Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-jn8rn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-25T19:22:31.689Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Chapter 20 - Understanding Child Development: A Biosocial Anthropological Approach to Early Life

from Section 5 - The Biosocial in Practice

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  20 June 2024

Michelle Pentecost
Affiliation:
King's College London
Jaya Keaney
Affiliation:
University of Melbourne
Tessa Moll
Affiliation:
University of the Witwatersrand
Michael Penkler
Affiliation:
University of Applied Sciences, Wiener Neustadt

Summary

In this chapter, we reflect on how different disciplines have conceptualised ‘early life’ with particular insights from evolutionary, social, and medical anthropology to challenge and further expand the narrow framing of a Developmental Origins of Health and Disease (DOHaD) focus and to show the scope of a biosocial perspective. First, we introduce how childhood and early life have been studied in anthropology, followed by a discussion on how early life has been conceptualised in public health, lifecourse, and development research. We then discuss how concepts of early life may impact caregiving practice and childhood environments, which in turn impacts research on early life itself, with longitudinal birth cohort studies as an example. We highlight the need for critical and reflective thinking about the ways in which we do biosocial research, and the impact it has on our understanding of the DOHaD. We suggest that a reflexively engaged biosocial anthropological dialogue around research on early life broadens the scope of cross-disciplinary work, engages with the complex and dynamic process of childhood development, and contributes to a more nuanced framework of early life for DOHaD-informed research and health practice.

Type
Chapter
Information
The Handbook of DOHaD and Society
Past, Present, and Future Directions of Biosocial Collaboration
, pp. 231 - 240
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2024
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This content is Open Access and distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence CC-BY 4.0 https://creativecommons.org/cclicenses/

20.1 Introduction

A biosocial understanding of child development frames development as a dynamic process that influences, and is influenced by, childrearing environments [Reference Harris and McDade1]. This encourages a complex understanding of the determinants of development by considering both biological and socio-cultural factors, which in turn encourages cross/interdisciplinary approaches. There is strong evidence that the Developmental Origins of Health and Disease (DOHaD) is best understood from a biosocial perspective that acknowledges and seeks to better understand the dynamic interactions between the biological and social [Reference Harris and McDade1]; however, cross/interdisciplinary research often encounters barriers and challenges such as epistemological differences and misunderstandings [Reference Emmott, Myers and Page2]. Understanding how research around child development has been conducted may help inform and facilitate effective biosocial collaborations.

Anthropology, being a diverse discipline spanning biological and social-cultural studies, is well positioned to examine and inform biosocial approaches. There has been long-standing interest in the biosocial within and beyond anthropology [Reference Fuentes3] as well as established traditions such as biocultural approaches in US-based anthropology that have long sought to better align social and biological sciences [Reference Goodman and Leatherman4]. Recently, there is an emerging biosocial agenda in anthropology, in particular, medical anthropology (cf. Singer et al. on ‘syndemics’ [Reference Singer, Bulled, Ostrach and Mendenhall5], Lock on ‘local and situated biologies’ [Reference Lock6], Roberts on ‘bioethnography’ [Reference Roberts7], and Gibbon et al. on ‘biosocial medical anthropology’ [Reference Gibbon, Daly, Parkhurst, Ryan, Salali and Tasker8]; see also Alvergne on ‘evolutionary medicine’ [Reference Alvergne, Jenkinson and Faurie9]). Anthropology is as a result particularly well placed to contribute to work within DOHaD to foster better biosocial frameworks of understanding child development.

Building on this previous work, in this chapter we reflect on how different disciplines have conceptualised ‘early life’ with particular insights from evolutionary, social, and medical anthropology to challenge and further expand the narrow framing of DOHaD focus and to show the scope of a biosocial perspective. First, we introduce how childhood and early life have been studied in anthropology, followed by a discussion on how early life has been conceptualised in public health, lifecourse, and development research. We then discuss how concepts of early life may impact caregiving practice and childhood environments, which in turn impacts research on early life itself, with longitudinal birth cohort studies as an example. While recognising that there are points of difference in approach and analysis in the disciplinary reflections brought together in this chapter, and also that our discussion and analysis are far from comprehensive, we nonetheless highlight the need for critical and reflective thinking about the ways in which we do biosocial research, and the impact it has on our understanding of DOHaD. Overall, we suggest that a more reflexively engaged biosocial anthropological dialogue around research on early life helps to broaden the scope of cross-disciplinary work that can more fully engage with the complex and dynamic process of childhood development and present a more nuanced framework of early life for DOHaD informed research and health practice.

