Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-7cvxr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T03:19:43.945Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Cumulative risk and adolescent emotional distress: A longitudinal moderated mediation analysis focusing on perceived stress and social support

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  19 September 2024

Emre Deniz*
Affiliation:
Department of Education, University of York, York, UK
Neil Humphrey
Affiliation:
Manchester Institute of Education, University of Manchester, Manchester, UK
Ola Demkowicz
Affiliation:
Manchester Institute of Education, University of Manchester, Manchester, UK
Suzet Tanya Lereya
Affiliation:
Evidence Based Practice Unit (EBPU), University College London and Anna Freud National Centre for Children and Families, London, UK
Jessica Deighton
Affiliation:
Evidence Based Practice Unit (EBPU), University College London and Anna Freud National Centre for Children and Families, London, UK
*
Corresponding author: Emre Deniz; Email: emre.deniz@york.ac.uk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

This study provides insights into the roles played by perceived stress and social support in the relationship between cumulative risk exposure (CRE) and adolescent emotional distress. Preregistered longitudinal moderated mediation analyses were used to test hypotheses relating to the association between CRE and later emotional distress; the mediating role of perceived stress in the relationship between CRE and later emotional distress; and, the moderating effects of peer and adult-level family support on the relationship between CRE and later perceived stress, among N = 19,159 adolescents over three annual waves (at ages 11/12, 12/13, 13/14). Analyses revealed that CRE significantly predicted later adolescent emotional distress. This relationship was partially mediated by perceived stress. Both peer and adult-level family support significantly moderated the impact of CRE on later perceived stress (i.e., adolescents reporting higher levels of support perceived significantly lower levels of stress resulting from CRE compared to those reporting lower levels of support). These findings provide critical empirical evidence of the roles played by perceived stress and social support in the relationship between CRE and adolescent emotional distress, with consequent implications for intervention.

Type
Regular Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press

Approximately one in five 11-to-16-year-olds have a probable mental health disorder (Newlove-Delgado et al., Reference Newlove-Delgado, Marcheselli, Williams, Mandalia, Davis, McManus, Savic, Treloar and Ford2022), with those reflecting emotional distress (e.g., anxiety, depression) being the most prevalent in this period (NHS Digital, 2018). Half of all lifetime cases begin in adolescence, with a peak age of onset of 14.5 years (Solmi et al., Reference Solmi, Radua, Olivola, Croce, Soardo, Salazar de Pablo, Il Shin, Kirkbride, Jones, Kim, Kim, Carvalho, Seeman, Correll and Fusar-Poli2021). Mental health disorders impair quality of life and are concurrently and prospectively associated with a range of maladaptive outcomes (e.g., lower academic attainment) (Humphrey, Reference Humphrey2018). There is therefore an urgent need to better understand the processes and mechanisms that underpin or counteract their development. Drawing on a very large longitudinal dataset (“HeadStart”) with three annual datapoints that span the transition from early- to mid-adolescence (T1, age 11/12; T2, age 12/13; T3, aged 13/14), this paper makes a significant contribution to knowledge by testing a series of propositions that underpin cumulative risk theory, before drawing upon resilience theory to examine the extent to which access to putative protective factors pertaining to social support can disrupt the process through which cumulative risk impacts later emotional distress.

Cumulative risk theory: the whole is greater than the sum of its parts

Risk factors are measurable characteristics (e.g., socioeconomic disadvantage) that are associated with a negative outcome (e.g., mental health difficulties). Since Rutter’s pioneering Isle of Wight studies (Rutter et al., Reference Rutter, Tizard, Yule, Graham and Whitmore1976), significant attention has been paid to the impact of exposure to multiple risk factors situated across different levels of the developmental ecosystem (e.g., individual, family, peer group, and community). In the current study, we draw on two key tenets of cumulative risk theory. First, the accumulation principle states that the more risk factors to which an individual is exposed, the greater the probability of maladaptive outcomes. Second, the number-over-nature principle states that it is the number of risk factors, as opposed to their nature, which is the central driver of said outcomes (Evans et al., Reference Evans, Li and Whipple2013). Drawing these two tenets together, we hypothesize that cumulative risk exposure (CRE) at T1 will predict increased emotional distress at T3, above and beyond each individual risk factor (Hypothesis 1). The evidence base to support these predictions is substantial, though there is a need for more longitudinal work (to establish temporal precedence), and in particular, research that can elucidate the proposed mechanisms and processes that underpin the relationship between CRE and maladaptive outcomes (Evans et al., Reference Evans, Li and Whipple2013).

