Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-fbnjt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-10T09:49:37.917Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Metonymic relations underlying the one-word utterances of Afrikaans-speaking infants and toddlers

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 October 2022

Nina Brink*
Affiliation:
North-West University, School of Languages, Potchefstroom, South Africa
*
Corresponding author. Email: Nina.Brink@nwu.ac.za
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Children’s processing and comprehension of metonymy have received little attention in the developmental literature, which has mainly focused on children’s acquisition of metaphor abilities. However, it has been found that metonymy production and comprehension precede metaphor production and comprehension (Falkum et al., 2017; Nerlich et al., 1999; Pérez-Hernández & Duvignau, 2016; Runblad & Annaz, 2010). Nerlich et al. (1999) suggest that metonymic relations are exploited in overextensions produced by children up to age 2;5 and call these “compelled metonymic overextensions”. At this very early age, a child’s vocabulary is still relatively small, and this compels them to extend already known words to cope with their increasing communicative needs. These overextensions are, however, in most cases not random, as some type of associative relation (e.g., causeeffect, objectact, containercontent, etc.) between the concepts referred to can be identified. This study focuses on the metonymic relations exploited by 18 Afrikaans-speaking infants and toddlers (between the ages of 0;6 and 2;0) in their early overextensions. The metonymic relations as described by Norrick (1981) as well as Radden and Kövecses (1999) are employed in the analysis. A total of 207 out of 1371 one-word utterances were identified as compelled metonymic overextensions and 11 types of metonymic relations could be identified as underlying these utterances. This study illustrates that the metonymic relations identified in such young children’s early language provide insight and understanding into how they categorise and associate various concepts with each other.

Type
Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press

1. Introduction

Little is known about how young pre-school children may possibly exploit metonymic relations that lead to utterances that, from an adult perspective, can be called metonyms. Köder and Falkum (Reference Köder and Falkum2020, p. 191) point out that there is growing consensus that children have early pragmatic competence but studies on children’s processing and comprehension of metonymy have received little attention in the developmental literature, which has mainly focused on children’s acquisition of metaphor abilities (see Gottfried, Reference Gottfried1997; Özçalışkan, Reference Özçalışkan2005; Özçalışkan, Reference Özçalışkan2007; Siqueira & Gibbs, Reference Siqueira and Gibbs2007; Starr & Srinivasan, Reference Starr and Srinivasan2018; Stites & Özçaliskan, Reference Stites and Özçaliskan2013).

According to Radden and Kövecses (Reference Radden, Kövecses, Panther and Radden1999: 21) “[m]etonymy is a cognitive process in which one conceptual entity, the vehicle, provides mental access to another conceptual entity, the target, within the same idealized cognitive model”. The reference-point entity serves as the vehicle for accessing the target – for instance, in the metonymic utterance She’s just a pretty face, the “pretty face” serves as the vehicle for accessing the “person” as target. The vehicle and target are both conceptually present although one of them is seen as being more salient than the other and is therefore selected as the vehicle to provide access to the target (Radden & Kövecses, Reference Radden, Kövecses, Panther and Radden1999:19; Langacker, Reference Langacker1993:30). It is important to note that metonymic relations are in principle reversible, for instance, both cause for effect and effect for cause are listed as metonymic relations. “We therefore need to ask if there are any preferred metonymic construals and, if this is the case, which ‘cognitive principles’ govern the selection of one type of vehicle entity over another” (Radden & Kövecses, Reference Radden, Kövecses, Evans, Bergen and Zinken2007:338).

Since the late 1990s and early 2000s, several authors have suggested that metonymy is a cognitive phenomenon that may be even more fundamental and basic than metaphor (Benczes et al., Reference Benczes, Barcelona, Ruiz de Mendoza Ibáñez, Benczes, Barcelona and Ruiz de Mendoza Ibáñez2011, p. 1). Panther and Thornburg (Reference Panther, Thornburg, Geeraerts and Cuyckens2007, p. 243) state that “[l]ike metaphor, metonymy is a means by which concepts with relatively little content may be conceptually elaborated and enriched”. Runblad and Annaz (Reference Runblad and Annaz2010, p. 556) argue that metonymy production and comprehension precede metaphor production and comprehension at any point in childhood because metonymy is cognitively more basic than metaphor as it only requires an association between concepts within the same conceptual domain. Pérez-Hernández and Duvignau (Reference Pérez-Hernández and Duvignau2016) also found that very young participants (1;8 to 4;2) initially use more metonymic than metaphoric utterances but that the use of metaphors also increases with age.

According to Falkum et al. (Reference Falkum, Recasens and Clark2017, p. 88) metonymy is used by speakers to communicate a variety of seemingly indefinite number of context-dependent meanings. In other words, an expression is used to refer to something that falls outside its conventional denotation, but there is a clear associative relation linking the two concepts. Nerlich et al. (Reference Nerlich, Clarke, Todd, Panther and Radden1999) suggest that metonymic relations are exploited in overextensions produced by children up to about age 2;5. They call these “compelled metonymic overextensions” because “they are based on the fact that at this age a child’s vocabulary, categories and conceptual systems are still relatively small and unstructured. This scarcity compels them to extend already known words to cope with increasing communicative needs, to comment on what they see and to request what they want” (Nerlich et al., Reference Nerlich, Clarke, Todd, Panther and Radden1999:364). Overextension takes place when a child applies a lexical item to members of a category that are perceptually similar, conceptually contiguous, or spatio-temporally related, therefore using the lexical item in a broader context than the denotation thereof, e.g., ball for an apple; horse for a cow, dada used for both father and mother, etc. (Ambridge et al., Reference Ambridge, Pine, Rowland, Chang and Bidgood2013; Clark, Reference Clark1993; Reference Clark2016; Gelman et al., Reference Gelman, Croft, Fu, Clausner and Gottfried1998; Hoek et al., Reference Hoek, Ingram and Gibson1986; Huttenlocher & Smiley, Reference Huttenlocher and Smiley1987; Nerlich et al., Reference Nerlich, Clarke, Todd, Panther and Radden1999; Walaszewska, Reference Walaszewska2011).