20.2 ‘Early Life’ in Anthropology

Children, childhood, and child development have long been a focus of interest in anthropology, with some considering it as central to its foundation and disciplinary development [Reference La Fontaine, Richards and Light10]. By studying children across cultures, anthropologists in the early twentieth century directly challenged the notion of ‘childhood’ as universal or that child development is shaped solely by physiology, biology, or hormones, instead showing how early life is a period of both intense socialisation and cultural transmission [Reference Boas and Spiller11Reference Benedict13]. Anthropologists continue to engage with early life by describing the variety of childhoods across cultures, examining how ecology and culture impact development, and testing processes of cultural transmission, to name a few [Reference LeVine and New14Reference Lancy16].

At the same time, there is a great deal of heterogeneity in the way that children and childhood have been studied [Reference Levine17, Reference Montgomery18], and how this period of the lifecourse is approached; a diversity that provides a particular resource for widening the lens of DOHaD research and perspectives on early life. This is reflected in a more psychologically informed focus on child development in US-based anthropology that draws from lifecourse theory [Reference Giele and Elder19]. A focus on childhood in anthropology is also informed by what is called ‘four field’ approaches that include physical and cultural anthropology as well as archaeology and linguistics [Reference LeVine and New14, Reference Lancy16], as demonstrated, for example, in the classic work of Margaret Mead [Reference Mead12]. While European anthropology also attended to children’s lives, this was by contrast more as part of an evolving ethnographic tradition that aimed to examine wider social structures rather than child development per se (cf. Malinowski [Reference Malinowski20] and Richards [Reference Richards21]). A more explicit focus on childhood, however, emerged within a constructivist-situated paradigm that highlights personhood and agency in examining early socialisation [Reference Montgomery18, Reference Bluebond-Langer22, Reference Hardman23]. While there are differences in the historical evolution of research in anthropology on children and childhood development, it is true to say that diverse traditions of anthropology (including those beyond a Euro-American context – see, for instance, work in South Africa such as Reynolds [Reference Reynolds24] and Ross and Pentecost [Reference Ross and Pentecost25]) have collectively helped to show how child socialisation is variably influenced by both culture and ecology, while also recognising that there are some shared mechanisms and processes.

Notably this broad landscape of anthropological work has led to directly challenging Eurocentric ideas in developmental psychology and beyond, including Bowlby and Ainsworth’s ‘attachment theory’, which not only fails to consider non-Western caregiving approaches [Reference Keller26] but sees intensive caregiving by a primary caregiver as biologically adaptive [Reference LeVine, Otto and Keller27]. An evolutionary anthropological perspective highlights the importance of a wide range of caregivers beyond the mother, with humans evolving a cooperative childrearing system [Reference Emmott, Page, Shackelford and Weekes-Shackelford28]. Cross-cultural, comparative research highlights how attachment to a single individual is not always observed nor optimal [Reference Keller26]. In summary, diverse histories of engagement with early life across different traditions of anthropology provide an important basis and resource in widening biosocial dialogue. This enables us to consider how DOHaD-informed research, policies, and practices might be expanded to encompass a broader range of factors and contexts in childhood development.