Another key tenet of cumulative risk theory is the hypothesis that risk factors interact and potentiate one another to impact on allostatic load, which is a marker of “wear and tear” on the body, produced by the mobilization of physiological systems in response to environmental stressors. Increased and recurrent exposure to stressors results in sustained production of the stress hormone, cortisol, which in turn shifts operating parameters of immune, neurological, metabolic, and endocrine biomarkers outside of their optimal range, leading to dysfunction (Evans et al., Reference Evans, Li and Whipple2013). These changes are proposed to alter neurological activity, underpinning the onset of disorder (Evans et al., Reference Evans, Kim, Ting, Tesher and Shannis2007). However, despite growing support for the theory that stress has a key role in how risk factors influence outcomes, our understanding of such mechanisms is still developing, particularly with regard to CRE. Existing evidence is often reliant on physiological biomarkers of stress and allostatic load (e.g., blood pressure, neuroendocrine stress activity) (Guidi et al., Reference Guidi, Lucente, Sonino and Fava2020). Such studies have not investigated mediation pathways in youth mental health and tend to have small sample sizes, given the high time and resource demand of measuring biomarkers. Further, through a focus on physiological data, they overlook the largely subjective nature of stress, as captured in the transactional model, wherein there are substantive individual differences in the extent to which people appraise an event, or their lives more generally, to be stressful (Cohen, Reference Cohen1986; Lazarus & Folkman, Reference Lazarus and Folkman1984). Thus, there is considerable value in further investigation of the role of perceived stress in the relationship between CRE and adolescent mental health outcomes. Promisingly, our initial evidence examining baseline data from the HeadStart dataset – albeit cross-sectional and focusing only on early adolescent girls – indicates that perceived stress may mediate this relationship (Demkowicz, Reference Demkowicz2019).

Based on the above, and building on Hypothesis 1, we make a further prediction, namely that increased levels of perceived stress at T2 will significantly mediate the association between CRE at T1 and emotional distress at T3, even after controlling for prior (T1) levels of emotional distress and co-variates (Hypothesis 2).

Fostering resilience: social support as ordinary magic

A key tenet of cumulative risk theory is that exposure increases the probability of disorder but does not guarantee it (Evans et al., Reference Evans, Li and Whipple2013). Thus, some adolescents will demonstrate positive adaptation despite being exposed to multiple risk factors. This brings us to the concept of resilience, defined as, “the capacity of a dynamic system to adapt successfully to disturbances that threaten system function, viability, or development” (Masten, Reference Masten2014, p. 10). Various models have been proposed to explain the process(es) through which healthy development can occur in the face of risk (e.g., compensatory, protective, innoculation; Fergus & Zimmerman, Reference Fergus and Zimmerman2005). In the current study, we draw directly on the protective model, in which access to putative protective factors is theorized to moderate the impact of CRE on later emotional distress (Masten, Reference Masten2014). More specifically, we anticipate a protective-reactive effect, in which a given protective factor diminishes, but does not completely eradicate, the expected association between risk exposure and a given negative outcome (Fergus & Zimmerman, Reference Fergus and Zimmerman2005). This assumption is based on the principle of equifinality (i.e., multiple routes to the same outcome, meaning no single protective factor in isolation can completely suppress the impact of risk exposure; Cicchetti & Rogosch, Reference Cicchetti and Rogosch1996) and the related proposition that protective factors can operate in tandem to disrupt the risk-outcome relationship (Leffert et al., Reference Leffert, Benson, Scales, Sharma, Drake and Blyth1998).

In her seminal work, Masten (Reference Masten2014) argues that resilience occurs through ordinary rather than extraordinary processes (i.e., “ordinary magic”). Using this as our starting point, we focus on a very ordinary process, social support, by which we mean, “the social resources that persons perceive to be available or that are actually provided to them by non-professionals in the context of both formal support groups and informal helping relationships” (Gottlieb & Bergen, Reference Gottlieb and Bergen2010, p. 512). More specifically, we examine the protective effects of peer and adult-level family support. Our rationale for this focus is threefold. First, heightened sensitivity to social stimuli is recognized as a characteristic feature of adolescence (Orben et al., Reference Orben, Tomova and Blakemore2020); consequently, social support may have particular utility during this period. Second, with specific reference to early adolescence, family adult and peer support are both highly salient; the former is seen as a critical source of interpersonal connection in the transition from childhood, with the latter becoming increasingly important over time as the social world is reoriented in service of development towards independence (Orben et al., Reference Orben, Tomova and Blakemore2020; Umberson et al., Reference Umberson, Crosnoe and Reczek2010). Third, our work reflects a broader effort in resilience research to shift the onus away from internal factors (which arguably place the onus on the individual to be responsible for their own resilience) onto external factors within the developmental ecosystem (Demkowicz, Reference Demkowicz2019; Ungar et al., Reference Ungar, Ghazinour and Richter2013).

