036 Barcelona, Spain Institute of Neurosciences (UBNeuro), Barcelona, Spain Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red de Salud Mental (CIBERSAM), Instituto de Salud Carlos III, Madrid, Spain Section of Psychiatry, Department of Neuroscience, Reproductive Science and Odontostomatology Federico II University of Naples, Naples, Italy
Vincenzo Oliva
Affiliation:
Departament de Medicina, Facultat de Medicina i Ciències de la Salut, Universitat de Barcelona (UB), c. Casanova, 143, 08036 Barcelona, Spain Bipolar and Depressive Disorders Unit, Hospìtal Clinic de Barcelona. c. Villarroel, 170, 08036 Barcelona, Spain Institut d'Investigacions Biomèdiques August Pi i Sunyer (IDIBAPS), c. Villarroel, 170, 08036 Barcelona, Spain Institute of Neurosciences (UBNeuro), Barcelona, Spain Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red de Salud Mental (CIBERSAM), Instituto de Salud Carlos III, Madrid, Spain Department of Biomedical and Neuromotor Sciences, University of Bologna, Bologna, Italy
Giovanna Fico
Affiliation:
Departament de Medicina, Facultat de Medicina i Ciències de la Salut, Universitat de Barcelona (UB), c. Casanova, 143, 08036 Barcelona, Spain Bipolar and Depressive Disorders Unit, Hospìtal Clinic de Barcelona. c. Villarroel, 170, 08036 Barcelona, Spain Institut d'Investigacions Biomèdiques August Pi i Sunyer (IDIBAPS), c. Villarroel, 170, 08036 Barcelona, Spain Institute of Neurosciences (UBNeuro), Barcelona, Spain Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red de Salud Mental (CIBERSAM), Instituto de Salud Carlos III, Madrid, Spain
Joaquim Radua
Affiliation:
Departament de Medicina, Facultat de Medicina i Ciències de la Salut, Universitat de Barcelona (UB), c. Casanova, 143, 08036 Barcelona, Spain Institute of Neurosciences (UBNeuro), Barcelona, Spain Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red de Salud Mental (CIBERSAM), Instituto de Salud Carlos III, Madrid, Spain Imaging of Mood- and Anxiety-Related Disorders (IMARD) Group, IDIBAPS, Barcelona, Spain Early Psychosis: Interventions and Clinical-Detection (EPIC) Lab, Department of Psychosis Studies, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King's College London, London, UK Centre for Psychiatric Research and Education, Department of Clinical Neuroscience, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden
Iria Grande
Affiliation:
Departament de Medicina, Facultat de Medicina i Ciències de la Salut, Universitat de Barcelona (UB), c. Casanova, 143, 08036 Barcelona, Spain Bipolar and Depressive Disorders Unit, Hospìtal Clinic de Barcelona. c. Villarroel, 170, 08036 Barcelona, Spain Institut d'Investigacions Biomèdiques August Pi i Sunyer (IDIBAPS), c. Villarroel, 170, 08036 Barcelona, Spain Institute of Neurosciences (UBNeuro), Barcelona, Spain Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red de Salud Mental (CIBERSAM), Instituto de Salud Carlos III, Madrid, Spain
Natalia Roberto
Affiliation:
Departament de Medicina, Facultat de Medicina i Ciències de la Salut, Universitat de Barcelona (UB), c. Casanova, 143, 08036 Barcelona, Spain Bipolar and Depressive Disorders Unit, Hospìtal Clinic de Barcelona. c. Villarroel, 170, 08036 Barcelona, Spain Institut d'Investigacions Biomèdiques August Pi i Sunyer (IDIBAPS), c. Villarroel, 170, 08036 Barcelona, Spain Institute of Neurosciences (UBNeuro), Barcelona, Spain Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red de Salud Mental (CIBERSAM), Instituto de Salud Carlos III, Madrid, Spain
Gerard Anmella
Affiliation:
Departament de Medicina, Facultat de Medicina i Ciències de la Salut, Universitat de Barcelona (UB), c. Casanova, 143, 08036 Barcelona, Spain Bipolar and Depressive Disorders Unit, Hospìtal Clinic de Barcelona. c. Villarroel, 170, 08036 Barcelona, Spain Institut d'Investigacions Biomèdiques August Pi i Sunyer (IDIBAPS), c. Villarroel, 170, 08036 Barcelona, Spain Institute of Neurosciences (UBNeuro), Barcelona, Spain Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red de Salud Mental (CIBERSAM), Instituto de Salud Carlos III, Madrid, Spain
Diego Hidalgo-Mazzei
Affiliation:
Departament de Medicina, Facultat de Medicina i Ciències de la Salut, Universitat de Barcelona (UB), c. Casanova, 143, 08036 Barcelona, Spain Bipolar and Depressive Disorders Unit, Hospìtal Clinic de Barcelona. c. Villarroel, 170, 08036 Barcelona, Spain Institut d'Investigacions Biomèdiques August Pi i Sunyer (IDIBAPS), c. Villarroel, 170, 08036 Barcelona, Spain Institute of Neurosciences (UBNeuro), Barcelona, Spain Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red de Salud Mental (CIBERSAM), Instituto de Salud Carlos III, Madrid, Spain
Michele Fornaro
Affiliation:
Section of Psychiatry, Department of Neuroscience, Reproductive Science and Odontostomatology Federico II University of Naples, Naples, Italy
Andrea de Bartolomeis
Affiliation:
Section of Psychiatry, Department of Neuroscience, Reproductive Science and Odontostomatology Federico II University of Naples, Naples, Italy
Alessandro Serretti
Affiliation:
Department of Biomedical and Neuromotor Sciences, University of Bologna, Bologna, Italy
Eduard Vieta*
Affiliation:
Departament de Medicina, Facultat de Medicina i Ciències de la Salut, Universitat de Barcelona (UB), c. Casanova, 143, 08036 Barcelona, Spain Bipolar and Depressive Disorders Unit, Hospìtal Clinic de Barcelona. c. Villarroel, 170, 08036 Barcelona, Spain Institut d'Investigacions Biomèdiques August Pi i Sunyer (IDIBAPS), c. Villarroel, 170, 08036 Barcelona, Spain Institute of Neurosciences (UBNeuro), Barcelona, Spain Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red de Salud Mental (CIBERSAM), Instituto de Salud Carlos III, Madrid, Spain
Andrea Murru
Affiliation:
Departament de Medicina, Facultat de Medicina i Ciències de la Salut, Universitat de Barcelona (UB), c. Casanova, 143, 08036 Barcelona, Spain Bipolar and Depressive Disorders Unit, Hospìtal Clinic de Barcelona. c. Villarroel, 170, 08036 Barcelona, Spain Institut d'Investigacions Biomèdiques August Pi i Sunyer (IDIBAPS), c. Villarroel, 170, 08036 Barcelona, Spain Institute of Neurosciences (UBNeuro), Barcelona, Spain Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red de Salud Mental (CIBERSAM), Instituto de Salud Carlos III, Madrid, Spain
*
Corresponding author: Eduard Vieta; Email: evieta@clinic.cat
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

People with bipolar disorder (BD) often present emotion dysregulation (ED), a pattern of emotional expression interfering with goal-directed behavior. ED is a transdiagnostic construct, and it is unclear whether it manifests itself similarly in other conditions, such as major depressive disorder (MDD) or borderline personality disorder (BPD), or has specific features in BD. The present systematic review and meta-analysis explored ED and adopted emotion regulation (ER) strategies in BD compared with other psychiatric conditions. PubMed/MEDLINE, EMBASE, Scopus, and PsycINFO databases were systematically searched from inception to April 28th, 2022. Studies implementing validated instruments assessing ED or ER strategies in BD and other psychiatric disorders were reviewed, and meta-analyses were conducted. Twenty-nine studies yielding multiple comparisons were included. BD was compared to MDD in 20 studies (n = 2451), to BPD in six studies (n = 1001), to attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in three studies (n = 232), to anxiety disorders in two studies (n = 320), to schizophrenia in one study (n = 223), and to post-traumatic stress disorder in one study (n = 31). BD patients did not differ from MDD patients in adopting most adaptive and maladaptive ER strategies. However, small-to-moderate differences in positive rumination and risk-taking behaviors were observed. In contrast, patients with BPD presented an overall higher degree of ED and more maladaptive ER strategies. There were insufficient data for a meta-analytic comparison with other psychiatric disorders. The present report further supports the idea that ED is a transdiagnostic construct spanning a continuum across different psychiatric disorders, outlining specific clinical features that could represent potential therapeutic targets.