Pérez-Hernández and Duvignau (Reference Pérez-Hernández and Duvignau2016), who studied the language of French-speaking children between the ages of 1;8 and 4;2, found that children use “metonymy-based semantic approximations”, such as specificgeneric, effectcause and whole (action)–one of its parts, as a tool to fill gaps in their immature lexicons. Very young children therefore see similarities, connections and class-inclusions between category members and display early categorisation abilities which may lead to overextensions due to them not yet having acquired the more appropriate or conventionalised term (Nerlich et al., Reference Nerlich, Clarke, Todd, Panther and Radden1999, p. 367; Pouscoulous, Reference Pouscoulous and Matthews2014, p. 253).

It is important to note that this notion of metonymy in child language differs from the one assumed by classical rhetoric “where the speaker is seen as consciously ‘replacing’ a conventional, literal expression with a related metonymic one” (Falkum et al., Reference Falkum, Recasens and Clark2017, p. 91). It has been proven that children have an early ability to make use of salient associative relations for communicative purposes, but this does not necessarily have to involve knowledge of the conventional term for the intended referent (Falkum et al., Reference Falkum, Recasens and Clark2017, p. 91). It is therefore assumed that very young children exploiting metonymic relations in their utterances do not yet know the term for the concept they are referring to.

Nerlich et al. (Reference Nerlich, Clarke, Todd, Panther and Radden1999, pp. 368–369) provide some examples from Braine’s (Reference Braine1976) corpus of two-word utterances which have an underlying metonymic relation, for instance:

Although these utterances can be viewed as overextensions, it is also evident that there are certain metonymic relations underlying the associations the children make between the concepts. Falkum et al. (Reference Falkum, Recasens and Clark2017, p. 90) suggest that some of the strategies young children use to fill gaps in their lexicon, such as overextension, are the precursors to metonymy. Nerlich et al. (Reference Nerlich, Clarke, Todd, Panther and Radden1999, p. 369) emphasise that children with limited lexicons focus on, for instance, “one object or attribute of an object to achieve certain speech acts in a metonymical way. They focus on one salient feature in a set framework or frame of repeated interactions with the caregiver or parent”. Later on, when children have a broader vocabulary (usually from age 2;5 onwards) and the metonymic overextensions are replaced by the “conventionalised terms”, they start making use of “creative metonymical shrinking” despite the fact that they could express the same meaning with the words they know (Nerlich et al., Reference Nerlich, Clarke, Todd, Panther and Radden1999, p. 363). This study will adopt Nerlich et al.’s (Reference Nerlich, Clarke, Todd, Panther and Radden1999) view of “compelled metonymic overextensions” and focus on the metonymic relations underlying these utterances made by children just starting to acquire their language.

In a study on 21 Afrikaans-speaking children’s (between the ages of 0;6 and 2;0) early form-meaning mappings, it was found that these infants and toddlers use compelled metonymic overextensions while uttering their first lexical items (i.e., one-word utterances), for instance initially naming all women mamma (“mother”) (Brink, Reference Brink2017). However, the specific metonymic relations underlying the children’s compelled metonymic overextensions were not identified and analysed further in that study. Focusing on the metonymic relations exploited by infants and toddlers in their early overextensions may provide insights into the early stages of language acquisition and how categorisation abilities are used to associate certain concepts with each other based on specific conceptual links.

Norrick (Reference Norrick1981) provides a detailed exposition of 17 types of metonymic relations underlying utterances in adult language. Nerlich et al. (Reference Nerlich, Clarke, Todd, Panther and Radden1999, pp. 368–369) use this list to provide some examples of the metonymic relations in very young children’s utterances, but their discussion is very limited, and another study extensively identifying and describing the types of metonymic relations in children’s early language, especially at the one-word stage, could not be traced. Radden and Kövecses (Reference Radden, Kövecses, Panther and Radden1999) further build on Norrick’s list and identify several additional types of metonymic relations in adult language, classified under idealised cognitive models (ICMs). The Norrick (Reference Norrick1981) and Radden and Kövecses (Reference Radden, Kövecses, Panther and Radden1999) lists are therefore revisited to determine which of these relations can also be identified in the utterances of Afrikaans-speaking infants and toddlers, and in what way. There are some overlap and differences between these lists, therefore providing an opportunity of combining them for analysing the Afrikaans child language data.

In sum, the research question of this study is which types of underlying metonymic relations can be identified in Afrikaans-speaking infants’ and toddlers’ compelled metonymic overextensions. The hypothesis is that several of Norrick’s (Reference Norrick1981) and Radden and Kövecses’ (Reference Radden, Kövecses, Panther and Radden1999) metonymic relations will also be identified in the early language of infants and toddlers due to these children using certain strategies to fill gaps in their lexicons. Children’s everyday embodied experiences are also within the same conceptual world that adults live in, and these metonymic relations are a reflection of existing conceptual links.

This research question is addressed by discussing the relevant metonymic relations set out by Norrick (Reference Norrick1981) and Radden and Kövecses (Reference Radden, Kövecses, Panther and Radden1999). One of the aims of this study is to combine and integrate these two lists for the purpose of identifying the types of metonymic relations in the Afrikaans child language data set. This is then followed by a discussion of the method of investigation. Lastly, the results and conclusion of the study are discussed and motivated through an illustration of usage-based language data.