In thinking about how these two putative protective factors may modify the effects of CRE on later emotional distress, we draw on Cohen and Wills’ stress buffering hypothesis (Reference Cohen and Wills1985), in which it is argued that social support functions to improve wellbeing by lessening the extent to which significant life challenges are experienced as stressful. As above, our initial evidence – albeit cross-sectional and focusing only on early adolescent girls – provides support for this proposition (Demkowicz, Reference Demkowicz2019). Based on the above, and building on Hypotheses 1 and 2, we make two final predictions. First, peer support at T2 will significantly moderate the effects of CRE at T1 on perceived stress at T2 (Hypothesis 3). Second, adult-level family support at T2 will significantly moderate the effects of CRE at T1 on perceived stress at T2 (Hypothesis 4).

The current study

In the current study, our aim is to provide much needed insights into the processes and mechanisms underpinning the relationship between CRE and adolescent emotional distress via a preregistered longitudinal moderated mediation analysis focusing on perceived stress and social support. To reiterate and summarize, our hypotheses are as follows (see also conceptual diagram, Figure 1):

Figure 1. Proposed moderated mediation model.

  • H1: CRE at T1 will predict increased emotional distress at T3 above and beyond each significant individual risk factor;

  • H2: Perceived stress at T2 will significantly mediate the association between CRE at T1 and emotional distress at T3, even after controlling for prior (T1) levels of emotional distress and co-variates;

  • H3: Peer support at T2 will significantly moderate the effects of CRE at T1 on perceived stress at T2;

  • H4: Adult-level family support at T2 will significantly moderate the effects of CREe at T1 on perceived stress at T2.

Note: H3 and H4 are tested through simultaneous interaction (i.e., both protective factors operating at once to moderate the effects of CRE at T1 on perceived stress at T2).

Method

Design

Secondary data analysis of the longitudinal cohort from the HeadStart study was undertaken, using the first three annual data points (T1, T2, and T3) and linked administrative data. HeadStart was a six-year project that aimed to explore and test new ways to improve the mental health and wellbeing of adolescents aged 10–16 years (for further details, see Evidence-Based Practice Unit, 2023). Ethical approval for HeadStart data collection was granted by University College London’s ethics committee (reference 8097/003). The design and analysis plan for this study was preregistered prior to the lead analyst begin granted access to the data (Deniz et al. Reference Deniz, Humphrey, Demkowicz, Lereya and Deighton2023).

Sample

The study sample comprised N = 19,159 adolescents (Mean age at T1 = 12.22, SD = .30), drawn from 113 secondary schools in six Local Authorities across England. Of these, 77% were White; 52% were girls; 14% had at least one special educational need (SEN); 37% were eligible for free school meals (FSM); 27% were living in a socioeconomically deprived neighborhood; 26% were summer born; and 6% were currently or had previously been classified as a child in need (i.e., they had been referred to social services). Selection of Local Authorities in the HeadStart project was purposive and not designed to be representative. Accordingly, the study sample were subject to somewhat higher levels of socioeconomic disadvantage than is seen nationally across England.

Measures

Cumulative risk exposure

Pragmatic selection of candidate risk factors that were available in the HeadStart survey data or linked administrative data was undertaken, informed by the extant evidence base (e.g., Evans et al., Reference Evans, Li and Whipple2013), and using the aforementioned definition (i.e., a measurable characteristic that is associated with a negative outcome). We also drew on the work of the World Health Organization (2012) and others (e.g., Furber et al., Reference Furber, Leach, Guy and Segal2017), in which risk factors can include individual attributes and characteristics, not just exposures.