Type
Review Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BYCreative Common License - NCCreative Common License - ND
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0), which permits non-commercial re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided that no alterations are made and the original article is properly cited. The written permission of Cambridge University Press must be obtained prior to any commercial use and/or adaptation of the article.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Emotion dysregulation (ED) is defined as a pattern of emotional experience or expression that interferes with appropriate goal-directed behavior (Thompson, Reference Thompson2019). The erratic changes in an individual's emotions can complicate their effective regulation, contributing to the development of ED (Cole, Ramsook, & Ram, Reference Cole, Ramsook and Ram2019), which is a transdiagnostic dimension observed in many mental illnesses (Beauchaine & Cicchetti, Reference Beauchaine and Cicchetti2019) with different degrees of pervasiveness (Aldao, Nolen-Hoeksema, & Schweizer, Reference Aldao, Nolen-Hoeksema and Schweizer2010). For instance, people diagnosed with major depressive disorder (MDD), anxiety and eating disorders (D'Agostino, Covanti, Monti, & Starcevic, Reference D'Agostino, Covanti, Monti and Starcevic2017), or schizophrenia spectrum disorders (Liu, Chan, Chong, Subramaniam, & Mahendran, Reference Liu, Chan, Chong, Subramaniam and Mahendran2020) may present reduced emotional awareness, while people diagnosed with borderline personality disorder (BPD), attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), or bipolar disorder (BD) may display inadequate emotional reactivity or intense emotional expression (D'Agostino et al., Reference D'Agostino, Covanti, Monti and Starcevic2017). Specifically, people diagnosed with BD often show difficulties in facial emotion recognition (Ulusoy, Gülseren, Özkan, & Bilen, Reference Ulusoy, Gülseren, Özkan and Bilen2020) or emotional experience (Carolan & Power, Reference Carolan and Power2011) and are more likely to feel emotions more intensely (Muhtadie, Johnson, Carver, Gotlib, & Ketter, Reference Muhtadie, Johnson, Carver, Gotlib and Ketter2014) compared to the general population. It appears that ED may be influenced by deficits in social cognition, like the ability to correctly perceive emotional information from others or the ability to make predictions about their attitudes or behaviors (Rowland et al., Reference Rowland, Hamilton, Vella, Lino, Mitchell and Green2013b). Emotional intelligence, a domain of social cognition, is also often impaired in people with BD who seem to have more difficulties in perceiving, using, understanding, and managing emotions (Varo et al., Reference Varo, Amoretti, Sparacino, Jiménez, Solé, del Mar Bonnin and Salagre2022). Emotion regulation (ER) is the ability of an individual to monitor, appraise, or adjust emotional reactions (Gross, Reference Gross2015) through the implementation of a range of strategies that can be labeled as adaptive (i.e. cognitive reframing, adaptive coping, and acceptance) or maladaptive (i.e. negative and positive rumination, negative focus, risk-taking behavior, suppression, and dampening) (Dodd, Lockwood, Mansell, & Palmier-Claus, Reference Dodd, Lockwood, Mansell and Palmier-Claus2019). ER requires complex interactions between cortical (i.e. prefrontal and orbitofrontal cortex) and subcortical regions (i.e. amygdala and hypothalamus) (Green & Malhi, Reference Green and Malhi2006) that appear to be altered in patients with BD (Bigot et al., Reference Bigot, Alonso, Houenou, Sarrazin, Dargél, Lledo and Henry2020). Two studies (Kjærstad et al., Reference Kjærstad, Damgaard, Knudsen, Vinberg, Kessing, Macoveanu and Miskowiak2022; Njau et al., Reference Njau, Townsend, Wade, Hellemann, Bookheimer, Narr and Brooks2020) reported a neurobiological heterogeneity in BD during the activation of these neural networks, defining two groups of patients. One of these groups showed higher amygdala activation, while the other exhibited a lower activation of both amygdala and prefrontal cortex during the experience of negative emotions. Depending on these clusters underlining different modalities of neuronal activity, people may preferably rely on specific ER strategies rather than others (Kjærstad et al., Reference Kjærstad, Damgaard, Knudsen, Vinberg, Kessing, Macoveanu and Miskowiak2022), which further contributes to the high heterogeneity of the construct. In our recent meta-analysis (De Prisco et al., Reference De Prisco, Oliva, Fico, Fornaro, de Bartolomeis, Serretti and Murru2022), individuals with BD were shown to adopt more maladaptive ER strategies and fewer adaptive ones than healthy controls and first-degree relatives, with the highest and strongest disparities found in ruminative and risk-taking behaviors. Whether these differences are characteristics of BD or are due to a common psychopathological burden shared by many psychiatric disorders requires further investigation. Indeed, studies focusing on differences in ED between BD and MDD reported mixed results, and higher levels of overall ED were described in both the former (Oymak Yenilmez et al., Reference Oymak Yenilmez, Atagün, Keleş Altun, Tunç, Uzgel, Altınbaş and Oral2021) and the latter (Becerra et al., Reference Becerra, Cruise, Murray, Bassett, Harms, Allan and Hood2013). As ED appears to be a transdiagnostic dimension and a potential target for personalized interventions, it is important to define the extent to which certain features are specific to BD or shared with other psychiatric disorders.

Given its complexity, several scales have been developed offering quick and useful clinical instruments to begin exploring this dimension, each one measuring specific aspects of ED. For example, following the operationalization proposed in a previous systematic review (Dodd et al., Reference Dodd, Lockwood, Mansell and Palmier-Claus2019), the Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (CERQ) (Garnefski & Kraaij, Reference Garnefski and Kraaij2007) is used to capture both maladaptive (i.e. rumination and negative focus) and adaptive (i.e. cognitive reframing and acceptance) ER strategies, the Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (ERQ) (Gross & John, Reference Gross and John2003) is employed to measure the levels of suppression and adaptive coping, while the Response Style Questionnaire (RSQ) (Nolen-Hoeksema, Reference Nolen-Hoeksema1991) is designed to measure the degree of negative rumination, risk-taking behavior, and adaptive coping. Other scales focus strictly on maladaptive strategies, such as the Ruminative Response Scale (RRS) (Treynor, Gonzalez, & Nolen-Hoeksema, Reference Treynor, Gonzalez and Nolen-Hoeksema2003) and the Response to Positive Affect (RPA) scale (Feldman, Joormann, & Johnson, Reference Feldman, Joormann and Johnson2008), which specifically assess negative rumination and positive rumination and dampening, respectively. Finally, the Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale (DERS) (Gratz & Roemer, Reference Gratz and Roemer2004) provides an overall assessment of ED while also allowing for a more targeted description of the difficulties in using specific strategies (i.e. risk-taking behavior, adaptive coping, and acceptance).

The present study aims to identify the ED characteristics and ER strategies that discriminate between individuals with BD from individuals diagnosed with any other psychiatric disorder when using these validated measurement scales.

Methods

The present systematic review and meta-analysis was conducted according to the Meta-analysis of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (MOOSE) (Stroup et al., Reference Stroup, Berlin, Morton, Olkin, Williamson, Rennie and Thacker2000) and the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) guidelines (Page et al., Reference Page, McKenzie, Bossuyt, Boutron, Hoffmann, Mulrow and Brennan2021). This systematic review and meta-analysis protocol was registered on the International Prospective Register of Systematic Reviews (PROSPERO) (https://www.crd.york.ac.uk/PROSPERO/; protocol CRD42021293357).

Eligibility criteria and study outcomes

Inclusion criteria were as follows: (i) original studies providing quantitative data on ER and/or ED, (ii) measured with a validated scale, (iii) in people diagnosed with BD, and (iv) compared with clinical groups (people diagnosed with psychiatric disorders other than BD). Psychiatric diagnoses had to be made according to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual for Mental Disorders (DSM) (APA, 1994, 2000, 2013) or the International Classification of Diseases (ICD) (WHO, 2004) diagnostic criteria. No sample size or language restrictions were applied. Both observational and interventional studies were eligible for inclusion, but we only considered baseline data. In the case of overlapping populations across multiple studies, we included the largest one with the most representative data relevant to our objectives. Exclusion criteria were as follows: (i) Studies that assessed ER and/or ED through behavioral measures or cognitive tasks of both explicit (e.g. affective picture viewing task and emotional film clips task) and implicit (e.g. affective go/no-go task and emotional stroop test) types; (ii) reviews; (iii) case reports and case series (not reliable control group); and (iv) studies conducted on animals (population not considered by our criteria).

Search strategy

We systematically searched the PubMed/MEDLINE, EMBASE, Scopus, and PsycINFO databases from inception until April 28th, 2022. Search strings are provided in online Supplementary Materials S1. The references of each included study, textbooks, and other material were hand-searched to identify potential additional studies not captured by the original search string.

Study selection and data extraction

Studies of potential interest were independently screened by two authors (M.D.P. and V.O.). A third author (A.M.) was consulted whenever a consensus could not be reached. Data extraction included (when available): author(s), publication year, geographical region and country, study design, diagnostic criteria, and (semi)structured interview adopted, setting of the study, type of validated scale, outcome type (primary or secondary), type of control group, number of cases and controls, mean and standard deviation (s.d.) of the outcome for cases and controls, mean age of cases and controls, % of females among cases and controls, duration of illness among people with BD, % of people diagnosed with BD-I, % of euthymic, depressed, or (hypo)manic patients, % of patients taking psychotropic medication, psychiatric or other medical comorbidities, and mean score obtained by cases and controls at symptom severity scales. WebPlotDigitizer was used to extract numerical variables from graphs whenever needed (https://automeris.io/WebPlotDigitizer/). In cases where the information was unavailable, we contacted the authors twice to request the necessary data.

Methodological quality appraisal

The risk of bias in the included studies was independently assessed by two authors (M.D.P. and V.O.), and any disagreement was resolved by a third author (A.M.). The Newcastle-Ottawa Scale (NOS) (Stang, Reference Stang2010) was adopted to grade the quality of observational studies, and the scores obtained at the NOS were converted to ‘Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality’ (AHRQ) standards as described elsewhere (Sharmin et al., Reference Sharmin, Kypri, Khanam, Wadolowski, Bruno and Mattick2017).

Statistical analyses

We performed the analyses using RStudio R version 4.1.2 (R Core Team, 2020) and conducted the meta-analysis through the metafor R-package (Viechtbauer & Viechtbauer, Reference Viechtbauer and Viechtbauer2015) using a random-effect model (restricted maximum-likelihood estimator) (Harville, Reference Harville1977). Effect sizes were calculated as standardized mean differences (s.m.d.) with their confidence intervals (CI) and represented by Hedge's g. We conducted sensitivity analyses by removing one study at a time from the analysis (leave-one-out analysis), by considering only good-quality studies according to AHRQ standards (high quality), and by including in the analyses only those studies that compared euthymic-only or depressed-only patients in both groups. Because we ran many statistical tests due to the large number of scales and subscales considered, we also conducted a sensitivity analysis using a more conservative significance level corrected for the number of total tests: this correction was done with the ‘p.adjust()’ function of the stats R-package (R Core Team, 2020) using the Bonferroni method. We performed cumulative analyses to evaluate the repercussions of the studies published over the years on the effect size. We assessed heterogeneity by using the Cochran's Q test (Cochran, Reference Cochran1950) and τ 2 and I 2 statistics (Higgins et al., Reference Higgins, Thomas, Chandler, Cumpston, Li, Page and Welch2019), and this was graphically evaluated by adopting the graphical display of study heterogeneity (GOSH) method (Olkin, Dahabreh, & Trikalinos, Reference Olkin, Dahabreh and Trikalinos2012); additionally, we estimated prediction intervals (Borenstein, Higgins, Hedges, & Rothstein, Reference Borenstein, Higgins, Hedges and Rothstein2017). When the Cochran's Q test presented a p < 0.05 and the I 2 statistic showed a value >50% and the analysis included more than three studies, we conducted a subgroup analysis according to a priori defined subgroups, whenever available (i.e. BD-type, the current mood-state, and the type of outcome according to the original study).

Results

A total of 3239 studies were identified across different sources, and after a semi-automatic duplicate removal, 1813 studies underwent further screening. Of these, 1667 were excluded at the title/abstract level, 112 after the full-text evaluation, and five could not be retrieved. Altogether, 29 studies were included in the present research, and 22 (yielding 145 comparisons) were eligible for performing a meta-analysis. Among the latter, 18 studies (100 comparisons) compared people diagnosed with BD to people diagnosed with MDD, while four studies (45 comparisons) compared patients with BD to patients with BPD. The PRISMA flowchart is shown in Fig. 1. Details on the studies included in the systematic review and meta-analysis are presented in Table 1 and in online Supplementary Materials S1. Details on the excluded studies are presented in online Supplementary Materials S1.