20.3 Concepts of ‘Early Life’ and Optimal Developmental Environments

In contemporary public health research and practice, child development is typically viewed as a process of growth where individuals gain socio-emotional, physical, and cognitive traits until they reach their ‘final state’ in adulthood [Reference Larcher, Schramme and Edwards29]. Development is often represented in terms of trajectories, where there is an expected and optimal path of growth, or in terms of milestones, where development is sequential and additive. Taking physical development as an example, anthropometric measures are commonly mapped onto the WHO’s child growth standards, which describe ‘normal child growth [trajectories] from birth to 5 years under optimal environmental conditions’ [30]. Here, average development is often perceived as ‘good development’, and being under- or overdeveloped may potentially be problematic with an increased risk of negative health outcomes. Similarly, motor skills may be mapped onto expected milestones using the Ages and Stages Questionnaire, a threshold-based, age-specific global screening tool assessing developmental progress [Reference Singh, Yeh and Boone31]. Here, development is conceptualised as hierarchical where later stages of development are ‘more advanced’ and often perceived as ‘good development’. For both trajectories and milestones, development is underpinned by biology, interacts with the environment, and builds through time. What children experience and how they develop in early life act as foundations for later life, framing the lifecourse approach as critical for developmental research. Combined with this is the idea of sensitive and critical periods, particularly in the first few years of life where ‘the brain is “tuned” by the input from the environment’ [Reference Krishnan and Johnson32].

This view of early life, by default, places children as immature beings on their journeys to adulthood, with implications for how we research and engage with children and caregivers, and what we consider as optimal childrearing practices and environments. For example, in ‘Western’ countries such as the United Kingdom, United States, and Australia where such views are normative, children are typically viewed as being different to adults, and as highly sensitive to caregiver input and external environments [Reference Van der Voort, Juffer and Bakermans-Kranenburg33Reference Garlen35]. Research findings recommend sensitive parenting practices that focus on understanding and responding to the child’s needs without frightening the child, and such parenting practices are argued to be crucial in constructing secure attachments to the caregiver, and a foundation for ‘good development’ across a range of socio-emotional and behavioural outcomes [Reference Van der Voort, Juffer and Bakermans-Kranenburg33]. Consequently, there are social expectations to create child-centred, developmentally appropriate, and stimulating environments that are typically age specific and separated from the adult world, allowing ‘children to be children’ [Reference Van der Voort, Juffer and Bakermans-Kranenburg33Reference Budds36]. For instance, caregivers in England are encouraged to ‘read and look at books together’ because ‘it will help [children] with their future learning … allows you to bond with them and is good for emotional wellbeing’ [37], informed by evidence that ‘books serve as inputs to influence an infant’s visual, social and linguistic development’ [Reference Krishnan and Johnson32]. Here, the act of reading books together is framed as a scientifically recommended extra activity caregivers can carry out for the infant, independent of their day-to-day ‘adult activities’, which serves as a developmentally appropriate stimulus to encourage ‘good development’.

However, not all cultures share the view that childhood is foundationally a time of growth, conflicting with the dominant notions of ‘early life’ in contemporary public health, lifecourse, and development literature [Reference Lachman38]. For example, Helen Kavapalu [Reference Kavapalu39] describes how concepts of childhood in Tonga stemmed from how ‘children are perceived as inherently vale (foolish, ignorant, “crazy”)’ where socially undesirable characteristics such as laziness, aggression, and disobedience were viewed as ‘natural’. Punishment, including corporal, was commonly used by caregivers to remove such traits and instil socially desirable traits – a form of caregiving practice viewed as harmful, outdated, and often outlawed in high-income nations. In Tonga, however, ‘good development’ is dependent on removing traits, contrasting with the idea that development is necessarily a process of cumulative growth, with caregiver punishment being an effective tool to guide children away from socially undesirable characteristics.

Broadly, the belief that development requires removal of traits is not necessarily an incorrect one, from what might be seen as a traditional science perspective. Removal of traits occurs throughout childhood, with processes such as synaptic pruning (a process of development in the nervous system that eliminates synapses) being a core aspect of brain development and possibly later health [Reference Sakai40]. Humans also possess a range of ontogenetic adaptations that are traits with specific functions for specific timepoints in development, to address the challenges associated with childhood and adolescence [Reference Bjorklund, Weekes-Shackelford, Shackelford and Weekes-Shackelford41]. These traits may disappear before adulthood and are not immature versions of adult traits. A simple example is the newborn rooting reflex, where neonates turn their heads with an open mouth when touched around their cheek or mouth; a trait that disappears during infancy. High self-efficacy in early childhood, where young children aged 3–4 years tend to overestimate their abilities, has also been argued to be an ontogenetic adaptation to facilitate exploration and engagement with the external environment [Reference Reed, Duncan, Lucier-Greer, Fixelle and Ferraro42]. If focusing on these characteristics of development, children could be conceptualised as ‘full beings’ who are perfectly adapted to their socio-ecological niche of childhood.