A CRE index was derived from the following candidate risk factor (RF) variables: gender (RF = girl); relative age (RF = summer born); bullying victimization (RF = bullied by other adolescents); academic attainment (RF = lowest quartile in prior attainment); SEN (RF = identified as having SEN); familial socioeconomic deprivation (RF = currently or previously eligible for FSM in the last 6 years); caregiving responsibilities (RF = has caregiving responsibilities); child in need status (RF = current or previous social services referral); neighborhood socioeconomic disadvantage (RF = resident in Index of Multiple Deprivation Quintile 1 neighborhood). Further articulation of these risk factors is presented in Table 1, including indicative evidence and theorization.

Table 1. Candidate risk factors: indicative evidence and theorization

Peer and adult-level family support

The Student Resilience Survey (SRS; Lereya et al., Reference Lereya, Humphrey, Patalay, Wolpert, Böhnke, Macdougall and Deighton2016) was used to assess peer (13 items) and adult-level family support (4 items). Respondents read statements (e.g., “At home there is an adult who listens to me when I have something to say”; “Are there students at your school who would make you feel better if something is bothering you”) and endorse them on a 5-point scale (1 = never to 5 = always). In each subscale, a sum score is calculated, with higher scores indicating greater social support.

Perceived stress

Following guidance by Demkowicz et al. (Reference Demkowicz, Panayiotou, Ashworth, Humphrey and Deighton2020), we used the two stress-focused items from the four-item Perceived Stress Scale (PSS-4; Cohen et al., Reference Cohen, Kamarck and Mermelstein1983) in recognition that the full measure does not function well as a unidimensional construct, and that a two-factor structure is supported (one of which being perceived stress, used in the current study). Respondents read statements (e.g., “In the last month, how often have you felt that you were unable to control the important things in your life”) and endorse them on a five-point scale (never, almost never, sometimes, often, very often). As above, a sum score is calculated, with higher scores indicating higher levels of perceived stress.

Emotional distress

The emotional symptoms subscale (5 items) of the Strength and Difficulties Questionnaire (Goodman, Reference Goodman1997) was used to assess emotional distress. Respondents read statements (e.g., “I worry a lot”) and endorse them on a three-point scale (not true, somewhat true, certainly true). As above, a sum score is calculated, with higher scores indicating higher levels of emotional distress.

Ethnicity

A binary ethnicity variable (White; minority ethnicity), derived from linked administrative data, was controlled as a covariate of both perceived stress (T2) and emotional distress (T3). Our intention here was to partial out variance accounted for by ethnicity, given the well-established evidence which indicates that it co-varies with our response variable (see for example Ahmad et al., Reference Ahmad, McManus, Bécares, Hatch and Das-Munshi2022; Bains & Gutman, Reference Bains and Gutman2021; Goodman et al., Reference Goodman, Patel and Leon2008).

Analytic strategy

All analyses were performed following the aforementioned preregistered statistical analysis plan and conducted in R version 4.2.1 (R Core Team, 2023). First, we performed point biserial correlations to report the zero-order correlations between the candidate risk factors (binary) and emotional distress at T3 (continuous). This step was taken so that each candidate risk factor could be coded in the same direction (i.e., for each, 0=absent, 1=present). Second, we constructed the CRE index by summing all candidate risk factors such that increased scores reflected increased CRE. Third, we fitted a structural equation model (SEM) to test the mediating role of stress (T2) in the association between CRE and later emotional distress (T3), controlling for ethnicity and prior levels of emotional distress (T1). Finally, we included adult-level family support and peer support as moderators of the associations between CRE and perceived stress to test the hypothesized moderated mediation model. Model fit indices such as the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA), comparative fit index (CFI), Tucker–Lewis index (TLI) were inspected and documented. The model was considered a good fit to the data if CFI and TLI ≥ .90 and RMSEA ≤ .08 (Hu & Bentler, Reference Hu and Bentler1999).

Missing data

In order to check that the missing at random (MAR) assumption was met, we generated a binary missingness variable which indicated whether an observation had any missing values in the focal variables. We then fitted a multiple logistic regression model to test whether missingness was conditional on observed variables in the dataset. This was found to be the case for several variables (i.e., ethnicity, FSM, SEN, academic attainment, and perceived stress), indicating that the MAR assumption was met. More information regarding the missingness pattern in the dataset can be found in Table S1 (Supplementary materials).

Given the above, multiple imputations by chained equations (MICE) and full information maximum likelihood (FIML) were used to handle missing data. MICE was used to report descriptive and regression analyses, while FIML was used to report SEM path coefficients.