2. Types of metonymic relations

Falkum et al. (Reference Falkum, Recasens and Clark2017, p. 90) state that “[i]n metonymy, salient associative relations (typically relations of contiguity) are exploited for the purpose of communication” and that children’s ability to identify such relations is present from an early age. Norrick (Reference Norrick1981, p. 41) proposes that the ultimate source of association by contiguity within perception and interpretation lies in inference; people build up a stock of these associations based on their experiences. Several indexes of metonymic relations have been proposed thus far (see, for instance, Barcelona, Reference Barcelona, Benczes, Barcelona and Ruiz de Mendoza Ibáñez2011; Carrión et al., Reference Carrión, Barcelona and Rossella2018; Lakoff & Johnson, Reference Lakoff and Johnson1980). Seeing that new metonyms can continually be created on the basis of metonymic relations as language and conceptualisation evolve, these indexes may constantly be expanded.

Norrick’s (Reference Norrick1981, pp. 86–100) classification and exposition of the relations/associations as well as the metonymic principles underlying metonymic utterances, conducted from a semiotic perspective, contributed greatly to the body of literature on metonymy. Radden and Kövecses (Reference Radden, Kövecses, Panther and Radden1999, pp. 17–59) further build on Norrick’s index, providing an extensive classification of metonymic relations underlying adults’ metonymic utterances. According to Littlemore (Reference Littlemore2015, p. 21), their classification has made a significant contribution to the metonymy and cognitive linguistics literature and is still widely cited. Radden and Kövecses correlate the metonymic relations with Lakoff’s (Reference Lakoff1987) framework of ICMs, which they believe capture metonymic conceptualisation processes best. According to Ruiz de Mendoza Ibáñez and Pascual Aransaez (Reference Ruiz de Mendoza Ibáñez and Pascual Aransaez1997, p. 262) an ICM “is a conventionalized pattern of belief or communication used in understanding” and is therefore a way of organising knowledge. Radden and Kövecses (Reference Radden, Kövecses, Panther and Radden1999, p. 21) explain ICMs as models of everything that we conceptualise, including our conceptualisation of things and events, word forms and their meanings, and things and events in the real world.

The ICM concept is meant to include not only people’s encyclopaedic knowledge of a particular domain but also the cultural models they are part of. The ICM notion is not restricted to either the world of reality, the world of conceptualization or the world of language, but […] may cut across ontological realms. ICMs and the network of conceptual relationships characterizing them give rise to associations which may be exploited in metonymic transfer. (Radden & Kövecses, Reference Radden, Kövecses, Panther and Radden1999, p. 20).

Although some of Radden and Kövecses’ types overlap and/or are based on Norrick’s types, there are also metonymic relations in their list that Norrick does not refer to or that are more nuanced than his descriptions of the relations. Littlemore (Reference Littlemore2015, p. 22) provides a taxonomy of Radden and Kövecses’ key metonymic relations. I have used this taxonomy as a starting point but also added other types mentioned by Radden and Kövecses (Reference Radden, Kövecses, Panther and Radden1999) that may prove to be relevant for child language. The combined and consolidated list of metonymic relations employed in the data analysis of this study is presented in Table 1.

Table 1. Consolidated list of types of metonymic relations for the analysis of early child language

These metonymic relations can be viewed as a representation of the way adults perceive connections between phenomena and concepts in the world; everyday language reflects some of these relations. These types of metonymic relations can be identified as underlying adult language, resulting in metonymic utterances, but this leads to the question of whether they can also be identified as underlying the compelled metonymic overextensions of very young children.

Norrick (Reference Norrick1981, p. 41) explains that the contiguity and fuzziness of semiotic categories make it difficult to draw clear-cut boundaries between certain relations, and this may especially be true for child language that is notorious for its fuzziness. However, categorising Afrikaans-speaking children’s compelled metonymic overextensions according to the metonymic relation they are exploiting may provide insight into how these young children already relate certain concepts to each other, even though their vocabularies are very limited.

3. Method

This research forms part of a project in which the Afrikaans data set is analysed for various purposes. Below, the participants, respondents, data collection method and ethical considerations are discussed (see Brink, Reference Brink2017; Reference Brink2020; Brink & Breed, Reference Brink and Breed2017 for more detail) and the criteria for the classification of the types of metonymic relations are described.

3.1. Participants

The participants of the project were 21 children (8 male; 13 female) between the ages of 0;6 and 2;0, and their mothers were the respondents. Twenty participants’ mother tongue is Afrikaans, and one participant is growing up bilingually in Afrikaans and Dutch. This child’s data were however not included in the data set of this study as there are other factors to be reckoned with when a child is bilingual. The participants were located in the provinces of Gauteng, Mpumalanga, North-West, Limpopo and the Free State, i.e., from the northern parts of South Africa, at the time of data collection.

The mothers fulfilled the role of observers of the children in their natural settings, taking notes of their children’s natural and spontaneous first lexical items as well as the contexts in which these items were used. Parents understand their children’s language best because they report from a wide database of daily interaction with them (Pan, Reference Pan and Hoff2012, p. 104). This is important in interpreting a lexical item that may not be as clear to an outside observer who does not have former experience with the child. Furthermore, the parent report is the least invasive method of collecting language data from such young participants where the presence of an unknown observer/researcher may make them feel uncomfortable. The main aim was to collect natural and spontaneous speech sample sets from the children in their everyday environments. Due to ethical constraints and a limited timeframe for data collection, it was not possible to video or audio record the children. This may be seen as a limitation of this study although it has been proven that the parent report method is a reliable and convenient measurement of early child language use (Bedore et al., Reference Bedore, Peña, Joyner and Macken2011; Eriksson, Reference Eriksson2016; Marchman et al., Reference Marchman, Martínez, Hurtado, Grüter and Fernald2017; Peccei, Reference Peccei2006; Rescorla & Alley, Reference Rescorla and Alley2001).