The view that children are ‘full beings’ has been documented across cultural contexts outside of the Western world, reported by many anthropologists including Margaret Mead who described Samoans as viewing ‘children as little adults’ [Reference Mead12]. Reframing children as ‘full beings’ somewhat conflicts with the Western focus on early life as an immature period of growth – with potential implications for what might be considered as an optimal developmental environment. In contrast to the child-centred approach frequently championed in the West, Samoan children were not provided with tailored age-specific, developmentally appropriate environments: when describing their play, Mead states, ‘For dolls they have real babies; at six they are expected to sweep up the real house and pick all the scraps off the floor’ [Reference Mead12]. In Samoa, therefore, ‘children being children’ did not require children to be removed from the adult world; they were assumed to be competent in carrying out specific tasks, and play was incorporated into everyday life. Full societal participation was seen as key for children to develop the skills and knowledge they required for the future, meaning the child-centred approach promoted in the West may be seen as a poor caregiving practice.

Overall, the concept of early life as a period of growth, dominant in Western contexts, influences how we construct optimal childrearing practices and environments. The focus on growth frames children as immature and sensitive, reflected in the promotion of child-centred sensitive parenting practices. However, in cultural contexts where early life is not strongly equated to be a period of growth and immaturity, there may be fundamental schematic conflicts, particularly around removing children from the adult world. While evidence shows sensitive parenting leads to ‘good development’ in the West [Reference Van der Voort, Juffer and Bakermans-Kranenburg33], it does not come without cost: assuming children are immature and vulnerable can encourage containment of children within developmentally appropriate and safe environments, which may limit their freedoms, agency, and social participation, which in itself impacts their learning and development [Reference Christensen34, Reference Garlen35]. The presumed significance of caregiver input for ‘good development’ may lead to excessive and intrusive caregiving, or helicopter parenting, which has been associated with poorer mental and physical health outcomes for adolescents and young adults [Reference Reed, Duncan, Lucier-Greer, Fixelle and Ferraro42]. It may also overburden common caregivers such as mothers, with the emergence of intensive mothering cultures that have implications for their health and well-being [Reference Budds36].

20.4 How Concepts of Early Life Impact Research

We have seen above how concepts of childhood and development impact how caregivers interact with and construct environments around children and how in turn these conceptual framings themselves impact practices of child development. We suggest that concepts of early life also inform research and subsequently the understanding of the DOHaD. Here, we examine this hypothesis by using longitudinal birth cohort studies as a paradigmatic broad terrain of research on early childhood and the lifecourse. Analysis of how early life research is framed and situated theoretically and methodologically in birth cohort studies further illuminates how cultural framings of early development shape research practices. We suggest that critical reflection on how this terrain of research on early life is culturally constituted within birth cohort studies may also help to inform future directions for biosocial research.

Longitudinal birth cohort studies that follow the social and biological aspects of people’s lives have been an important methodological tool for different research communities, mainly epidemiological, for over 60 years [Reference Lawlor, Andersen and Batty43]. These studies have been particularly useful for understanding developmental patterns and causal pathways [Reference Bynner and Joshi44], contributing to the DOHaD knowledge/evidence base. In this sense, birth cohort studies serve as a resource for and are also a ‘technology of’ biosocial research [Reference Gibbon and Pentecost45]. Recently, there has been an explosion of interest in birth cohort studies, with renewed efforts to maintain existing cohorts and new birth cohorts being established in many national contexts [Reference Wijmenga and Zhernakova46]). Detailed historical records that track the social context of intergenerational lives, while not always necessarily formulated as birth cohort studies as such, have been equally important. With public health and child development often underpinned by DOHaD frameworks, birth cohorts and other longitudinal studies have fuelled and facilitated an intense research focus on the ‘early life stage’ of pregnancy, infancy, and childhood and now also encompass the preconception period [Reference Pentecost and Meloni47]. In turn, a range of ‘life stages’ have become ‘critical windows’ for public and global health interventions, with early interventions championed for their preventative approach and effectiveness [Reference Krishnan and Johnson32, Reference Bruder48]. As Lappé and Landecker [Reference Lappé, Hein and Landecker49] point out, specific environments have become foregrounded in postgenomic and biosocial research, with consequences for how different stages of the lifecourse, including childhood and parenting, are temporally situated as being relevant to health. In this way, birth cohort research contributes not only to new temporal framings of the lifecourse but also to its explicit periodisation, suggesting discrete and definable life stages.