Figure 1. PRISMA flowchart, 2020 edition, adapted.

Table 1. Characteristics of the studies included in the systematic review and meta-analysis

Note: AAQ-II, Acceptance and Action Questionnaire, II; ACS, Affective Control Scale; ACS-1, anger; ACS-2, positive-affect; ACS-3, depression; ACS-4, anxiety; ADHD, attention deficit-hyperactivity disorder; BD, bipolar disorder; BDI, Beck Depression Inventory; BPD, borderline personality disorder; CERQ, Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire; CERQ-1, self-blame; CERQ-2, blaming others; CERQ-3, rumination; CERQ-4, catastrophizing; CERQ-5, putting into perspective; CERQ-6, positive refocus; CERQ-7, positive reappraisal; CERQ-8, acceptance; CERQ-9, focus on replanning; DASS, Depression Anxiety Stress Scales; DERS, Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale; DERS-1, nonacceptance; DERS-2, goals; DERS-3, impulse; DERS-4, awareness; DERS-5, strategies; DERS-6, clarity; DERS-TOT, total score; DIGS, diagnostic interview for genetic studies; DSM, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders; ERQ, Emotion Regulation Questionnaire; ERQ-1, reappraisal; ERQ-2, suppression; HDRS, Hamilton Depression Rating Scale; HC, healthy controls; IDS-C, Inventory of Depressive Symptomatology; LESS, Leahy Emotional Schema Scale; LESS-1, adaptive emotional schemas; LESS-2, rigid emotional schemas; LESS-3, negative beliefs about emotions; MADRS, Montgomery-Asberg Depression Rating Scale; MADRS, Montgomery–Åsberg Depression Rating Scale; MDD, major depressive disorder; MINI, The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview; MRI, magnetic resonance imaging; NOS, Newcastle-Ottawa Scale; PHQ-9, Patient Health Questionnaire-9; PTSD, post-traumatic stress disorder; QIDS-SR, Quick Inventory of Depressive Symptoms-Self Report; RIPoSt-40, Reactivity, Intensity, Polarity and Stability Questionnaire; RIPoSt-40-1, affective instability; RIPoSt-40-2, emotional impulsivity; RIPoSt-40-3, negative emotionality; RIPoSt-40-4, positive emotionality; RIPoSt-Y, Reactivity, Intensity, Polarity and Stability Questionnaire, Youth version; RIPoSt-Y-1, affective instability; RIPoSt-Y-2, positive emotionality; RIPoSt-Y-3, emotional reactivity; RIPoSt-Y-4, interpersonal sensitivity; RPA, response to positive affect; RPA-1, emotion focus; RPA-2, dampening; RPA-3, self-focus; RRS, Ruminative Response Scale; RRS-1, reflective pondering; RRS-2, brooming; RSQ, Response Styles Questionnaire; RSQ-1, rumination; RSQ-2, adaptive; RSQ-3, risk-taking; SADS, schedule for affective disorders and schizophrenia; SCID, Structured Clinical Interview for DSM Disorders; YMRS, Young Mania Rating Scale.

Main analyses

The main results of the meta-analyses conducted are presented in Table 2 and Fig. 2, organized according to overall ED measures, maladaptive ER strategies, and adaptive ER strategies, as in previous systematic reviews (De Prisco et al., Reference De Prisco, Oliva, Fico, Fornaro, de Bartolomeis, Serretti and Murru2022; Dodd et al., Reference Dodd, Lockwood, Mansell and Palmier-Claus2019). Overall, people with BD significantly differed from people with MDD in adopting more positive rumination and risk-taking behaviors. On the other hand, people with BPD relied less on adaptive ER strategies and exhibited higher levels of self-blaming and catastrophic thinking than people with BD.

Table 2. Meta-analyses results of emotion dysregulation between bipolar disorder and major depressive disorder or borderline personality disorder

Note: BD, Bipolar Disorder; BPD, Borderline Personality Disorder; CERQ, Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire; CI, confidence intervals; DERS, Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale; ERQ, Emotion Regulation Questionnaire; MDD, Major Depressive Disorder; PI, prediction intervals; RPA, response to positive affect; RRS, Rumination Response Scale; RSQ, Response Style Questionnaire; s.m.d., standardized mean difference (represented as Hedge's). Significant results are shown in bold and the exact p-value is given in the relative column. When the prediction intervals are the same as the confidence interval, the p-value is the same. In the two cases where the significant prediction intervals are different from the confidence intervals, their p-value is ≤ 0.01.

Figure 2. Differences in adopted emotion regulation strategies between people with bipolar disorder and people with major depressive disorder (left) and borderline personality disorder (right). Overall results of the comparisons included in the meta-analysis. Note: BD, ‘bipolar disorder’; BPD, ‘borderline personality disorder’; CERQ, ‘cognitive emotion regulation scale’; CERQ-1, ‘self-blame’; CERQ-2, ‘blaming others’; CERQ-3, ‘rumination’; CERQ-4, ‘catastrophizing’; CERQ-5, ‘putting into perspective’; CERQ-6, ‘positive refocusing’; CERQ-7, ‘positive reappraisal’; CERQ-8, ‘acceptance’; CERQ-9, ‘focus on replanning’; DERS, ‘difficulties in emotion regulation scale’; DERS-1, ‘nonacceptance’; DERS-2, ‘goals’; DERS-3, ‘impulse’; DERS-4, ‘awareness’; DERS-5, ‘strategies’; DERS-6, ‘clarity’; DERS-TOT, ‘total score’; ERQ, ‘emotion regulation questionnaire’; ERQ-1, ‘reappraisal’; ERQ-2, ‘suppression’; MDD, ‘major depressive disorder’; RPA, ‘response to positive affect’; RPA-1, ‘emotion focus’; RPA-2, ‘dampening’; RPA-3, ‘self-focus’; RRS, ‘ruminative response scale’; RRS-1, ‘reflective pondering’; RRS-2, ‘brooming’; RSQ-, ‘response style questionnaire’; RSQ-1, ‘rumination’; RSQ-2, ‘adaptive’; RSQ-3, ‘risk-taking’.

*These items' effect sizes (ES) have been inverted to present graphically coherent results since the scale initially measures the individual's difficulties in adopting that emotion regulation strategy.

The point size is proportional to the total sample size for that particular comparison. The number in parentheses indicates the number of studies considered for that particular comparison.

Additional details on the main analyses are provided in online Supplementary Materials S1.

Subgroup analyses

Subgroup analyses were conducted only for comparisons with high heterogeneity and according to (i) the BD-type; (ii) the current mood-state; and (iii) the type of outcome according to the original study.

Studies comparing BD and MDD and exploring differences in the total DERS score as a primary outcome presented a significant and opposite effect size with reduced heterogeneity (test for subgroup difference: Q M = 7.26; p value = 0.01).

Studies comparing BD and BPD and exploring differences in the ‘clarity’ subscale of the DERS as a primary outcome presented a significantly lower effect size and reduced heterogeneity (test for subgroup difference: Q M = 5.59; p value = 0.02).

No significant differences were observed according to the BD-type or current mood state.

Additional details on the subgroup analyses are provided in online Supplementary Materials S1.

Sensitivity analyses

Sensitivity analyses were conducted (i) removing one study at a time; (ii) considering good-quality studies only; (iii) considering only those studies that compared euthymic-only or depressed-only patients in both groups; (iv) considering a more conservative significance level. GOSH plots were graphically inspected to further evaluate outliers and heterogeneity.

In studies comparing BD and MDD, the following comparisons became significant after the leave-one-out sensitivity analysis: (i) ‘reflective pondering’ subscale of the RRS, by removing the study (Aslan & Baldwin, Reference Aslan and Baldwin2021), (ii) ‘putting into perspective’ subscale of the CERQ, by removing the study (Wolkenstein, Zwick, Hautzinger, & Joormann, Reference Wolkenstein, Zwick, Hautzinger and Joormann2014), and (iii) ‘goals’ subscale of the DERS, by removing the study (Oymak Yenilmez et al., Reference Oymak Yenilmez, Atagün, Keleş Altun, Tunç, Uzgel, Altınbaş and Oral2021), all passing from a small to a small-to-moderate effect size; (iv) ‘awareness’ subscale of the DERS, by removing the study (Musket, Hansen, Welker, Gilbert, & Gruber, Reference Musket, Hansen, Welker, Gilbert and Gruber2021), passing from a small-to-moderate to a moderate effect size. On the contrary, by removing the studies ([Becerra et al., Reference Becerra, Cruise, Murray, Bassett, Harms, Allan and Hood2013], [Becerra, Bassett, & Harms, Reference Becerra, Bassett and Harms2016], or [Weintraub, Van de Loo, Gitlin, & Miklowitz, Reference Weintraub, Van de Loo, Gitlin and Miklowitz2017]) in the comparison relative to the ‘clarity’ subscale of the DERS, the overall effect became nonsignificant with an effect size comparable in magnitude.

In studies comparing BD and BPD and exploring differences of the ‘clarity’ subscale of the DERS, the overall effect became insignificant by removing the study (Bayes, Parker, & McClure, Reference Bayes, Parker and McClure2016), with an effect size comparable in magnitude.

In good-quality studies comparing BD and MDD and exploring differences of the ‘clarity’ subscale of the DERS, the overall effect became nonsignificant. In good-quality studies comparing BD and MDD and exploring differences of the ‘reflective pondering’ subscale of the RRS, the overall effect became significant.

In studies comparing BD and MDD and exploring differences in the ‘clarity’ subscale of the DERS and by solely considering only those studies that compared euthymic-only or depressed-only patients in both groups, the overall effect was not significant.

In studies comparing BD and MDD, differences found in the ‘clarity’ subscale of the DERS were no longer significant when using a more conservative significance level.

In studies comparing BD and BPD, differences found in the ‘positive refocusing’, ‘positive reappraisal’, and ‘refocus on planning’ subscales of the CERQ and ‘clarity’ subscale of the DERS were no longer significant when using a more conservative significance level.