3.2. Data collection procedure

After recruiting respondents by means of snowball sampling and social media, the respondents completed an electronic diary template (in Microsoft Word) for a period of five months. The template contained the following columns: (1) lexical item; (2) contexts of use (at least three); (3) possible meaning and (4) age at which the child used the lexical item for the first time. Additional resources provided to the respondents included an information document containing the details of the study, and a short video in which I tutored the respondents on how the diary template should be completed. The respondents sent in their first draft of diary entries after one month of data collection after which certain recommendations were made on how the entries could be improved for the data to be more suitable for the aims of the research.

In the last month of data collection, a questionnaire was also sent to the respondents in which they had to provide certain contextual information on the child’s everyday environment such as how many members of the family are living with the child, other languages the child hears regularly, information about the child’s caregiver, pets, daily routines, activities, and so forth. This questionnaire was not formally analysed as part of the data set but was used to (1) in some cases clear up possible uncertainties regarding the context in which certain lexical items were used; (2) prompt the respondent to possibly remember other lexical items that were not initially included in the diary entries (such as the names of family members or objects that form part of favourite routines); and (3) elucidate the diary entries as best as possible (e.g., knowing that the child grows up on a game farm would make it clearer why s/he frequently uses the word koedoe (“kudu”)).

3.3. Ethical considerations

Ethical clearance for the data collection of the project was obtained from the North-West University, South Africa (NWU-00184-14-A7). The respondents signed consent forms and were informed that they could withdraw their participation at any point during data collection. Pseudonyms were allocated to the children to protect their privacy and the data were fully anonymised.

3.4. Criteria for classification of metonymic overextensions

Microsoft Excel was used for the annotation and classification of the utterances for the purpose of this current study. The data set consists of 1371 usable utterancesFootnote 1 that were included in the study.

The first phase of classification involved identifying the utterances that may have an underlying metonymic relation, therefore being compelled metonymic overextensions. An utterance was classified as a compelled metonymic overextension when a complex conceptualisation process or association between concepts that signify some type of underlying metonymic relation (see Table 1) could be identified. For example, when a child said bad (“bath”) while in the bath, it was not classified as a compelled metonymic overextension because the association between the concepts is simple and easily derived. The child is merely commenting on what s/he is doing or where s/he is. But when the child was for example feeling tired and associates bathing with a process that takes place before going to sleep, and the child said bad (“bath”) while the family was doing something else, it implies a more complex association and may be considered as an utterance with an underlying metonymic relation.

Another example is that when a child was merely making sound effects that accompanied a specific act, such as am-am while playing with a toy car or wheee while swinging, it was not considered as having an underlying metonymic relation. However, if the child named the specific object according to the sound it makes, i.e., a toy car an am-am or a swing a wheee, it would be considered a compelled metonymic overextension. A total of 207 utterances in the data set were identified as compelled metonymic overextensions during the first phase of classification.

The second phase of classification entailed determining which type of metonymic relation, as presented in Table 1, the child is possibly exploiting while making the utterance. As Littlemore (Reference Littlemore2015, p. 27) points out, identifying and categorising instances of metonymy in real-life data are often difficult because utterances can convey several different meanings at once. This also rings true for first utterances that consist of only one lexical item (and not a whole phrase or sentence) and meanings and metonymic relations should then be derived from the contexts in which the utterances were used.

The third phase of classification involved organising the data according to the types of metonymic relations as well as the children who made the utterances. In two of the children’s data, no compelled metonymic overextensions could be identified, bringing the total number of participants for this study to 18. These two children were also those who produced the smallest number of utterances in the data set. There is, however, not necessarily a predictive relationship between age and the total number of usable utterances or compelled metonymic relations the children produced. Children start to use their first lexical items at various ages and therefore it was necessary to include children from a wide age range (0;6 to 2;0). Because this study focuses on children’s first lexical items, it was also expected that the number of utterances with an underlying metonymic relation would not be very high.

4. Results and discussion

Of the 30 types of metonymic relations listed in Table 1, 11 were identified in the data set. Table 2 indicates which types were identified together with their frequency, as well as an example of such an utterance from the data set. These types of metonymic relations as well as other relevant examples of the compelled metonymic overextensions are discussed in detail in the following subsections.

Table 2. Types of metonymic relations identified in the data set

a All these metonymic relations are also reversible, e.g., in some cases the cause may be used as source expression to refer to the effect as target expression, the act to refer to the object, etc.

4.1. Causation

4.1.1. causeeffect

Norrick (Reference Norrick1981, pp. 41–42) explains that people regularly infer both causes from effects and effects from causes, and that this principle for instance defines the relation between a falling object and the existence of gravity. This type of metonymic relation was identified in three utterances in the data, indicating that it is not a very active categorisation mechanism at this stage. These three examples are:

These examples illustrate how the children draw conceptual connections between the cause and effect of certain processes. In (1) the child knows that touching the stove top (cause – even though in this case she has not touched the stove top) will lead to being burned (effect) and that will cause pain, therefore the effect is lexically expressed through the Afrikaans equivalent of ouch. In examples (2) and (3) the children made a conceptual connection between when s/he does something wrong or something s/he is not supposed to do (cause) and then having an adult or someone else say nee (“no”) to them (effect). In all these utterances with the underlying causeeffect metonymic relation, the effect is the expressed source, and the cause is referred to.

4.2. Acts and major participants/agents

4.2.1. objectact

According to Norrick (Reference Norrick1981, p. 48), the objectact principle is a perceived contiguity relation and describes for instance the relations between a nail and an act of hammering, or a book and an act of writing. Of the utterances in the data set, 25 were classified as objectact. Most of these utterances’ communicative function is one of the following: (1) The child wants to or is doing a specific act with an object; (2) The child is commenting on an act in which the object is involved in or (3) The child wants someone else to do a specific act with an object. Example (4) illustrates a child expressing the need to do a certain act with an object.