Deviations from the analysis plan

As outlined earlier, the design and reporting of the current study followed a preregistered analysis plan. However, there was an unprecedented deviation from the registered analysis plan. More specifically, we initially hypothesized that stress at T2 would significantly mediate the associations between CRE at T1 and emotional distress at T3. However, in the main analysis, the proposed model did not fit the data well. Therefore, unpredictable model modification indices were applied to improve the model fit. This is a common practice in the SEM literature (MacCallum et al., Reference MacCallum, Roznowski and Necowitz1992) and is acceptable as far as the undertaken modifications are theoretically or empirically meaningful (Wang & Wang, Reference Wang and Wang2019). The first undertaken modification indices, suggested by R package modindices (R Core Team, 2023), was to add a path between emotional distress at T1 and stress at T2. This aligns well with the existing literature as previous researchers have suggested that emotional distress predicts later stress (Kim et al., Reference Kim, Conger, Elder and Lorenz2003; Phillips et al., Reference Phillips, Carroll and Der2015). Additionally, emotional distress at T1, which was a control covariate, was transformed to an observed variable (as opposed to latent), to reduce model complexity and improve fit. Arguably, treating the emotional distress subscale of the SDQ as an observed variable is acceptable in the sense that it has a well-constructed and repeatedly confirmed factor structure in this population (e.g., He et al., Reference He, Burstein, Schmitz and Merikangas2013; Van Roy et al., Reference Van Roy, Veenstra and Clench-Aas2008). Upon the application of these modifications, a good model fit was established.

Finally, two further substantive changes to our analyses were undertaken on the basis of peer review recommendations. First, we reconstructed our CRE index, which originally included only those candidate risk factors that were significantly associated (in the direction anticipated) with emotional distress at T3, to include them all irrespective of their association with the outcome variable. Second, we had originally included a series of predictions and preliminary analyses based on the classic mediation approach (i.e., testing individual paths prior to undertaking formal mediation analysis); these were excised following advice that they were no longer commonly applied in the contemporary literature.

Results

Constructing the CRE index

Prior to constructing the CRE index, we tested the zero-order correlations between candidate risk factors and emotional distress at T3. Zero order correlations showed that being a girl (corr = .34, p <, 001) and being a victim of bullying (corr = .19, p < .001) were the two strongest correlates of T3 emotional distress amongst all candidate risk factors. Additionally, relative age, familial socioeconomic deprivation, caregiving responsibilities, and child in need status were other significant correlates of emotional distress at T3. Interestingly, and unexpectedly, low academic attainment was negatively correlated with emotional distress at T3. For this variable to be coded in line with other risk factors, we therefore changed the risk factor designation from poor to high academic attainment (i.e., 0 = risk absent, low academic attainment; 1 = risk present, high academic attainment). Details can be found in Table 2.

Table 2. Point biserial correlations (zero-order correlations) between the individual risk factors and emotional distress at T3

Note. a: All regression coefficients are reported on the multiply imputed dataset. Risk factor designation: gender (RF = girl); relative age (RF = summer born); bullying victimization (RF = bullied by other adolescents); academic attainment (RF = lowest quartile in prior attainment); SEN (RF = identified as having SEN); familial socioeconomic deprivation (RF = currently or previously eligible for FSM in the last 6 years); caregiving responsibilities (RF = has caregiving responsibilities); child in need status (RF = current or previous social services referral); neighborhood socioeconomic disadvantage (RF = resident in Index of Multiple Deprivation Quintile 1 neighborhood).

The CRE index score originally ranged from 0 to 8 (i.e., no participants exposed to all nine candidate risk factors), with higher scores indicating higher risk exposure (N at each level of CRE: 0 = 1386; 1 = 4247; 2 = 5477; 3 = 4371; 4 = 2353; 5 = 1014; 6 = 255; 7 = 50; 8 = 6). Since the two highest levels (7 and 8) had very low power (N < 1%), we rescaled the CRE index to combine them with the next highest level (i.e., 0 = 1386; 1 = 4247; 2 = 5477; 3 = 4371; 4 = 2353; 5 = 1014; 6+ = 311), in line with both the preregistered analysis plan and standard practice in the cumulative risk literature (Appleyard et al., Reference Appleyard, Egeland, van Dulmen and Sroufe2005). We then fitted separate linear regression models to determine whether increased levels of CRE at T1 predicted increased emotional distress at T3, above and beyond each significant individual risk factor (see Table 3). As above, each regression model controlled for emotional distress and ethnicity at T1. In all cases, higher CRE scores significantly predicted increased levels of emotional distress at T3 above and beyond each candidate risk factor in question and covariates, confirming Hypothesis 1.