Inspecting the GOSH plots in studies comparing BD and MDD, the following results were found (each referring to the comparisons related to each mentioned subscale): (i) in the ‘putting into perspective’ subscale of the CERQ, subsets including the study (Wolkenstein et al., Reference Wolkenstein, Zwick, Hautzinger and Joormann2014) presented higher heterogeneity and lower effect size; (ii) in the ‘positive refocusing’ subscale of the CERQ, subsets including the study (Ariana Kia & Hasani, Reference Ariana Kia and Hasani2014) presented higher heterogeneity and effect size; (iii) in the ‘refocus on planning’ subscale of the CERQ, subsets including the study (Kjærstad et al., Reference Kjærstad, Vinberg, Goldin, Køster, Støttrup, Knorr and Miskowiak2016) presented higher heterogeneity and effect size; (iv) in the ‘goals’ subscale of the DERS, subsets including the study (Oymak Yenilmez et al., Reference Oymak Yenilmez, Atagün, Keleş Altun, Tunç, Uzgel, Altınbaş and Oral2021) presented higher heterogeneity and opposite effect size; (v) in the ‘non acceptance’ subscale of the DERS, subsets including the study (Oymak Yenilmez et al., Reference Oymak Yenilmez, Atagün, Keleş Altun, Tunç, Uzgel, Altınbaş and Oral2021) presented higher heterogeneity and opposite effect size; and (vi) in the ‘awareness’ subscale of the DERS, subsets including the study (Musket et al., Reference Musket, Hansen, Welker, Gilbert and Gruber2021) presented higher heterogeneity and lower effects size.

By inspecting the GOSH plots in the studies comparing BD and BPD and exploring differences in the total DERS score, subsets including the study (Das, Calhoun, & Malhi, Reference Das, Calhoun and Malhi2014) presented higher heterogeneity and effect size.

Additional details on the sensitivity analyses and the GOSH plots are provided in online Supplementary Materials S1.

Characteristics of the studies included in the qualitative synthesis

The following studies reported information about the comparison of BD with other psychiatric conditions; however, it was not possible to perform a meta-analysis on these data. One study (Rowland et al., Reference Rowland, Hamilton, Lino, Ly, Denny, Hwang and Green2013a) compared BD with schizophrenia using the CERQ. Three studies (Kim, Kim, & Kim, Reference Kim, Kim and Kim2022; Masi et al., Reference Masi, Sesso, Pfanner, Valente, Molesti, Placini and Montesanto2021; Sesso et al., Reference Sesso, Milone, Drago, Viglione, Berloffa, Boldrini and Placini2021) compared BD with ADHD using the ERQ (Kim et al., Reference Kim, Kim and Kim2022), the Reactivity, Intensity, Polarity and Stability questionnaire (RIPoSt-40) (Masi et al., Reference Masi, Sesso, Pfanner, Valente, Molesti, Placini and Montesanto2021), or the RIPoSt youth version (Sesso et al., Reference Sesso, Milone, Drago, Viglione, Berloffa, Boldrini and Placini2021). Two studies (Becerra et al., Reference Becerra, Cruise, Murray, Bassett, Harms, Allan and Hood2013; Kim, Yu, Lee, & Kim, Reference Kim, Yu, Lee and Kim2012) compared BD with anxiety disorders adopting the DERS (Becerra et al., Reference Becerra, Cruise, Murray, Bassett, Harms, Allan and Hood2013) or the RRS (Kim et al., Reference Kim, Yu, Lee and Kim2012). Finally, one study (Kim et al., Reference Kim, Yu, Lee and Kim2012) compared BD with post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) using the ERQ (Kim et al., Reference Kim, Kim and Kim2022).

Detailed characteristics of these studies are provided in Table 1 and in online Supplementary Materials S1.

Discussion

The present systematic review and meta-analysis aimed at exploring the differences in terms of adopted ER strategies and ED characteristics among people diagnosed with BD compared with people diagnosed with other psychiatric disorders. Overall, BD patients were quite similar to patients with MDD in both overall ED and adopted ER strategies, although they showed more positive rumination and risk-taking behaviors. Patients with BD exhibited a lower degree of ED when compared with BPD patients, as they adopted more adaptive ER strategies and fewer maladaptive ones.

Specifically, no significant differences were found in almost all BD-MDD comparisons. Previous studies focusing on neuroimaging correlates of ER and ED found that patients with MDD compared to healthy controls presented altered connections between the amygdala and both lateral (Johnstone, Van Reekum, Urry, Kalin, & Davidson, Reference Johnstone, Van Reekum, Urry, Kalin and Davidson2007) and medial (Perlman et al., Reference Perlman, Simmons, Wu, Hahn, Tapert, Max and Campbell-Sills2012) prefrontal cortex during reappraisal, and similar changes were also outlined in studies comparing BD patients with healthy subjects (Bigot et al., Reference Bigot, Alonso, Houenou, Sarrazin, Dargél, Lledo and Henry2020; Townsend & Altshuler, Reference Townsend and Altshuler2012). However, when people diagnosed with BD and MDD were directly compared, they displayed significant differences in activating those brain areas during ER tasks (Rai et al., Reference Rai, Griffiths, Breukelaar, Barreiros, Chen, Boyce and Bryant2021; Rive et al., Reference Rive, Mocking, Koeter, van Wingen, de Wit, van den Heuvel and Schene2015). Taking these data together, the overall impaired connections found in these patients could support the shared difficulties in ER, while the dissimilarities might point toward psychopathological differences that are too subtle to be detected by self-administered tests used in the included studies and also a misdiagnosis of BD as MDD appears to be frequent (Daveney, Panagioti, Waheed, & Esmail, Reference Daveney, Panagioti, Waheed and Esmail2019), so diagnostic biases might dilute a possibly existing difference in terms of ED. Although most of the included studies supported their diagnoses with the adoption of structured or semistructured interview tools, BD patients may be very aware of their depressive symptoms while ignoring or minimizing hypomanic ones (Vieta et al., Reference Vieta, Berk, Schulze, Carvalho, Suppes, Calabrese and Grande2018) or experience several depressive episodes before the onset of mania or hypomania (O'Donovan & Alda, Reference O'Donovan and Alda2020), impeding the proper diagnostic orientation. Moreover, the high heterogeneity observed in some CERQ-related comparisons and in all DERS-related comparisons, which is not explained by consideration of the BD type or affective state, could also be related to other aspects interacting with ER. One study (Varo et al., Reference Varo, Kjærstad, Poulsen, Meluken, Vieta, Kessing and Miskowiak2021) explored the heterogeneity of emotional cognition in a sample of people diagnosed with BD or MDD, finding that people with mood disorders can be grouped into different clusters based on their emotional reactivity, adopted ER strategies, or correct recognition of facial expressions. Due to the unavailability of this information, it has not been possible to know how this result may have affected our population. Nevertheless, additional features beyond those related to the diagnosis alone are expected to help us better understand the phenomenon.

In our meta-analysis, patients with BD significantly differed from patients with MDD regarding positive rumination, as confirmed by the small effect sizes of the comparisons of the ‘emotion focus’ and ‘self-focus’ subscales of the RPA, and this difference remained when analyzing the prediction intervals. BD is often linked to increased expression of positive emotions in response to or in expectation of positive or rewarding stimuli (Gruber, Reference Gruber2011). The persistence of positive emotions may be related to high-risk and impulsive behavior in this population (Muhtadie et al., Reference Muhtadie, Johnson, Carver, Gotlib and Ketter2014). This type of conduct is often later associated with feelings of regret (Rydahl et al., Reference Rydahl, Brund, Medici, Straarup, Straszek and Østergaard2022), and for this reason, we expected that people with BD could have been more involved in dampening their positive emotions when compared with MDD patients. However, there was no difference in the ‘dampening’ subscale of the RPA between these groups, even when looking at the sensitivity analyses. Still, specific features of this subscale may partially explain this finding. Some of the items of the RPA contributing to the ‘dampening’ subscale score (e.g. ‘Thinking: My streak of luck is going to end soon’ or ‘Thinking: I don't deserve this’) (Feldman et al., Reference Feldman, Joormann and Johnson2008) may be related to pessimistic thinking, which is a core MDD characteristic (Katayama et al., Reference Katayama, Nakagawa, Umeda, Terasawa, Kurata, Tabuchi and Mimura2019), contributing to overall high scores in this population rather than the need to soothe positive emotions.

Patients with BD and MDD did not significantly differ in ruminating on negative emotions. Considering the comparison relative to the ‘reflective pondering’ subscale of the RRS, significance was achieved after removing one study (Aslan & Baldwin, Reference Aslan and Baldwin2021) that included only currently depressed patients. In contrast, other studies (Kim et al., Reference Kim, Yu, Lee and Kim2012; Shapero et al., Reference Shapero, Stange, Goldstein, Black, Molz, Hamlat and Alloy2015) included both symptomatic and euthymic patients in their sample, suggesting that the current affective symptomatology may influence the differences in terms of negative rumination. In addition, acutely depressed patients with MDD scored significantly higher than acutely depressed BD patients when looking at more absorbing and negative emotion-focused form of ruminations, although this comparison was not significant after conducting a sensitivity analysis. A study comparing MDD patients presenting different levels of rumination (Tsuchiyagaito et al., Reference Tsuchiyagaito, Sánchez, Misaki, Kuplicki, Park, Paulus and Guinjoan2022) observed that higher degrees of rumination were associated with greater connectivity between brain areas dedicated to language processing and areas involved in ER: the heterogeneity found at the level of brain connectivity may further influence the differences observed in our analyses.

BD and MDD patients also significantly differed at the ‘risk-taking’ subscale of the RSQ. BD is related to higher risk-taking behavior (Hıdıroğlu et al., Reference Hıdıroğlu, Esen, Tunca, Yalçìn, Lombardo, Glahn and Özerdem2013), which may be followed by dramatic social and health consequences, especially in the case of comorbid substance abuse (Meade, Graff, Griffin, & Weiss, Reference Meade, Graff, Griffin and Weiss2008). No differences were found relative to the ‘impulse’ subscale of the DERS. Most of the participants included in these studies were euthymic, and our findings corroborate with existing literature that failed to discriminate euthymic people with BD and MDD based on their trait-impulsivity (Henna et al., Reference Henna, Hatch, Nicoletti, Swann, Zunta-Soares and Soares2013; Ozten & Erol, Reference Ozten and Erol2019). This finding remained even when a subgroup analysis based on mood states was conducted, but more studies focused on acutely ill patients are lacking.