While there is today an intensive contemporary research focus on early life, its environment, and its consequences, this is arguably far from comprehensive. Birth cohort studies do allow researchers to better understand the biological and social determinants of development, including in childhood development and also across the lifecourse of participants [Reference Bynner and Joshi44]. Nevertheless, these studies have traditionally focused on limited aspects of the childrearing environment, almost exclusively focusing on the ‘microsystem’ within the ecological system (i.e. the immediate environment experienced by the child) [Reference Bronfenbrenner50], and in particular relying on the concept of the nuclear family household. For example, the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children [Reference Boyd, Golding, Macleod, Lawlor, Fraser and Henderson51] reveals an impressively detailed account of parenting and the household environment, including the pets that were owned by the families of birth cohort participants, how much toothpaste was put on the toothbrush, and when children first ate a raw carrot. However, there is surprisingly little information from beyond the household, such as who children see outside of this social context and what activities they do with them. This household focus persists in recent British birth cohorts such as the Millennium Cohort Study [Reference Connelly and Platt52], which continue to hold limited information on how families and children interact with potentially important caregivers such as grandparents and even siblings of cohort participants, who are not always included in such studies.

The prioritisation of the microsystem and the household arguably stems from biases in what is valued as important aspects of the childrearing environment, including by the DOHaD and birth cohort research community, with research focus (and funding) directed towards these topics. To date, researching ‘early life’ has been heavily influenced by norms such as intensive mothering and the perception that two-parent nuclear families are the ‘default’ family structure [Reference Budds36, Reference Emmott, Myers and Page53]. However, it has long been established that the environments beyond the household matter [Reference Connelly and Platt52], including non-parental caregivers who are essential in the human childrearing system [Reference Emmott, Myers and Page2, Reference Emmott, Myers and Page53]. Disciplinary silos, with their own traditions and theories, not only limit the understanding of DOHaD but may also introduce monocultural biases [Reference Emmott, Myers and Page53] and perpetuate an ongoing tendency for dyadic thinking in foregrounding parent (mostly mother) and child relations [Reference Pentecost, Ross and Macnab54]. We suggest therefore that biosocial collaborations require critical reflection on how early life and childhood environments are culturally framed and examined in research contexts such as longitudinal birth cohorts, including how this may vary depending on histories and genealogies that shape systems of public health, concepts of the biosocial, and the emergence and evolution of birth cohorts in diverse national contexts. Understanding how norms and assumptions are built into research on early life is the first step in both challenging these normative parameters and evolving new approaches that can more fully address both diversity and variability in childhood development.

20.5 Conclusion

The examples above evidence how societal views of childhood and development have implications for childrearing practices, and in turn, understanding children’s developmental outcomes in DOHaD research, outlining how biology and society may interact to shape the DOHaD. Further, our cross-disciplinary and cross-cultural examinations show how our understanding of DOHaD is influenced by the meaning of childhood and development. This also has consequences for the staging and framing of the current intense focus on early life and childhood in birth cohort studies, raising many questions about what ‘good’ development in childhood looks like and challenging the idea of this process as necessarily linear and additive.