Table 3. Regression of emotional distress at T3 on to the cumulative risk exposure index, controlling for all individual risk factors and covariates (N = 19,159a)

Note. a: CRE is tested as predictor of emotional distress at T3 alongside each significant individual risk factor and controlling for covariates (emotional distress T1 and ethnicity), separately. These covariates are not shown in the table in the interest of brevity and clarity. All regression coefficients are reported on the multiply imputed dataset. CRE = Cumulative Risk Exposure.

Cumulative risk exposure, perceived stress, and emotional distress (hypothesis 2)

To test Hypothesis 2, we fitted the hypothesized mediation model. As noted earlier, the preregistered model did not fit well to the data (Goodness of Fit: x 2 (p) < .001, RMSEA = .16, CFI = .15, TLI = .10, SRMR = .56), so a number of modifications were undertaken (see “Deviations From The Analysis Plan”). The modified model (see Table 4) showed a good fit to the data (x 2 (p) < .001, df = 33, RMSEA = .040, CFI = .97, TLI = .95, SRMR = .021). In support of Hypothesis 2, we found that perceived stress at T2 significantly mediated the association between CRE at T1 and emotional distress at T3. More specifically, our model suggested that CRE at T1 significantly predicted stress at T2 (β = .17, p < .001) which, in turn, significantly predicted emotional distress at T3 (β = .17, p < .001). The direct effects of CRE at T1 (β = .10, p < .001) appeared to be significantly higher than its indirect effects through perceived stress (β = .03, p < .001). Hence, our mediation analysis confirmed Hypothesis 2, indicating that stress is a significant mediator in the association between CRE at T1 and emotional distress at T3.

Table 4. The mediating role of perceived stress at T2 between cumulative risk exposure at T1 and emotional distress at T3 (N = 19,159)

Note. Model Fit: RMSEA = .040, CFI = .97, TLI = .95, SRMR = .021, df = 33, x2 (p) < .001. ED = Emotional Distress, CRE = Cumulative Risk Exposure. Estimator: Full information maximum likelihood.

Peer and adult-level family support as moderators of the impact of cumulative risk exposure on perceived stress (hypotheses 6 and 7)

Our final model, moderated mediation model (Table 5 and Figure 2), fit the data well (x 2 (p) < .001, df = 45, RMSEA = .039, CFI = .96, TLI = .94, SRMR = .019). The model revealed that relations between CRE at T1 and perceived stress at T2 were significantly moderated by both peer (β = −.01, p < .001) and adult-level family support (β = −.03, p < .001) at T2. That is, confirming Hypotheses 3 and 4, adolescents reporting higher levels of peer and adult-level family support at T2 perceived significantly lower levels of stress resulting from CRE at T1 compared to those reporting lower levels of peer and adult-level family support at T2.

Figure 2. Reported moderated mediation model.

Table 5. Hypothesized moderated mediation (N = 18,979, missing not handled = 180)

Note. Model Fit: RMSEA = .039, CFI = .96 TLI = .94, SRMR = .019, df = 45, x2 (p) < .001. INTPS (mod1): Interaction Term Peer Support. INTFS (mod2): Interaction Term Family Support. ED = Emotional Distress, CRE = Cumulative Risk Exposure. Estimator: Full information maximum likelihood.

Discussion

In the current study, preregistered longitudinal moderated mediation analyses were used to test a series of hypotheses relating to the association between CRE and later emotional distress; the mediating role of perceived stress in the relationship between CRE and later emotional distress; and, the moderating effects of peer and adult-level family support on the relationship between CRE and later perceived stress. Data were drawn from the HeadStart study longitudinal cohort (N = 19,159 adolescents; mean age = 12.22 at T1). Analyses revealed that CRE significantly predicted later adolescent emotional distress; this relationship was partially mediated by perceived stress; and, both peer and adult-level family support significantly moderated the impact of CRE on later perceived stress.