In this meta-analysis, BD patients presented a significantly lower level of overall difficulties in ER compared to BPD patients. Among maladaptive ER strategies, they significantly differ from patients with BPD in negative focus, scoring lower at the ‘self-blame’ and ‘catastrophizing’ subscales of the CERQ. Usually, patients with BPD display monocausal reasoning when explaining positive or negative situations, with the tendency to catastrophize as they only consider a little explanation (Schilling, Moritz, Schneider, Bierbrodt, & Nagel, Reference Schilling, Moritz, Schneider, Bierbrodt and Nagel2015). Additionally, self-blame seems to be a core feature of BPD (Sorgi-Wilson & McCloskey, Reference Sorgi-Wilson and McCloskey2022); it could represent a significant clinical target due to its role in internalizing symptoms like anxiety, depression, or emotional withdrawal (Tanzer, Salaminios, Morosan, Campbell, & Debbané, Reference Tanzer, Salaminios, Morosan, Campbell and Debbané2021). Despite these elements' importance in BD, their predominance in BPD may be suggested by different clinical characteristics. Indeed, unlike BD, BPD presents a more continuous and trait-like course, a lower age at onset, and fewer available treatment strategies (Leichsenring, Leibing, Kruse, New, & Leweke, Reference Leichsenring, Leibing, Kruse, New and Leweke2011), which may contribute to the more pervasive adoption of maladaptive ER strategies. For the same reasons, these groups may differ in their impulsive response to negative emotions. This finding is in line with previous reports underlining BPD's higher broad and trait-impulsiveness compared with BD patients (Bøen et al., Reference Bøen, Hummelen, Elvsåshagen, Boye, Andersson, Karterud and Malt2015).

No differences were found in the ‘rumination’ subscale of the CERQ. Since negative rumination can be defined as a form of thinking that involves excessive and repetitive negative thoughts or themes that interfere with other forms of mental activity, several types of ruminations may be considered depending on their object (e.g. depression, worry, and anger). However, the items composing the ‘rumination’ subscale of the CERQ may be too general and lacking in specificity, failing to capture the various aspects and types of rumination. Therefore, although both BD and BPD patients may share the same propensity to ruminate, they may differ in their content, considering the association of depressive and anger rumination with borderline personality features (Baer & Sauer, Reference Baer and Sauer2011).

Interestingly, in our meta-analysis, people with BD rely on more adaptive ER strategies than BPD patients. Adopting specific ER strategies is possibly influenced by the family environment and the relationships experienced during childhood (Thompson, Reference Thompson2019). Importantly, early-life abuse and childhood maltreatment are associated with higher levels of ED (Gruhn & Compas, Reference Gruhn and Compas2020). Childhood abuse is commonly reported by patients with BD (Fisher & Hosang, Reference Fisher and Hosang2010), but rates of emotional, physical, or sexual trauma seem higher in the BPD population (Merza, Papp, & Kuritárné Szabó, Reference Merza, Papp and Kuritárné Szabó2015; Temes et al., Reference Temes, Magni, Fitzmaurice, Aguirre, Goodman and Zanarini2017), suggesting that previously learned strategies, usefully adopted in response to a real or perceived threat, turned into dysfunctional or inappropriate coping mechanisms. However, some of these observed differences are not significant using a more conservative significance level, suggesting the need for further investigation in this direction.

It is important to note that all studies included in the meta-analyses adopted structured or semistructured interview tools to confirm their diagnosis and exclude comorbid cases. However, the prevalence of BPD in BD is reported to be around 21%, rising up to 37% in BD type II (Fornaro et al., Reference Fornaro, Orsolini, Marini, De Berardis, Perna, Valchera and Stubbs2016), and real-world patients may have more severe ED characteristics than those described in this systematic review and meta-analysis, as suggested by studies specifically focusing on this population (Bayes et al., Reference Bayes, Parker and McClure2016). Hence, the clinical question on the intensity of ED in BD comorbid to BPD requires additional research.

A quantitative approach was not viable to study the differences in ER-ED between BD and other psychiatric conditions. The higher scores at the ‘blaming the others’ subscale of the CERQ reported by patients diagnosed with schizophrenia may be due to the higher rate of self-serving bias or broader social cognition deficits described in this population (Müller, Betz, & Bechdolf, Reference Müller, Betz and Bechdolf2021). People with BD compared to young patients with ADHD showed higher levels of ED relative to negative emotions. Despite no available evidence allowing us to explore this comparison in adult life, varying clinical course and high rates of psychiatric comorbidities (Franke et al., Reference Franke, Michelini, Asherson, Banaschewski, Bilbow, Buitelaar and Haavik2018) could worsen the intensity and presentation of ED in ADHD patients. Studies comparing BD to anxiety disorders reported contrasting results and this may be due to the broad diagnostic category considered. Anxiety disorders are in fact conditions with widely heterogeneous clinical presentations and severity, so future studies should consider these entities separately to assess ED.

To the best of our knowledge, this is the first systematic review and meta-analysis focusing on different aspects of ED in BD compared with other psychiatric disorders. In this meta-analysis, patients with BD displayed fewer differences than those found in our previous work involving healthy controls and first-degree relatives (De Prisco et al., Reference De Prisco, Oliva, Fico, Fornaro, de Bartolomeis, Serretti and Murru2022). ED is a transdiagnostic construct, present throughout different disorders, albeit with different pervasiveness. Our results can be useful in outlining features that are particularly relevant for understanding BD, although the extent of divergences between conditions is smaller. As suggested for mood instability (Kessing & Faurholt-Jepsen, Reference Kessing and Faurholt-Jepsen2022), ED assessment in clinical trials may be more valuable than conventional outcomes in estimating disease severity or patient functioning. In general, addressing ED in clinical practice can be helpful in accelerating the transition from categorical to quantitative nosology. In fact, this dimension lies above the classical diagnostic categories and could guide future genetic research better than the latter (Kotov et al., Reference Kotov, Krueger, Watson, Cicero, Conway, DeYoung and Latzman2021) as well as to assist the development of treatments aimed at these transdiagnostic features (Capitão et al., Reference Capitão, Chapman, Filippini, Wright, Murphy, James and Harmer2022; Solmi et al., Reference Solmi, Bodini, Cocozza, Seeman, Vieta, Dragioti and Fusar-Poli2020).

The present study has some limitations. First, the available evidence precluded us from making meta-analytic comparisons with other psychiatric disorders like schizophrenia, ADHD, anxiety disorders, or PTSD, limiting the understanding of differential ED features between BD and those conditions. As described in other ED-related constructs (Miskowiak & Varo, Reference Miskowiak and Varo2021), there is no specific consensus on which tools are more appropriate to assess ED, with the risk that the same aspects are measured by different tools, making a meta-analytic approach difficult. Second, the paucity of studies did not permit us to perform meta-regressions to study the impact of continuous variables on the overall effect size or to explore publication bias. Meta-regressions on affective symptom severity scales would have given us a better understanding of their role on different effect sizes, especially considering that in some studies the two groups appear to differ significantly, although often within a similar range of severity. Third, precise data about medical and psychiatric comorbidities or psychopharmacological treatments were seldom reported, limiting our understanding of their influence on ED. Additionally, we did not have data on patients' previous psychotherapy experience or whether they suffered from subthreshold symptomatology that is concerning, given their role on ED (Crockett, Martínez, & Jiménez-Molina, Reference Crockett, Martínez and Jiménez-Molina2020; Sloan et al., Reference Sloan, Hall, Moulding, Bryce, Mildred and Staiger2017). Fourth, sample sizes were small in general, and only a few studies contributed to any comparison, suggesting the need for further studies on the matter. Fifth, most of the included studies did not provide sufficient information on sample recruitment or the statistical methods used to calculate sample size, and the study populations were not always adequately matched. As a result, less than a third of them were considered to be of good quality, limiting the strength of the observations made in this review. Sixth, the exclusion from this review of studies assessing ED (or related aspects) through behavioral measures limits the generalizability of our findings. Although our goal was to focus on instruments that are quick to administer and easy to use in clinical settings, the inclusion of tasks designed to explore explicit or implicit emotional regulation may have allowed for the identification of more subtle differences in the populations studied. Seventh, some studies included populations without specifying their affective status or enrolled patients at different phases of illness. In the latter cases, however, most of the patients in the two groups belonged to the same affective polarity.

In conclusion, ED is present in different degrees among psychiatric disorders. This construct appears as a transdiagnostic feature, and small to moderate differences were found when comparing BD with other conditions. Patients diagnosed with BD did not differ from those diagnosed with MDD in adopting many of the adaptive and maladaptive ER strategies. However, positive rumination and risk-taking behaviors were more common in the former. Conversely, people with BPD displayed a higher degree of ED compared with patients diagnosed with BD. This suggests a potential clue to clinically discriminate between these two populations and a potential treatment target.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S003329172300243X

Acknowledgements

G.F. received the support of a fellowship from ‘La Caixa’ Foundation (ID 100010434 – fellowship code LCF/BQ/DR21/11880019). J.R. acknowledges the support of the Spanish Ministry of Science and Innovation (PI19/00394, CPII19/00009) integrated into the Plan Nacional de I + D + I and co-financed by the ISCIII-Subdirección General de Evaluación and the Fondo Europeo de Desarrollo Regional (FEDER) and the Instituto de Salud Carlos III. I.G. acknowledges the support of the Spanish Ministry of Science and Innovation (MCIN) (PI19/00954) integrated into the Plan Nacional de I + D + I and cofinanced by the ISCIII-Subdirección General de Evaluación y el Fondos Europeos de la Unión Europea (FEDER, FSE, Next Generation EU/Plan de Recuperación Transformación y Resiliencia_PRTR); the Instituto de Salud Carlos III; the CIBER of Mental Health (CIBERSAM); and the the Secretaria d'Universitats i Recerca del Departament d'Economia i Coneixement 2021-SGR-01358), CERCA Programme/Generalitat de Catalunya as well as the Fundació Clínic per la Recerca Biomèdica (Pons Bartran 2022-FRCB_PB1_2022). G.A. was supported by a Rio Hortega 2021 Grant (CM21/00017) from the Spanish Ministry of Health financed by the Instituto de Salud Carlos III (ISCIII) and co-financed by the Fondo Social Europeo Plus (FSE+). DHM's research was supported by a Juan Rodés JR18/00021 granted by the Instituto de Salud Carlos III (ISCIII). E.V. acknowledges the support of the Spanish Ministry of Science and Innovation (PI18/00805, PI21/00787) integrated into the Plan Nacional de I + D + I and co-financed by the ISCIII-Subdirección General de Evaluación and the Fondo Europeo de Desarrollo Regional (FEDER); the Instituto de Salud Carlos III; the CIBER of Mental Health (CIBERSAM); the Secretaria d'Universitats i Recerca del Departament d'Economia i Coneixement (2021-SGR-01358), the CERCA Programme, and the Departament de Salut de la Generalitat de Catalunya for the PERIS Grant SLT006/17/00357. Thanks the support of the European Union Horizon 2020 research and innovation program (EU.3.1.1. Understanding health, wellbeing and disease: Grant No. 754907 and EU.3.1.3. Treating and managing disease: Grant No. 945151). A.M. acknowledges the support of the Spanish Ministry of Science and Innovation (PI19/00672) integrated into the Plan Nacional de I + D + I and co-financed by the ISCIII-Subdirección General de Evaluación and the Fondo Europeo de Desarrollo Regional (FEDER). All authors are grateful to Derek Clougher for carefully reviewing the language throughout the text.