Cross-disciplinary research initiatives such as the Biosocial Birth Cohort Research (BBCR) network (https://bbcrnetwork.com) provide an important infrastructure for widening the frame of research in DOHaD on child development. They also help create contexts for collaboration, such as that between our own sub-disciplinary expertise of biological and medical anthropology. Such collaborations while nascent and in dynamic formation also lead to new research questions and challenges. This includes other dimensions of a biosocial approach that we have not been able to fully address in this chapter and that also need further elaboration through more detailed, reflexive, and engaged cross-disciplinary dialogue. Exactly how the cultural politics of childhood are variously invoked and contested in the intense focus on this stage of the lifecourse in birth cohort studies and in the way that DOHaD is implemented in public and global health are just some of the areas for future investigation. Similarly how the figure of the child and childhood continues to symbolically represent future promise in these contexts is, as yet, relatively underexamined. Integrating analysis of the wider institutional contexts of research cultures (including funding priorities) that are manifest in and help sustain the infrastructure for DOHaD-focused and birth cohort studies would also further expand the scope of critical reflexive engagement. DOHaD research has much to gain from viewing developmental processes that shape childhood and health outcomes in highly context-specific ways; an understanding that is both underlined and strengthened through cross-disciplinary dialogues, such as those with biosocial anthropology outlined here.