Cumulative risk exposure, perceived stress, and emotional distress (hypotheses 1 to 3)

Consistent with Hypothesis 1, our analyses demonstrated that CRE at T1 significantly predicted later emotional distress at T3, above and beyond each significant individual risk factor. Our findings thereby provide support for both the accumulation principle (i.e., the more risk factors to which an individual is exposed, the greater the probability of maladaptive outcomes) and the number-over-nature principle (i.e., it is the number of risk factors, as opposed to their nature, which is the central driver of maladaptive outcomes) in cumulative risk theory (Evans et al., Reference Evans, Li and Whipple2013). The specific risk factors identified in our analysis are consistent with prior research that has identified being female (Campbell et al., Reference Campbell, Bann and Patalay2021), exposure to bullying (Deniz & Toseeb, Reference Deniz and Toseeb2023; Moore et al., Reference Moore, Norman, Suetani, Thomas, Sly and Scott2017), having special educational needs (Deighton et al., Reference Deighton, Lereya, Casey, Patalay, Humphrey and Wolpert2019), and experiencing familial socioeconomic deprivation (Verhulst & Tiemeier, Reference Verhulst, Tiemeier, Taylor, Verhulst, Wong and Yoshida2020) as being predictive of emotional distress in children and adolescents. Interestingly, several previously established risk factors such as relative age (Patalay et al., Reference Patalay, Belsky, Fonagy, Vostanis, Humphrey, Deighton and Wolpert2015) were not confirmed in our analysis, and low prior academic attainment actually predicted lower levels of later emotional distress (hence the decision to recode the risk factor designation in our analyses – see Constructing the CRE Index). This surprising finding may reflect greater complexity in the relationship between attainment and emotional distress than has previously been assumed. While it has traditionally been proposed that low academic attainment would increase emotional distress because of self-perceived failure, there is evidence that this process may only (or particularly) apply to girls (Panayiotou & Humphrey, Reference Panayiotou and Humphrey2017; Verboom et al., Reference Verboom, Sijtsema, Verhulst, Penninx and Ormel2014). Furthermore, although studies have often cited negative relationships between greater attainment and mental health difficulties, it has been suggested that perhaps high attainment and corresponding engagement in more challenging academic work could result in increased workload and pressure, thus heightening stress and the associated risk of difficulties. Patalay and Fizsimons (Reference Patalay and Fitzsimons2018) reported that higher cognitive ability – typically predictive of greater academic attainment (Peng & Kievit, Reference Peng and Kievit2020) - was associated with poorer self-reported mental health among a UK sample of adoelscents. Other evidence has indicated that high-performing students may be more emotionally affected by academic-related pressures (D’agostino et al., Reference D’agostino, Schirripa Spagnolo and Salvati2022). Further research examining potential complexities in how attainment may operate as a risk factor beyond a binary function may be useful, particularly giving growing concerns about the extent of academic pressure experienced by adolescents (The Lancet Child & Adolescent Health, 2023).

In line with Hypothesis 2, we found that perceived stress significantly mediated the association between CRE at T1 and emotional distress at T3. These effects were evident even after controlling for prior (T1) levels of emotional distress and co-variates. Our findings therefore offer clear support for the theorized role of stress in explaining how CRE impacts later physical and mental health outcomes (Evans et al., Reference Evans, Li and Whipple2013; Guidi et al., Reference Guidi, Lucente, Sonino and Fava2020). In particular, they add new evidence highlighting the importance of perceived stress as a mediating variable in this process, aligning with the transactional model (Cohen, Reference Cohen1986; Lazarus & Folkman, Reference Lazarus and Folkman1984). However, it is noteworthy that we only found evidence of partial, as opposed to full mediation, prompting consideration of other factors and processes (e.g., disruption of proximal developmental processes; Evans et al., Reference Evans, Li and Whipple2013) that have been proffered. Further investigation is required to confirm such processes, including how they may intersect with stress appraisal mechanisms.

Peer and family adult social support as moderators of the impact of cumulative risk exposure on perceived stress (hypotheses 3 and 4)

In support of Hypotheses 3 and 4, we found that social support from both peers and parent/carers significantly moderated the impact of CRE on later perceived stress (i.e., adolescents reporting higher levels of support perceived significantly lower levels of stress resulting from CRE compared to those reporting lower levels of support). These findings extend previous, cross-sectional evidence (e.g., Butler et al., Reference Butler, Quigg, Bates, Jones, Ashworth, Gowland and Jones2022; Demkowicz, Reference Demkowicz2019) to show longitudinal effects, and offer support for the protective model, in which access to putative protective factors is theorized to moderate the impact of CRE on later emotional distress (Masten, Reference Masten2014). As expected, protective-reactive effects were observed (i.e., each protective factor diminished, but did not completely eradicate, the expected association between CRE and later emotional distress; Fergus & Zimmerman, Reference Fergus and Zimmerman2005).