In example (5) a child comments on the certain act in which the object is involved in, i.e., a fan (object) that turns (act).

Example (6) shows how a child wants someone else to do a specific act with an object.

In 13 utterances, the object is the vehicle that provides access to the act as target and in 12 utterances vice versa. As these frequencies regarding vehicle and target are so close, it cannot be determined whether the object or the act is more salient for these children at this stage.

4.2.2. instrumentact

Similar to acts that “stand for the class of their objects, they [can also] serve as signs of their instruments”, such as pens and writing and cups and drinking (Norrick, Reference Norrick1981, p. 50). In a total of 21 compelled metonymic utterances, a relation between an instrument and the act with which it is associated can be observed. Although the metonymic relations of instrumentact and objectact are not easily differentiated, especially in child language, the former differs from the latter by involving a specific instrument that is crucial in the execution of the action. Therefore, when the child specifically referred to an instrument that needs to be used for a certain act, or used the lexical item for the act to refer to the instrument, it was classified as instrumentact:

In eight of these cases, the act was used as the vehicle to access the instrument as target and in 13 cases the instrument was used as the vehicle. Determining whether the act or instrument is the preferred lexical item used by these children is therefore not possible.

4.2.3. agentact

This metonymic relation refers to the conceptual association between an act and the agent responsible for the act, such as a baker and baking (Norrick, Reference Norrick1981, p. 51). There are six instances in the data set where a metonymic relation between an agent and an act can be identified. Two examples are provided below.

These examples also correspond with the metonymic relation of salient propertycategory (see Section 4.4.1) because it refers to a characteristic of respectively dogs and birds, but in these instances the lexical items are used to refer to these agents’ specific acts. In four cases, the acts are employed as the vehicle and in two cases the agents are employed as the vehicle providing mental access to the target. As there are few examples of agentact it is difficult to determine which “side” of the conceptual relation is possibly favoured by the children.

4.2.4. destinationmotion

The destinationmotion metonymic relation does not appear explicitly in Norrick’s index but Radden and Kövecses (Reference Radden, Kövecses, Panther and Radden1999, p. 37) include it under the Action ICM. The example that they provide is to porch the newspaper. Two possible utterances in which this type of metonymic relation may be present were identified:

Interestingly, these two examples show how the same lexical item is used by two different children. These two utterances are not classified under instrumentact or objectact as they specifically refer to the destination (the vehicle) where the children wish the object should be placed, therefore also implicating a motion (the target) of placing an object around their necks.

4.3. Category and member(s)

4.3.1. categorymember/individual of the category

This metonymic relation that falls under the ICM Category and member(s) was taken from Radden and Kövecses’ list (Radden & Kövecses, Reference Radden, Kövecses, Panther and Radden1999, pp. 34–35) although it also corresponds with the specificgeneric and partwhole relations as explained by Norrick (Reference Norrick1981, pp. 35–36). This type of metonymic relation was identified as second most frequent in the data set, namely 56 times. In 41 of the utterances, an individual (as a typical member and as the vehicle) of a category was extended to include all the members of that specific superordinate category (the target). Examples include the following:

In one example, i.e., where the child said boom (“tree”) to refer to the branches of the tree, the superordinate category was the source expression. In the other 14 utterances, a somewhat different metonymic relation was noticed, namely an individual member of the category standing in for another member of the same category. These utterances also involve an easily established relationship between category members and was therefore regarded as a subtype here and not added as an additional type of metonymic relation in the list. See the following examples in this regard:

These utterances are usually categorised as children’s overextensions, and this is the case here as well. However, a metonymic relation can also be identified as one category member is used as a vehicle to access another category member as target within the same conceptual domain. Children usually extend these words “to instances of other categories within the same or an adjacent conceptual domain” (Clark, Reference Clark1993, p. 34), mostly on the basis of “perceptual similarity and conceptual contiguity” (Nerlich et al., Reference Nerlich, Clarke, Todd, Panther and Radden1999, p. 365).

4.4. Category and property

4.4.1. salient/defining propertycategory

Radden and Kövecses (Reference Radden, Kövecses, Panther and Radden1999, p. 35) explain properties may be seen either metaphorically as possessed objects (corresponding with the Possession ICM) or metonymically as parts of an object (corresponding with Norrick’s objectact or agentact metonymic relation). They further explain that:

[i]f categories are intentionally defined by a set of properties, these properties are necessarily part of the category. Categories typically evoke, and metonymically stand for, one of their defining or otherwise essential properties and, conversely, a defining or essential property may evoke, or stand for, the category it defines.

(Radden & Kövecses, Reference Radden, Kövecses, Panther and Radden1999, p. 35).

Bearing this in mind, it is not surprising that this metonymic relation was identified as most frequent in the data set (N = 63) as children are fine observers of objects’ salient and/or defining properties. Onomatopoeic utterances (that are very common in children’s early language use – Kauschke & Hofmeister, Reference Kauschke and Hofmeister2002; Laing, Reference Laing2014; Perry et al., Reference Perry, Perlman and Lupyan2015, Reference Perry, Perlman, Winter, Massaro and Lupyan2017) were classified as salient/defining propertycategory and occurred frequently. Motamedi et al. (Reference Motamedi, Murgiano, Perniss, Wonnacott, Marshall, Goldin-Meadow and Vigliocco2021) explain that onomatopoeia is particularly useful in children’s early vocabulary development as it offers a link between word and sensory experience that is not present in arbitrary forms. They found that onomatopoeic forms are learnt more easily by children compared with non-iconic forms. See the following examples of onomatopoeic utterances:

Other examples of this salient/defining propertycategory that are not onomatopoeic are:

In seven of the utterances identified as salient/defining propertycategory, an initial association between a salient property and a category was made but thereafter an extension of that association to that of member/individual of the categorycategory took place. See the following examples:

In (21) the salient property of a ball’s roundness is further extended to include any object in the conceptual domain of roundness. In (22) the very distinctive and loud three to four note call of a hadada ibis, found in many parts of South Africa, is extended to not only refer to this specific type of bird but to all types of birds, even though birds produce different sounds. These examples also correspond with Clark’s (Reference Clark1993, p. 34) description of words being overextended to instances of other categories within the same or an adjacent conceptual domain.