References

Harris, KM, McDade, TW. The Biosocial Approach to Human Development, Behavior, and Health Across the Life Course. RSF. 2018 Apr;4(4):226.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Emmott, EH, Myers, S, Page, AE. Who Cares for Women with Children? Crossing the Bridge between Disciplines. Philosophical Transactions on Royal Society London B Biological Science. 2021/05/03 ed. 2021 21 Jun;376(1827):20200019–20200019.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fuentes, A. The Extended Evolutionary Synthesis, Ethnography, and the Human Niche. Current Anthropology. 2016 1 Jun;57(S13):S1326.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodman, AH, Leatherman, TL. Building a New Biocultural Synthesis: Political-Economic Perspectives on Human Biology. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press; 1998.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Singer, M, Bulled, N, Ostrach, B, Mendenhall, E. Syndemics and the Biosocial Conception of Health. The Lancet. 2017 Mar;389(10072):941–50.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lock, M. Mutable Environments and Permeable Human Bodies. Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute. 2018 20 Jun;24(3):449–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roberts, EFS. Bio-Ethnography: A Collaborative, Methodological Experiment in Mexico City. Somatosphere. 2015 26 Feb. Available from: http://somatosphere.net/2015/bio-ethnography.html/Google Scholar
Gibbon, S, Daly, L, Parkhurst, A, Ryan, C, Salali, GD, Tasker, A. Biosocial Medical Anthropology in the Time of Covid-19: New Challenge and Opportunities. London: UCL; 2020. Available from: https://medanthucl.com/2020/04/29/biosocial-medical-anthropology-in-the-time-of-covid-19-new-challenges-and-opportunities/Google Scholar
Alvergne, A, Jenkinson, C, Faurie, C. Evolutionary Thinking in Medicine: From Research to Policy and Practice. New York: Springer; 2016.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
La Fontaine, J. An Anthropological Perspective on Children in Social Worlds. In: Richards, M, Light, P, editors. Children of Social Worlds: Development in a Social Context. Cambridge: Polity; 1986. pp. 1030.Google Scholar
Boas, F. Instability of Human Types. In: Spiller, G, editor. Papers on Interracial Problems. Boston, MA: Ginn and Company; 1912. p. 99103.Google Scholar
Mead, M. Coming of Age in Samoa. A Psychological Study of Primitive Youth for Western Civilisation. New York: William Morrow Paperback; 1971 [1928].Google Scholar
Benedict, R. Continuities and Discontinuities in Cultural Conditioning. Psychiatry. 1938 May;1(2):161–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
LeVine, RA, New, RS. Anthropology and Child Development: A Cross-Cultural Reader. Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell; 2008.Google Scholar
Lancy, DF, Bock, J, Gaskins, S. The Anthropology of Learning in Childhood. Walnut Creek: AltaMira Press; 2010.Google Scholar
Lancy, DF. Why Anthropology of Childhood? A short history of an emerging discipline. AnthropoChildren: French Studies in the Anthropology of Childhood. 2012 Jan; 1(1). Available from: http://popups.ulg.ac.be/AnthropoChildren/document.php?id=918Google Scholar
Levine, RA. Ethnographic Studies of Childhood: A Historical Overview. American Anthropologist. 2007 Jun;109(2):247–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Montgomery, H. An Introduction to Childhood: Anthropological Perspectives on Children’s Lives. Malden: Wiley-Blackwell; 2009.Google Scholar
Giele, J, Elder, G. Methods of Life Course Research: Qualitative and Quantitative Approaches. 1998; Available from: http://dx.doi.org/10.4135/9781483348919CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Malinowski, B. Argonauts of the Western Pacific: An Account of Native Enterprise and Adventure in the Archipelagoes of Melanesian New Guinea, Enhanced Edition. Oxford: Routledge and Kegan Paul; 1922.Google Scholar
Richards, A. Chisungu, a Girls’ Initiation Ceremony among the Bemba of Northern Rhodesia. London: Faber & Faber; 1956.Google Scholar
Bluebond-Langer, M. The Private Worlds of Dying Children. Princeton: Princeton University Press; 1978.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hardman, C. Can There Be an Anthropology of Children? Childhood. 2001 Nov;8(4):501–17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reynolds, P. ‘Not Known Because Not Looked for’: Ethnographers Listening to the Young in Southern Africa. Ethnos. 1995 Jan;60(3–4):193221.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ross, FC, Pentecost, M. Still Unknown and Overlooked? Anthropologies of Childhood and Infancy in Southern Africa, 1995–2020. Ethnos. 2021 1 Nov;1–22. 10.1080/00141844.2021.1994622CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keller, H. Universality Claim of Attachment Theory: Children’s Socioemotional Development across Cultures. Proceedings of the National Academy Science USA. 2018 6 Nov;115(45):11414–19.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
LeVine, RA. Attachment Theory as Cultural Ideology. In: Otto, H, Keller, H, eds. Different Faces of Attachment: Cultural Variations on a Universal Human Need. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press; 2014. pp. 50-65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Emmott, EH, Page, AE. Alloparenting. In: Shackelford, TK, Weekes-Shackelford, VA, editors. Encyclopedia of Evolutionary Psychological Science [Internet]. Cham: Springer International Publishing; 2019 [cited 9 Jun 2023]. pp. 114. Available from: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-16999-6_2253–1Google Scholar
Larcher, V. Children Are Not Small Adults: Significance of Biological and Cognitive Development in Medical Practice. In: Schramme, T, Edwards, S, editors. Handbook of the Philosophy of Medicine [Internet]. Dordrecht: Springer Netherlands; 2015 [cited 9 Jun 2023]. pp. 123. Available from: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-017-8706-2_16–1Google Scholar