Funding statement

This research received no specific grant from any funding agency, commercial or not-for-profit sectors, other than those mentioned in the acknowledgements.

Competing interests

G.F. has received CME-related honoraria or consulting fees from Angelini, Janssen-Cilag, and Lundbeck; I.G. has received grants and served as a consultant, advisor, or CME speaker for the following identities: ADAMED, Angelini, Casen Recordati, Ferrer, Janssen Cilag, and Lundbeck, Lundbeck-Otsuka, Luye, SEI Healthcare outside the submitted work;

G.A. has received CME-related honoraria or consulting fees from Janssen-Cilag, Lundbeck, Lundbeck/Otsuka, and Angelini, with no financial or other relationship relevant to the subject of this article.

A.d.B. has received research support from Janssen, Lundbeck, and Otsuka and lecture fees for educational meeting from Chiesi, Lundbeck, Roche, Sunovion, Vitria, Recordati, Angelini, and Takeda; he has served on advisory boards for Eli Lilly, Jansen, Lundbeck, Otsuka, Roche, and Takeda, Chiesi, Recordati, Angelini, and Vitria; A.S. is or has been a consultant/speaker for Abbott, Abbvie, Angelini, AstraZeneca, Clinical Data, Boehringer, Bristol-Myers Squibb, Eli Lilly, GlaxoSmithKline, Innovapharma, Italfarmaco, Janssen, Lundbeck, Naurex, Pfizer, Polifarma, Sanofi, Servier, and Taliaz; E.V. has received grants and served as a consultant, advisor, or CME speaker for the following entities: AB-Biotics, AbbVie, Angelini, Biogen, Boehringer-Ingelheim, Celon Pharma, Dainippon Sumitomo Pharma, Ethypharm, Ferrer, Gedeon Richter, GH Research, Glaxo-Smith Kline, Janssen, Lundbeck, Medincell, Novartis, Orion Corporation, Organon, Otsuka, Rovi, Sage, Sanofi-Aventis, Sunovion, Takeda, and Viatris, outside the submitted work; A.M. has received grants and served as a consultant, advisor, or CME speaker for the following entities: Angelini, Idorsia, Lundbeck, Pfizer, and Takeda, outside of the submitted work. All the other authors have no conflict to declare.

Open practices statement

The datasets and the codes used for this research are available on request.