In all the utterances with this underlying metonymic relation, the salient property is used as the source expression to refer to the category as the target.

4.5. Event

4.5.1. actcomplex act/sub-eventwhole event

Norrick’s (Reference Norrick1981) actcomplex act and Radden and Kövecses’ (Reference Radden, Kövecses, Panther and Radden1999) sub-eventwhole event amount to the same type of metonymic relation and were therefore combined here. Radden and Kövecses (Reference Radden, Kövecses, Panther and Radden1999, p. 32) explain that events can metaphorically be viewed as having different parts, or rather sub-events. Norrick (Reference Norrick1981, p. 93) provides examples of the verb cook that includes a range of operations associated with preparing food as well as eat that does not only refer to chewing and swallowing. The 11 utterances identified as involving this metonymic relation do not include very complex acts, but the children do make connections between initial or preparatory events/acts that are part of a larger set of events/acts. See two examples below:

These two utterances refer to a complex set of acts/events that the children associate with each other. In (23), the child associates lighting a fire as a preparatory act with the more complex set of acts that a person performs during the barbecuing (braaiing) process and thereafter. In (24), the child associates his mother going to the bathroom with her having to use the toilet (although this is not always the reason for going to a bathroom). In all these types of utterances, the act or sub-event is used as vehicle to refer to the complex act or whole event as target. This makes sense in terms of child language as it is assumed that the sub-event is more easily conceptualised than the whole event.

4.6. Containment

4.6.1. contentcontainer

The Containment ICM is salient in everyday life and very basic and well-entrenched (Radden & Kövecses, Reference Radden, Kövecses, Panther and Radden1999, p. 41). With the contentcontainer metonymic relation, people are generally more interested “in the contents of a container than in the mere container so that we commonly find metonymies which target the contents via the container […] rather than the reverse metonymic relationship” (Radden & Kövecses, Reference Radden, Kövecses, Panther and Radden1999, p. 41). However, in all eight utterances, the children refer to the content (vehicle) of a specific container (target). Examples include the following:

Example (25) illustrates the salience of a small child’s (baby) bottle in his/her life and that they usually associate these containers with specific contents. Example (26) is interesting as the child first made an association between cinnamon (which in this case will be her basic level category for spices – Rosch et al., Reference Rosch, Mervis, Gray, Johnson and Boyes-Braem1976) and its container, and then extended the content as source expression to include any container of spices (therefore leaning more towards a member/individual of the categorycategory metonymic relation).

4.7. Location

4.7.1. localityoccupant/placeinhabitants

Both Norrick (Reference Norrick1981, p. 96) and Radden and Kövecses (Reference Radden, Kövecses, Panther and Radden1999, p. 41) include this metonymic relation in their indexes although Norrick refers to localityoccupant and Radden and Kövecses to placeinhabitants. Radden and Kövecses (Reference Radden, Kövecses, Panther and Radden1999, p. 41) explain that places are often associated with people living there, and in four of the seven utterances this is the case. See (28) in which the utterance indicates a simple metonymic relation between a place and its inhabitant.

In the following two examples, an object (not a person) and the place/locality in which you will “normally” find that object are metonymically associated. These utterances are also classified under this type of metonymic relation as they do involve the Location ICM.

In all these utterances, the occupant is used as the vehicle to refer to the locality as target.

4.8. Possession

4.8.1. possessorpossession

The last type of metonymic relation to be discussed here is possessorpossession. This metonymic relation is well-entrenched and hardly noticeable in adult language (compare expressions such as I am parked over there and the often cited The ham sandwich had a side dish of salad) (Radden & Kövecses, Reference Radden, Kövecses, Panther and Radden1999, p. 41). Although salient in adult language, this metonymic relation was identified in only five utterances. Examples of a relation between a possessor and their possession include (31) and (32).

In the following example, a further extension of this relation is made by the child.

In this example, the child is making an association between a possession normally belonging to her (as indicated by mine) and a possession belonging to someone else. She then extends the lexical item to also include something belonging to someone else (which will normally be indicated by a lexical item such as yours or not mine). In all five utterances, the possessor is the vehicle, and the possession is the target.

5. Conclusion

Norrick’s (Reference Norrick1981) and Radden and Kövecses’ (Reference Radden, Kövecses, Panther and Radden1999) metonymic relations are illustrations of how people perceive and associate concepts with each other. Children are fine observers of how certain concepts and processes in their environment are connected and can identify associations from an early age. This study found that certain metonymic relations are underlying to Afrikaans-speaking children’s earliest one-word utterances. These one-word utterances are viewed here as compelled metonymic overextensions (Nerlich et al., Reference Nerlich, Clarke, Todd, Panther and Radden1999). The children are “compelled” to use such utterances to communicate the widest possible range of meanings with their limited lexicons. This connects with another study from Brink (Reference Brink2020) in which the same data set is analysed to identify the range of communicative intentions of the children’s holophrases (one-word utterances). This is done in accordance with Tomasello’s (Reference Tomasello2003) list of communicative intentions typically expressed through one-word utterances, such as requesting or indicating the existence of objects, requesting or describing the recurrence of objects or events, commenting on the location of objects and people, etc.

All the utterances described in this study have underlying illocutionary functions but the difference here is that a metonymic relation could also be identified as one of the driving forces of these utterances. This corresponds with Pérez-Hernández and Duvignau’s (Reference Pérez-Hernández and Duvignau2020, p. 299) finding that the cognitive operations of children’s language use “fulfil a more basic function, namely, that of allowing the expression of a thought or a description of reality, not necessarily an abstract one, for which the kid still lacks the precise vocabulary”.