Child Growth Standards [Internet]. WHO. 2005 [cited 9 Jun 2023]. Available from: www.who.int/news-room/questions-and-answers/item/child-growth-standardsGoogle Scholar
Singh, A, Yeh, CJ, Boone, Blanchard S. Ages and Stages Questionnaire: A Global Screening Scale. Boletín Médico del Hospital Infantil de México (English Edition). 2017 Jan;74(1):512.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krishnan, S, Johnson, MH. A Review of Behavioural and Brain Development in the Early Years: The “Toolkit” for Later Book-related Skills [Internet]. Booktrust. 2014. [cited 11 Apr 2022]. Available from: www.booktrust.org.uk/globalassets/resources/research/krishnan--johnson-2014-full-report-a-review-of-behavioural-and-brain-development-in-the-early-years-the-toolkit-for-later-book-related-skills-.pdfGoogle Scholar
Van der Voort, A, Juffer, FJ, Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. Sensitive Parenting Is the Foundation for Secure Attachment Relationships and Positive Social-Emotional Development of Children. Journal of Children’s Services. 2014 10 Jun;9(2):165–76.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Christensen, PH. Childhood and the cultural constitution of vulnerable bodies. In The Body, Childhood and Society. London: Palgrave Macmillan UK. 2000;3859.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garlen, JC. Interrogating Innocence: ‘Childhood’ as Exclusionary Social Practice. Childhood. 2018 22 Nov;26(1):5467.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Budds, K. Validating Social Support and Prioritizing Maternal Wellbeing: Beyond Intensive Mothering and Maternal Responsibility. Philosophical Transactions on Royal Society London B Biological Science. 2021/05/03 ed. 2021 21 Jun;376(1827):20200029–20200029.Google Scholar
Baby and Toddler Play Ideas [Internet]. nhs.uk. 2020 [cited 9 Jun 2023]. Available from: www.nhs.uk/conditions/baby/babys-development/play-and-learning/baby-and-toddler-play-ideas/Google Scholar
Lachman, et al., this volume.Google Scholar
Kavapalu, H. Dealing with the Dark Side in the Ethnography of Childhood: Child Punishment in Tonga. Oceania. 1993 Jun;63(4):313–29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sakai, J. Core Concept: How Synaptic Pruning Shapes Neural Wiring during Development and, Possibly, in Disease. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science U S A. 2020/06/24 ed. 2020 14 Jul;117(28):16096–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bjorklund, DF. Ontogenetic Adaptations. In: Weekes-Shackelford, V, Shackelford, TK, Weekes-Shackelford, VA, editors. Encyclopedia of Evolutionary Psychological Science [Internet]. Cham: Springer International Publishing; 2016 [cited 9 Jun 2023]. pp. 13. Available from: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-16999-6_2388–1Google Scholar
Reed, K, Duncan, JM, Lucier-Greer, M, Fixelle, C, Ferraro, AJ. Helicopter Parenting and Emerging Adult Self-Efficacy: Implications for Mental and Physical Health. Journal of Child and Family Studies. 2016 6 Jun;25(10):3136–49.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lawlor, DA, Andersen, AMN, Batty, GD. Birth Cohort Studies: Past, Present and Future. International Journal of Epidemiology. 2009 26 Jun;38(4):897902.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bynner, J, Joshi, H. Building the Evidence Base from Longitudinal Data. Innovation: The European Journal of Social Science Research. 2007 Jun;20(2):159–79.Google Scholar
Gibbon, S, Pentecost, M. Excavating and (Re)creating the Biosocial: Birth Cohorts as Ethnographic Objects of Inquiry and Sites of Intervention. Somatosphere. 2019 14 Nov. Available from: http://somatosphere.net/2019/introduction-excavating-and-recreating-the-biosocial-birth-cohorts-as-ethnographic-object-of-inquiry-and-site-of-intervention.html/ [Accessed 01/01/22]Google Scholar
Wijmenga, C, Zhernakova, A. The Importance of Cohort Studies in the Post-GWAS Era. Nature Genetics. 2018 Mar;50(3):322–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pentecost, M, Meloni, M. “It’s Never Too Early”: Preconception Care and Postgenomic Models of Life. Frontiers in Sociology. 2020 21 Apr;5:2121:322–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bruder, MB. Early Childhood Intervention: A Promise to Children and Families for Their Future. Exceptional Children. 2010 Apr;76(3):339–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lappé, M, Hein, RJ, Landecker, H. Environmental Politics of Reproduction. Annu Rev Anthropol. 2019 Oct;48:133–50.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bronfenbrenner, U. Ecological Systems Theory (1992). In: Making Human Beings Human: Bioecological Perspectives on Human Development. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications Ltd; 2005. pp. 106–73. 10.1093/ije/dys064Google Scholar
Boyd, A, Golding, J, Macleod, J, Lawlor, DA, Fraser, A, Henderson, J, et al. Cohort Profile: The ‘Children of the 90s’ – The Index Offspring of the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children. International Journal of Epidemiology 2013 Feb;42(1):111–27.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Connelly, R, Platt, L. Cohort Profile: UK Millennium Cohort Study (MCS). International Journal of Epidemiology. 2014 17 Feb;43(6):1719–25.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Emmott, EH, Myers, S, Page, AE. Who Cares for Women with Children? Crossing the Bridge between Disciplines. Philosophical Transactions on Royal Society London B Biological Science. 2021 21 Jun;376(1827):20200019–20200019.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pentecost, M, Ross, FC, Macnab, A. Beyond the Dyad: Making Developmental Origins of Health and Disease (DOHaD) Interventions More Inclusive. Journal of Developmental Origins of Health and Disease. 2018 Feb;9(1):1014.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure no-reply@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×