References

Aldao, A., Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Schweizer, S. (2010). Emotion-regulation strategies across psychopathology: A meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 30(2), 217237. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2009.11.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ambrosi, E., Arciniegas, D. B., Madan, A., Curtis, K. N., Patriquin, M. A., Jorge, R. E., … Salas, R. (2017). Insula and amygdala resting-state functional connectivity differentiate bipolar from unipolar depression. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 136(1), 129139. doi: 10.1111/acps.12724CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
APA. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Arlington, VA, US: American Psychiatric Publishing, Inc.Google Scholar
APA. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed., text rev.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Publishing, Inc.Google Scholar
APA. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Washington, D.C.: American Psychiatric Publishing, Inc.Google Scholar
Ariana Kia, E., & Hasani, J. (2014). Impulsivity and cognitive emotion regulation strategies in patients with bipolar- and major depressive disorders. Advances in Cognitive Science, 16(2), 110. http://icssjournal.ir/article-1-225-en.html.Google Scholar
Aslan, I. H., & Baldwin, D. S. (2021). Ruminations and their correlates in depressive episodes: Between-group comparison in patients with unipolar or bipolar depression and healthy controls. Journal of Affective Disorders, 280(Pt A), 16. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2020.10.064CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baer, R. A., & Sauer, S. E. (2011). Relationships between depressive rumination, anger rumination, and borderline personality features. Personality Disorders: Theory, Research, and Treatment, 2(2), 142. doi: 10.1037/a0019478CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Batmaz, S., Kaymak, S. U., Kocbiyik, S., & Turkcapar, M. H. (2014). Metacognitions and emotional schemas: A new cognitive perspective for the distinction between unipolar and bipolar depression. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 55(7), 15461555. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2014.05.016CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bayes, A., Parker, G., & McClure, G. (2016). Emotional dysregulation in those with bipolar disorder, borderline personality disorder and their comorbid expression. Journal of Affective Disorders, 204, 103111. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2016.06.027CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beauchaine, T. P., & Cicchetti, D. (2019). Emotion dysregulation and emerging psychopathology: A transdiagnostic, transdisciplinary perspective. Development and Psychopathology, 31(3), 799804. doi: 10.1017/S0954579419000671CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Becerra, R., Bassett, D., & Harms, C. (2016). Emotion regulation in bipolar disorder: Self-report profiles and effects of psychotropic medication. Clinical Neuropsychiatry: Journal of Treatment Evaluation, 13(4), 5967. https://ro.ecu.edu.au/ecuworkspost2013/5399/.Google Scholar
Becerra, R., Cruise, K., Murray, G., Bassett, D., Harms, C., Allan, A., & Hood, S. (2013). Emotion regulation in bipolar disorder: Are emotion regulation abilities less compromised in euthymic bipolar disorder than unipolar depressive or anxiety disorders? Open Journal of Psychiatry, 03(04), 17. doi: 10.4236/ojpsych.2013.34A001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bigot, M., Alonso, M., Houenou, J., Sarrazin, S., Dargél, A. A., Lledo, P.-M., & Henry, C. (2020). An emotional-response model of bipolar disorders integrating recent findings on amygdala circuits. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 118, 358366. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.07.037CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bøen, E., Hummelen, B., Elvsåshagen, T., Boye, B., Andersson, S., Karterud, S., & Malt, U. F. (2015). Different impulsivity profiles in borderline personality disorder and bipolar II disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders, 170, 104111. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2014.08.033CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Borenstein, M., Higgins, J. P., Hedges, L. V., & Rothstein, H. R. (2017). Basics of meta-analysis: I2 is not an absolute measure of heterogeneity. Research Synthesis Methods, 8(1), 518. doi: 10.1002/jrsm.1230CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Capitão, L. P., Chapman, R., Filippini, N., Wright, L., Murphy, S. E., James, A., … Harmer, C. J. (2022). Acute neural effects of fluoxetine on emotional regulation in depressed adolescents. Psychological Medicine, 53, 112. doi: 10.1017/S0033291722001805.Google ScholarPubMed
Carolan, L. A., & Power, M. J. (2011). What basic emotions are experienced in bipolar disorder? Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy, 18(5), 366378. doi: 10.1002/cpp.777CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cochran, W. G. (1950). The comparison of percentages in matched samples. Biometrika, 37(3/4), 256266. Retrieved from https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/14801052CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cole, P. M., Ramsook, K. A., & Ram, N. (2019). Emotion dysregulation as a dynamic process. Development and Psychopathology, 31(3), 11911201. doi: 10.1017/S0954579419000695CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crockett, M. A., Martínez, V., & Jiménez-Molina, Á. (2020). Subthreshold depression in adolescence: Gender differences in prevalence, clinical features, and associated factors. Journal of Affective Disorders, 272, 269276. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2020.03.111CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
D'Agostino, A., Covanti, S., Monti, M. R., & Starcevic, V. (2017). Reconsidering emotion dysregulation. Psychiatric Quarterly, 88(4), 807825. doi: 10.1007/s11126-017-9499-6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Das, P., Calhoun, V., & Malhi, G. S. (2014). Bipolar and borderline patients display differential patterns of functional connectivity among resting state networks. NeuroImage, 98, 7381. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.04.062CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Daveney, J., Panagioti, M., Waheed, W., & Esmail, A. (2019). Unrecognized bipolar disorder in patients with depression managed in primary care: A systematic review and meta-analysis. General Hospital Psychiatry, 58, 7176. doi: 10.1016/j.genhosppsych.2019.03.006CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Prisco, M., Oliva, V., Fico, G., Fornaro, M., de Bartolomeis, A., Serretti, A., … Murru, A. (2022). Defining clinical characteristics of emotion dysregulation in bipolar disorder: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 142, 104914. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2022.104914CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dodd, A., Lockwood, E., Mansell, W., & Palmier-Claus, J. (2019). Emotion regulation strategies in bipolar disorder: A systematic and critical review. Journal of Affective Disorders, 246, 262284. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2018.12.026CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Feldman, G. C., Joormann, J., & Johnson, S. L. (2008). Responses to positive affect: A self-report measure of rumination and dampening. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 32(4), 507525. doi: 10.1007/s10608-006-9083-0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fisher, H. L., & Hosang, G. M. (2010). Childhood maltreatment and bipolar disorder: A critical review of the evidence. Mind & Brain, The Journal of Psychiatry, 1(1), 7585.Google Scholar
Fletcher, K., Parker, G., Bayes, A., Paterson, A., & McClure, G. (2014). Emotion regulation strategies in bipolar II disorder and borderline personality disorder: Differences and relationships with perceived parental style. Journal of Affective Disorders, 157, 5259. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2014.01.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fletcher, K., Parker, G. B., & Manicavasagar, V. (2013). Coping profiles in bipolar disorder. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 54(8), 11771184. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2013.05.011CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fornaro, M., Orsolini, L., Marini, S., De Berardis, D., Perna, G., Valchera, A., … Stubbs, B. (2016). The prevalence and predictors of bipolar and borderline personality disorders comorbidity: Systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Affective Disorders, 195, 105118. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2016.01.040CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fowler, J. C., Madan, A., Allen, J. G., Oldham, J. M., & Frueh, B. C. (2019). Differentiating bipolar disorder from borderline personality disorder: Diagnostic accuracy of the difficulty in emotion regulation scale and personality inventory for DSM-5. Journal of Affective Disorders, 245, 856860. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2018.11.079CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Franke, B., Michelini, G., Asherson, P., Banaschewski, T., Bilbow, A., Buitelaar, J. K., … Haavik, J. (2018). Live fast, die young? A review on the developmental trajectories of ADHD across the lifespan. European Neuropsychopharmacology, 28(10), 10591088. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2018.08.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garnefski, N., & Kraaij, V. (2007). The cognitive emotion regulation questionnaire. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 23(3), 141149. doi: 10.1027/1015-5759.23.3.141.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gilbert, K. E., Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Gruber, J. (2013). Positive emotion dysregulation across mood disorders: How amplifying versus dampening predicts emotional reactivity and illness course. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 51(11), 736741. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2013.08.004.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gratz, K. L., & Roemer, L. (2004). Multidimensional assessment of emotion regulation and dysregulation: Development, factor structure, and initial validation of the difficulties in emotion regulation scale. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 26(1), 4154. doi: 10.1023/B:JOBA.0000007455.08539.94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Green, M. J., & Malhi, G. S. (2006). Neural mechanisms of the cognitive control of emotion. Acta Neuropsychiatrica, 18(3–4), 144153. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-5215.2006.00149.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gross, J. J. (2015). Emotion regulation: Current status and future prospects. Psychological Inquiry, 26(1), 126. doi: 10.1080/1047840X.2014.940781.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gross, J. J., & John, O. P. (2003). Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: Implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(2), 348. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.348CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gruber, J. (2011). Can feeling too good be bad? Positive emotion persistence (PEP) in bipolar disorder. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20(4), 217221. doi: 10.1177/0963721411414632CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gruhn, M. A., & Compas, B. E. (2020). Effects of maltreatment on coping and emotion regulation in childhood and adolescence: A meta-analytic review. Child Abuse & Neglect, 103, 104446. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2020.104446CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harville, D. A. (1977). Maximum likelihood approaches to variance component estimation and to related problems. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 72(358), 320338. doi: 10.2307/2286796.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henna, E., Hatch, J. P., Nicoletti, M., Swann, A. C., Zunta-Soares, G., & Soares, J. C. (2013). Is impulsivity a common trait in bipolar and unipolar disorders? Bipolar Disorders, 15(2), 223227. doi: 10.1111/bdi.12034.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hıdıroğlu, C., Esen, Ö. D., Tunca, Z., Yalçìn, Ş. N. G., Lombardo, L., Glahn, D. C., … Özerdem, A. (2013). Can risk-taking be an endophenotype for bipolar disorder? A study on patients with bipolar disorder type I and their first-degree relatives. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 19(4), 474482. doi: 10.1017/S1355617713000015CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Higgins, J. P., Thomas, J., Chandler, J., Cumpston, M., Li, T., Page, M. J., … Welch, V. A. (2019). Cochrane handbook for systematic reviews of interventions. Chichester (UK): John Wiley & Sons.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnstone, T., Van Reekum, C. M., Urry, H. L., Kalin, N. H., & Davidson, R. J. (2007). Failure to regulate: Counterproductive recruitment of top-down prefrontal-subcortical circuitry in major depression. Journal of Neuroscience, 27(33), 88778884. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2063-07.2007CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Katayama, N., Nakagawa, A., Umeda, S., Terasawa, Y., Kurata, C., Tabuchi, H., … Mimura, M. (2019). Frontopolar cortex activation associated with pessimistic future-thinking in adults with major depressive disorder. NeuroImage: Clinical, 23, 101877. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2019.101877CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kessing, L. V., & Faurholt-Jepsen, M. (2022). Mood instability—A new outcome measure in randomised trials of bipolar disorder? European Neuropsychopharmacology: The Journal of the European College of Neuropsychopharmacology, 58, 3941. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2022.02.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, K., Kim, S. H., & Kim, S. (2022). Psychometric properties of the Korean version of the Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (K-ERQ) in a clinical sample. Psychiatry Investigation, 19(2), 125. doi: 10.30773/pi.2021.0269CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kim, S., Yu, B. H., Lee, D. S., & Kim, J.-H. (2012). Ruminative response in clinical patients with major depressive disorder, bipolar disorder, and anxiety disorders. Journal of Affective Disorders, 136(1–2), e77e81. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2011.06.034CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kjærstad, H. L., Damgaard, V., Knudsen, G. M., Vinberg, M., Kessing, L. V., Macoveanu, J., & Miskowiak, K. W. (2022). Neural underpinnings of emotion regulation subgroups in remitted patients with recently diagnosed bipolar disorder. European Neuropsychopharmacology, 60, 718. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2022.04.010CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kjærstad, H. L., Vinberg, M., Goldin, P. R., Køster, N., Støttrup, M. M. D., Knorr, U., … Miskowiak, K. W. (2016). Impaired down-regulation of negative emotion in self-referent social situations in bipolar disorder: A pilot study of a novel experimental paradigm. Psychiatry Research, 238, 318325. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2016.02.047CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kotov, R., Krueger, R. F., Watson, D., Cicero, D. C., Conway, C. C., DeYoung, C. G., … Latzman, R. D. (2021). The Hierarchical Taxonomy of Psychopathology (HiTOP): A quantitative nosology based on consensus of evidence. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 17, 83108. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-081219-093304CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leichsenring, F., Leibing, E., Kruse, J., New, A. S., & Leweke, F. (2011). Borderline personality disorder. The Lancet, 377(9759), 7484. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(10)61422-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liu, J., Chan, T. C., Chong, S. A., Subramaniam, M., & Mahendran, R. (2020). Impact of emotion dysregulation and cognitive insight on psychotic and depressive symptoms during the early course of schizophrenia spectrum disorders. Early Intervention in Psychiatry, 14(6), 691697. doi: 10.1111/eip.12895CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lois, G., Gerchen, M. F., Kirsch, P., Kanske, P., Schönfelder, S., & Wessa, M. (2017). Large-scale network functional interactions during distraction and reappraisal in remitted bipolar and unipolar patients. Bipolar Disorders, 19(6), 487495. doi: 10.1111/bdi.12512CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marwaha, S., Price, C., Scott, J., Weich, S., Cairns, A., Dale, J., … Broome, M. R. (2018). Affective instability in those with and without mental disorders: A case control study. Journal of Affective Disorders, 241, 492498. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2018.08.046CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Masi, G., Sesso, G., Pfanner, C., Valente, E., Molesti, A., Placini, F., … Montesanto, A. R. (2021). An exploratory study of emotional dysregulation dimensions in youth with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and/or bipolar spectrum disorders. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 12, 409. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.619037CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meade, C. S., Graff, F. S., Griffin, M. L., & Weiss, R. D. (2008). HIV risk behavior among patients with co-occurring bipolar and substance use disorders: Associations with mania and drug abuse. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 92(1–3), 296300. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2007.07.013CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Merza, K., Papp, G., & Kuritárné Szabó, I. (2015). The role of childhood traumatization in the development of borderline personality disorder in Hungary. The European Journal of Psychiatry, 29(2), 105118. doi: 10.4321/S0213-61632015000200002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miskowiak, K. W., & Varo, C. (2021). Social cognition in bipolar disorder: A proxy of psychosocial function and novel treatment target? European Neuropsychopharmacology: The Journal of the European College of Neuropsychopharmacology, 46, 3738. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2021.03.017CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muhtadie, L., Johnson, S. L., Carver, C. S., Gotlib, I. H., & Ketter, T. A. (2014). A profile approach to impulsivity in bipolar disorder: The key role of strong emotions. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 129(2), 100108. doi: 10.1111/acps.12136CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Müller, H., Betz, L. T., & Bechdolf, A. (2021). A comprehensive meta-analysis of the self-serving bias in schizophrenia spectrum disorders compared to non-clinical subjects. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 120, 542549. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.09.025CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murray, R. J., Gentsch, K., Pham, E., Celen, Z., Castro, J., Perroud, N., … Piguet, C. (2021). Identifying disease-specific neural reactivity to psychosocial stress in borderline personality disorder. Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging, 7(11), 11371148. doi: 10.1016/j.bpsc.2021.11.015Google ScholarPubMed
Musket, C. W., Hansen, N. S., Welker, K. M., Gilbert, K. E., & Gruber, J. (2021). A pilot investigation of emotional regulation difficulties and mindfulness-based strategies in manic and remitted bipolar I disorder and major depressive disorder. International Journal of Bipolar Disorders, 9(1), 18. doi: 10.1186/s40345-020-00206-0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Njau, S., Townsend, J., Wade, B., Hellemann, G., Bookheimer, S., Narr, K., & Brooks, J. O. III (2020). Neural subtypes of euthymic bipolar I disorder characterized by emotion regulation circuitry. Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging, 5(6), 591600. doi: 10.1016/j.bpsc.2020.02.011Google ScholarPubMed
Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1991). Responses to depression and their effects on the duration of depressive episodes. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100(4), 569. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.100.4.569CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O'Donovan, C., & Alda, M. (2020). Depression preceding diagnosis of bipolar disorder. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 11, 500. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2020.00500CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olkin, I., Dahabreh, I. J., & Trikalinos, T. A. (2012). GOSH–A graphical display of study heterogeneity. Research Synthesis Methods, 3(3), 214223. doi: 10.1002/jrsm.1053.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oymak Yenilmez, D., Atagün, M. İ., Keleş Altun, İ., Tunç, S., Uzgel, M., Altınbaş, K., … Oral, E. T. (2021). Relationship between childhood adversities, emotion dysregulation and cognitive processes in bipolar disorder and recurrent depressive disorder. Türk Psikiyatri Dergisi, 32(1), 816. doi: 10.5080/u23415Google ScholarPubMed
Ozten, M., & Erol, A. (2019). Impulsivity differences between bipolar and unipolar depression. Indian Journal of Psychiatry, 61(2), 156. doi: 10.4103/psychiatry.IndianJPsychiatry_166_18Google ScholarPubMed
Page, M. J., McKenzie, J. E., Bossuyt, P. M., Boutron, I., Hoffmann, T. C., Mulrow, C. D., … Brennan, S. E. (2021). The PRISMA 2020 statement: An updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. British Medical Journal, 372, 19. doi: 10.1136/bmj.n71Google ScholarPubMed
Perich, T., Manicavasagar, V., Mitchell, P. B., & Ball, J. R. (2011). Mindfulness, response styles and dysfunctional attitudes in bipolar disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders, 134(1–3), 126132. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2011.06.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Perlman, G., Simmons, A. N., Wu, J., Hahn, K. S., Tapert, S. F., Max, J. E., … Campbell-Sills, L. (2012). Amygdala response and functional connectivity during emotion regulation: A study of 14 depressed adolescents. Journal of Affective Disorders, 139(1), 7584. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2012.01.044CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rai, S., Griffiths, K., Breukelaar, I. A., Barreiros, A. R., Chen, W., Boyce, P., … Bryant, R. A. (2021). Investigating neural circuits of emotion regulation to distinguish euthymic patients with bipolar disorder and major depressive disorder. Bipolar Disorders, 23(3), 284294. doi: 10.1111/bdi.13042CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
R Core Team. (2020). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Retrieved from https://www.R-project.org/Google Scholar
Rive, M. M., Mocking, R. J., Koeter, M. W., van Wingen, G., de Wit, S. J., van den Heuvel, O. A., … Schene, A. H. (2015). State-dependent differences in emotion regulation between unmedicated bipolar disorder and major depressive disorder. JAMA Psychiatry, 72(7), 687696. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2015.0161.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rowland, J. E., Hamilton, M. K., Lino, B. J., Ly, P., Denny, K., Hwang, E.-J., … Green, M. J. (2013a). Cognitive regulation of negative affect in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Psychiatry Research, 208(1), 2128. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2013.02.021CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rowland, J. E., Hamilton, M. K., Vella, N. C., Lino, B. J., Mitchell, P. B., & Green, M. J. (2013b). Adaptive associations between social cognition and emotion regulation are absent in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 607. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00607.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rydahl, K. F., Brund, R. B., Medici, C. R., Straarup, K. M., Straszek, S. P., & Østergaard, S. D. (2022). Bipolar disorder and regretted behavior in relation to use of social media and online dating. Bipolar Disorders, 24(1), 2738. doi: 10.1111/bdi.13169CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schilling, L., Moritz, S., Schneider, B., Bierbrodt, J., & Nagel, M. (2015). Attributional “tunnel vision” in patients with borderline personality disorder. Journal of Personality Disorders, 29(6), 839846. doi: 10.1521/pedi_2015_29_181.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sesso, G., Milone, A., Drago, F., Viglione, V., Berloffa, S., Boldrini, S., … Placini, F. (2021). A novel multidimensional questionnaire for the assessment of emotional dysregulation in adolescents: Reactivity, Intensity, Polarity and Stability questionnaire–youth version (RIPoSt–Y). Journal of Affective Disorders, 291, 359367. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2021.05.037CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shapero, B. G., Stange, J. P., Goldstein, K. E., Black, C. L., Molz, A. R., Hamlat, E. J., … Alloy, L. B. (2015). Cognitive styles in mood disorders: Discriminative ability of unipolar and bipolar cognitive profiles. International Journal of Cognitive Therapy, 8(1), 3560. doi: 10.1521/ijct.2015.8.1.35CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sharmin, S., Kypri, K., Khanam, M., Wadolowski, M., Bruno, R., & Mattick, R. P. (2017). Parental supply of alcohol in childhood and risky drinking in adolescence: Systematic review and meta-analysis. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 14(3), 287. doi: 10.3390/ijerph14030287CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sloan, E., Hall, K., Moulding, R., Bryce, S., Mildred, H., & Staiger, P. K. (2017). Emotion regulation as a transdiagnostic treatment construct across anxiety, depression, substance, eating and borderline personality disorders: A systematic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 57, 141163. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2017.09.002.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Solmi, M., Bodini, L., Cocozza, S., Seeman, M. V., Vieta, E., Dragioti, E., … Fusar-Poli, P. (2020). Aripiprazole monotherapy as transdiagnostic intervention for the treatment of mental disorders: An umbrella review according to TRANSD criteria. European Neuropsychopharmacology, 41, 1627. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2020.09.635.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sorgi-Wilson, K. M., & McCloskey, M. S. (2022). Emotion regulation strategies among individuals with borderline personality disorder relative to other groups: A review. Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy, 29(5), 16551678. doi: 10.1002/cpp.2738.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stang, A. (2010). Critical evaluation of the Newcastle-Ottawa scale for the assessment of the quality of nonrandomized studies in meta-analyses. European Journal of Epidemiology, 25(9), 603605. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10654-010-9491-zCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stroup, D. F., Berlin, J. A., Morton, S. C., Olkin, I., Williamson, G. D., Rennie, D., … Thacker, S. B. (2000). Meta-analysis of observational studies in epidemiology: A proposal for reporting. JAMA, 283(15), 20082012. doi: 10.1001/jama.283.15.2008CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tanzer, M., Salaminios, G., Morosan, L., Campbell, C., & Debbané, M. (2021). Self-blame mediates the link between childhood neglect experiences and internalizing symptoms in low-risk adolescents. Journal of Child & Adolescent Trauma, 14(1), 7383. doi: 10.1007/s40653-020-00307-z.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Temes, C. M., Magni, L. R., Fitzmaurice, G. M., Aguirre, B. A., Goodman, M., & Zanarini, M. C. (2017). Prevalence and severity of childhood adversity in adolescents with BPD, psychiatrically healthy adolescents, and adults with BPD. Personality and Mental Health, 11(3), 171178. doi: 10.1002/pmh.1387CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thompson, R. A. (2019). Emotion dysregulation: A theme in search of definition. Development and Psychopathology, 31(3), 805815. doi: 10.1017/S0954579419000282.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Townsend, J., & Altshuler, L. L. (2012). Emotion processing and regulation in bipolar disorder: A review. Bipolar Disorders, 14(4), 326339. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2012.01021.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Treynor, W., Gonzalez, R., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2003). Rumination reconsidered: A psychometric analysis. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 27(3), 247259. doi: 10.1023/A:1023910315561.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tsuchiyagaito, A., Sánchez, S. M., Misaki, M., Kuplicki, R., Park, H., Paulus, M. P., … Guinjoan, S. M. (2022). Intensity of repetitive negative thinking in depression is associated with greater functional connectivity between semantic processing and emotion regulation areas. Psychological Medicine, First View, 112. doi: 10.1017/S0033291722002677.Google ScholarPubMed
Ulusoy, S. I., Gülseren, Ş. A., Özkan, N., & Bilen, C. (2020). Facial emotion recognition deficits in patients with bipolar disorder and their healthy parents. General Hospital Psychiatry, 65, 914. doi: 10.1016/j.genhosppsych.2020.04.008.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Meter, A. R., & Youngstrom, E. A. (2016). Distinct roles of emotion reactivity and regulation in depressive and manic symptoms among euthymic patients. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 40(3), 262274. doi: 10.1007/s10608-015-9738-9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Varo, C., Amoretti, S., Sparacino, G., Jiménez, E., Solé, B., del Mar Bonnin, C., … Salagre, E. (2022). Emotional intelligence: A comparison between patients after first episode mania and those suffering from chronic bipolar disorder type I. Psychological Medicine, 53, 112. doi: 10.1017/S0033291721005122.Google ScholarPubMed
Varo, C., Kjærstad, H. L., Poulsen, E., Meluken, I., Vieta, E., Kessing, L. V., … Miskowiak, K. W. (2021). Emotional cognition subgroups in mood disorders: Associations with familial risk. European Neuropsychopharmacology, 51, 7183. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.euroneuro.2021.05.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Viechtbauer, W., & Viechtbauer, M. W. (2015). Package ‘metafor’. The Comprehensive R Archive Network. Package ‘metafor’. http://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/metafor/metafor.pdfGoogle Scholar
Vieta, E., Berk, M., Schulze, T. G., Carvalho, A. F., Suppes, T., Calabrese, J. R., … Grande, I. (2018). Bipolar disorders. Nature Reviews Disease Primers, 4(1), 116. doi: 10.1038/nrdp.2018.8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weinstock, L. M., Chou, T., Celis-deHoyos, C., Miller, I. W., & Gruber, J. (2018). Reward and punishment sensitivity and emotion regulation processes differentiate bipolar and unipolar depression. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 42(6), 794802. doi: 10.1007/s10608-018-9945-2.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weintraub, M. J., Van de Loo, M. M., Gitlin, M. J., & Miklowitz, D. J. (2017). Self-harm, affective traits, and psychosocial functioning in adults with depressive and bipolar disorders. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 205(11), 896899. doi: 10.1097/NMD.0000000000000744CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
WHO. (2004). ICD-10: International statistical classification of diseases and related health problems (10th rev.). Genève, Switzerland: World Health Organization (WHO).Google Scholar
Wolkenstein, L., Zwick, J. C., Hautzinger, M., & Joormann, J. (2014). Cognitive emotion regulation in euthymic bipolar disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders, 160, 9297. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2013.12.022CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Figure 1. PRISMA flowchart, 2020 edition, adapted.