Eleven of the initial 30 proposed types of metonymic relations were identified as underlying 207 utterances in the data set (see Table 2). The four types of relations that were identified as most frequent are salient/defining propertycategory (N = 63), categorymember/individual of the category (N = 56), objectact (N = 25) and instrumentact (N = 21). The identified types of metonymic relations in the Afrikaans data set provide a window into understanding the conceptual and cognitive underpinnings of the children’s early language use.

It is possible that there may be preferred metonymic construals, or preferred routes in child language that can serve as the “default cases of metonymy”. References to which “side” of the metonymic relation served as vehicle and as target were also included in the discussion. As the data set is limited, it is not possible to make any generalisations, but a few remarks are in order as this can provide further insights into children’s conceptualisation processes. The preferred routes regarding the metonymic relations that were identified are the following: (1) act for complex act; (2) salient property for category; (3) individual/member for the category; (4) effect for cause; (5) content for container; (6) occupant for locality; (7) destination for motion and (8) possessor for possession. With the following metonymic relations, a clear preferred route could not be established: (1) agentact; (2) instrumentact and (3) objectact. A conclusion that can, however, be drawn, is that in most of these cases, the metonymic vehicle is more concrete, basic, salient and related to the experiential world of the child. Therefore, as can be expected of such young children, they rely more on perceptually observable entities to make connections between various concepts and categories.

All these Afrikaans-speaking children grow up in similar households and socio-cultural contexts, but it cannot be said with certainty that these contexts specifically influence the types of metonymic relations the children draw from. A contrastive, cross-linguistic study focusing on the metonymic relations underlying children’s utterances in other languages may provide further insight into this matter. However, as can be expected, there are several context-specific utterances in the data set that relate directly to the South African environment and socio-cultural context, such as the use of the lexical items baai (“braai”), hahaaá (the sound of the hadada ibis) and several utterances relating to wild animals.

Acknowledgements

I want to thank the two reviewers of this article for their invaluable suggestions and comments on how to revise and improve this manuscript.

Data availability statement

The raw Afrikaans data set of the research project is available as an open resource on the South African Centre for Digital Language Resources’ (SADiLaR’s) website. It can be accessed by following this link: https://repo.sadilar.org/handle/20.500.12185/479.

Competing interests

The author declares none.

Footnotes

1 A total of 67 utterances in the data set were annotated as “unusable”, for one or more of the following reasons: (1) The respondent was uncertain about the meaning or function of the particular lexical item; (2) A lexical item was not used in a specific, distinguishable context; (3) The lexical item was not established or identifiable and (4) Too little information was provided by the respondent to be able to derive a specific meaning or intention of the utterance.

References

Ambridge, B., Pine, J. M., Rowland, C., Chang, F., & Bidgood, A. (2013). The retreat from overgeneralization in child language acquisition: word learning, morphology, and verb argument structure. WIREs Cognitive Science, 4, 4762.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barcelona, A. (2011). Reviewing the properties and prototype structure of metonymy. In Benczes, R., Barcelona, A., & Ruiz de Mendoza Ibáñez, F. J. (Eds.), Defining metonymy in cognitive linguistics (pp. 760). John Benjamins Publishing Company.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bedore, L. M., Peña, E. D., Joyner, D., & Macken, C. (2011). Parent and teacher rating of bilingual language proficiency and language development concerns. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 14(5), 489511.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benczes, R., Barcelona, A., & Ruiz de Mendoza Ibáñez, F. J. (2011). Introduction. In Benczes, R., Barcelona, A., & Ruiz de Mendoza Ibáñez, F. J. (Eds.), Defining metonymy in cognitive linguistics (pp. 16). John Benjamins Publishing Company.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Braine, M. D. S. (1976). Children’s first word combinations. Monographs of the Society of Research in Child Development (Serial No. 164), 41, 1104.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brink, N. (2020). A usage-based investigation of Afrikaans-speaking children’s holophrases and communicative intentions. Stellenbosch Papers in Linguistics Plus, 59, 2144.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brink, N. & Breed, A. (2017). Hoe jong Afrikaanssprekende kinders betekenis aan hul vroeë taalgebruik verbind / The way in which young Afrikaans-speaking children connect meaning to their early vocabulary. Journal of Humanities, 57(4), 10121036.Google Scholar
Brink, R. C. (2017). Afrikaanssprekende kinders se eerste leksikale items / Afrikaans-speaking children’s first lexical items. Master’s dissertation, North-West University, Potchefstroom Campus, South Africa. https://repository.nwu.ac.za/handle/10394/25206.Google Scholar
Carrión, O. B., Barcelona, A., & Rossella, A. (Eds.) (2018). Conceptual metonymy: Methodological, theoretical, and descriptive issues. John Benjamins Publishing Company.Google Scholar
Clark, E. V. (1993). The lexicon in acquisition. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, E. V. (2016). First language acquisition. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eriksson, M. (2016). The Swedish communicative development inventory III: Parent reports on language in preschool children. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 41(5), 647654.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Falkum, I. L., Recasens, M., & Clark, E. V. (2017). “The moustache sits down first”: On the acquisition of metonymy. Journal of Child Language, 44, 87119.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gelman, S. A., Croft, W., Fu, P., Clausner, T., & Gottfried, G. (1998). Why is a pomegranate an apple? The role of shape, taxonomic relatedness, and prior lexical knowledge in children’s overextensions of apple and dog. Journal of Child Language, 25(2), 267297.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gottfried, G. M. (1997). Using metaphors as modifiers: Children’s production of metaphoric compounds. Journal of Child Language, 24, 567601.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoek, D., Ingram, D., & Gibson, D. (1986). Some possible causes of children’s early word overextensions. Journal of Child Language, 13(3), 477494.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huttenlocher, J. & Smiley, P. (1987). Early word meanings: The case of object names. Cognitive Psychology, 19, 6389.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kauschke, C. & Hofmeister, C. (2002). Early lexical development in German: A study on vocabulary growth and vocabulary composition during the second and third year of life. Journal of Child Language, 29(4), 735757.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Köder, F. & Falkum, I. L. (2020). Children’s metonymy comprehension: Evidence from eye-tracking and picture selection. Journal of Pragmatics, 156, 191205.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Laing, C. E. (2014). A phonological analysis of onomatopoeia in early word production. First Language, 34(5), 387405.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lakoff, G. (1987). Women, fire, and dangerous things. University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lakoff, G. & Johnson, M. (1980). Metaphors we live by. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Langacker, R. W. (1993). Reference-point constructions. Cognitive Linguistics, 4, 138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Littlemore, J. (2015). Metonymy: Hidden shortcuts in language, thought and communication. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marchman, V. A., Martínez, L. Z., Hurtado, N., Grüter, T., & Fernald, A. (2017). Caregiver talk to young Spanish-English bilinguals: Comparing direct observation and parent-report measures of dual-language exposure. Developmental Science, 20(1), 113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Motamedi, Y., Murgiano, M., Perniss, P., Wonnacott, E., Marshall, C., Goldin-Meadow, S., & Vigliocco, G. (2021). Linking language to sensory experience: Onomatopoeia in early language development. Developmental Science, 24(3), 113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nerlich, B., Clarke, D. D., & Todd, Z. (1999). “Mummy, I like being a sandwich”: Metonymy in language acquisition. In Panther, K. & Radden, G. (Eds.), Metonymy in language and thought (pp. 361383). John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Norrick, N. R. (1981). Semiotic principles in semantic theory. John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Özçalışkan, S. (2005). On learning to draw the distinction between physical and metaphorical motion: Is metaphor an early emerging cognitive and linguistic capacity? Journal of Child Language, 32, 291318.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Özçalışkan, S. (2007). Metaphors we move by: children’s developing understanding of metaphorical motion in typologically distinct language. Metaphor and Symbol, 22(2), 147168.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pan, B. A. (2012). Assessing vocabulary skills. In Hoff, E. (Ed.), Research methods in child language (pp. 240251). Blackwell Publishing Ltd.Google Scholar
Panther, K. & Thornburg, L. (2007). Metonymy. In Geeraerts, D. & Cuyckens, H. (Eds.), Cognitive linguistics (pp. 236263). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Peccei, J. S. (2006). Child language: A resource book for students. Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pérez-Hernández, L. & Duvignau, K. (2016). Metaphor, metonymy, and their interaction in the production of semantic approximations by monolingual children: A corpus analysis. First Language, 36(4), 383406.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pérez-Hernández, L. & Duvignau, K. (2020). A cognitive approach to semantic approximations in monolingual English-speaking children. Poznan Studies in Contemporary Linguistics, 56(2), 277313.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perry, L. K., Perlman, M., & Lupyan, G. (2015). Iconicity in English and Spanish and its relation to lexical category and age acquisition. PLoS One, 10(9), 117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perry, L. K., Perlman, M., Winter, B., Massaro, D. W., & Lupyan, G. (2017). Iconicity in the speech of children and adults. Developmental Science, 21(3), 18.Google ScholarPubMed
Pouscoulous, N. (2014). “The elevator’s buttocks”: Metaphorical abilities in children. In Matthews, D. (Ed.), Pragmatic development in first language acquisition (pp. 239259). John Benjamins Publishing Company.Google Scholar
Radden, G. & Kövecses, Z. (1999). Towards a theory of metonymy. In Panther, K. & Radden, G. (Eds.), Metonymy in language and thought (pp. 1759). John Benjamins Publishing Company.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Radden, G. & Kövecses, Z. (2007). Towards a theory of metonymy. In Evans, V., Bergen, B., & Zinken, J. (Eds.), The cognitive linguistics reader (pp. 335359). Equinox.Google Scholar
Rescorla, L. & Alley, A. (2001). Validation of the language development survey (LDS): A parent report tool for identifying language delay in toddlers. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 44(1), 434445.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosch, E. H., Mervis, C. B., Gray, W. D., Johnson, D. M., & Boyes-Braem, P. (1976). Basic objects in natural categories. Cognitive Psychology, 8(3), 382439.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ruiz de Mendoza Ibáñez, F. J. & Pascual Aransaez, C. (1997). Conceptual schemas as propositional idealized cognitive models: In search of a unified framework for the analysis of knowledge organization. Cuadernos de Investigación Filológica, 23–24, 257270.Google Scholar
Runblad, G. & Annaz, D. (2010). Development of metaphor and metonymy comprehension: Receptive vocabulary and conceptual knowledge. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 28, 547563.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Siqueira, M. & Gibbs, R. (2007). Children’s acquisition of primary metaphors: A crosslinguistic study. Organon, 43, 161179.Google Scholar
Starr, A. & Srinivasan, M. (2018). Spatial metaphor and the development of cross-domain mappings in early childhood. Developmental Psychology, 54(10), 18221832.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stites, L. J. & Özçaliskan, S. (2013). Teasing apart the role of cognitive and verbal factors in children’s early metaphorical abilities. Metaphor and Symbol, 28, 116129.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2003). Constructing a language. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Walaszewska, E. (2011). Broadening and narrowing in lexical development: How relevance theory can account for children’s overextensions and underextensions. Journal of Pragmatics, 43(1), 314326.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Consolidated list of types of metonymic relations for the analysis of early child language

Figure 1

Table 2. Types of metonymic relations identified in the data set