Figure 1

Table 1. Characteristics of the studies included in the systematic review and meta-analysis

Figure 2

Table 2. Meta-analyses results of emotion dysregulation between bipolar disorder and major depressive disorder or borderline personality disorder

Figure 3

Figure 2. Differences in adopted emotion regulation strategies between people with bipolar disorder and people with major depressive disorder (left) and borderline personality disorder (right). Overall results of the comparisons included in the meta-analysis. Note: BD, ‘bipolar disorder’; BPD, ‘borderline personality disorder’; CERQ, ‘cognitive emotion regulation scale’; CERQ-1, ‘self-blame’; CERQ-2, ‘blaming others’; CERQ-3, ‘rumination’; CERQ-4, ‘catastrophizing’; CERQ-5, ‘putting into perspective’; CERQ-6, ‘positive refocusing’; CERQ-7, ‘positive reappraisal’; CERQ-8, ‘acceptance’; CERQ-9, ‘focus on replanning’; DERS, ‘difficulties in emotion regulation scale’; DERS-1, ‘nonacceptance’; DERS-2, ‘goals’; DERS-3, ‘impulse’; DERS-4, ‘awareness’; DERS-5, ‘strategies’; DERS-6, ‘clarity’; DERS-TOT, ‘total score’; ERQ, ‘emotion regulation questionnaire’; ERQ-1, ‘reappraisal’; ERQ-2, ‘suppression’; MDD, ‘major depressive disorder’; RPA, ‘response to positive affect’; RPA-1, ‘emotion focus’; RPA-2, ‘dampening’; RPA-3, ‘self-focus’; RRS, ‘ruminative response scale’; RRS-1, ‘reflective pondering’; RRS-2, ‘brooming’; RSQ-, ‘response style questionnaire’; RSQ-1, ‘rumination’; RSQ-2, ‘adaptive’; RSQ-3, ‘risk-taking’.*These items' effect sizes (ES) have been inverted to present graphically coherent results since the scale initially measures the individual's difficulties in adopting that emotion regulation strategy.The point size is proportional to the total sample size for that particular comparison. The number in parentheses indicates the number of studies considered for that particular comparison.

Supplementary material: File

De Prisco et al. supplementary material

De Prisco et al. supplementary material
Download De Prisco et al. supplementary material(File)
File 2.9 MB