These findings are important because though previous studies have found evidence that social connection moderates how negative life events influence mental health difficulties (e.g., Anderson et al., Reference Anderson, Salk and Hyde2015; Ditzen & Heinrichs, Reference Ditzen and Heinrichs2014; Hazel et al., Reference Hazel, Oppenheimer, Technow, Young and Hankin2015), they have often focused on moderation of direct effects rather than underlying stress processes. Our findings therefore offer support for Cohen and Wills’ (Reference Cohen and Wills1985) stress buffering hypothesis, in which it is argued that social support functions to improve wellbeing by lessening the extent to which significant life challenges are experienced as stressful. Furthermore, they emphasize the importance of recognizing risk as non-deterministic and indeed socially embedded, whereby an adolescent’s wider social context can influence the extent to which adversity is likely to lead to distress. Qualitative research has provided insight into the ways that social support processes can function as adolescents manage difficulties in their lives. For instance, Stapley et al. (Reference Stapley, Demkowicz, Eisenstadt, Wolpert and Deighton2019) found that talking to trusted others and drawing on available social support can help individuals to cope with or resolve problems, navigate stressful situations, and cope with difficult feelings.

Strengths and limitations

The current study benefitted from a very large sample, longitudinal design, and use of robust, preregistered statistical modeling procedures to test theoretically informed hypotheses. Nonetheless, there are numerous limitations that need to be borne in mind ahead of considering the implications of our findings. First, though our handling of candidate risk factors was in line with cumulative risk theory, it is important to acknowledge alternative models that would prompt different treatment of these data. For example, in the dimensional risk model, factors representing threat (i.e., the presence of harmful input, such as bullying victimization) and those representing deprivation (i.e., the absence of expected input, such as poverty) are differentiated, with distinct downstream consequences for later mental health outcomes (McLaughlin & Sheridan, Reference McLaughlin and Sheridan2019).

Second, as is the case with all secondary analyses, we were limited to those measures that were available in the dataset (in this case, the HeadStart longitudinal cohort). It is important to note, therefore, that our CRE index was not comprehensive (for example, established risk factors such as parental psychiatric symptoms (Wille et al., Reference Wille, Bettge and Ravens-Sieberer2008) were not included). It should also be noted that the lack of a physiological stress measure in the dataset makes it challenging to differentiate the impact of perceived stress and physiological stress (i.e., does perceived stress simply act as a proxy for underpinning physiological stress, or does it yield distinct effects?). Furthermore, we acknowledge the concerns raised regarding the measures of perceived stress (Demkowicz et al., Reference Demkowicz, Panayiotou, Ashworth, Humphrey and Deighton2020) and emotional distress (Black et al., Reference Black, Mansfield and Panayiotou2020) we used, while noting the steps taken to address these in the current study (e.g., modeling perceived stress as a two-item manifest variable).

Third, in focusing exclusively on the effects of peer and adult-level family support, we neglected internal protective factors, including for example self-efficacy (Klasen et al., Reference Klasen, Otto, Kriston, Patalay, Schlack and Ravens-Sieberer2014). Fourth, the purposive sampling of Local Authorities employed in the HeadStart study meant that it was not designed to be representative. Notably, the study sample were subject to somewhat higher levels of socioeconomic disadvantage than is seen nationally across England. Accordingly, caution is required in generalizing the findings of the current study to the broader adolescent population. Finally, the dataset used in the current study was generated prior to the onset of the COVID-19 pandemic. This is particularly important given its impact on adolescent mental health (Mansfield et al., Reference Mansfield, Santos, Deighton, Hayes, Velikonja, Boehnke and Patalay2022), and consequent implications for the nature and extent of risk factor exposure and access to protective factors (Kuhlman et al., Reference Kuhlman, Straka, Mousavi, Tran and Rodgers2021).

Implications

In demonstrating that CRE significantly predicts later adolescent emotional distress, that this relationship is partially mediated by perceived stress, and that both peer and adult-level family support significantly moderate the impact of CRE on later perceived stress, the current study provides support for both cumulative risk theory (Evans et al., Reference Evans, Li and Whipple2013) and the protective model of adolescent resilience (Fergus & Zimmerman, Reference Fergus and Zimmerman2005; Masten, Reference Masten2014). This empirical delineation of the processes and mechanisms underpinning the development of emotional distress (and their moderation) also provides multiple practical implications. First and foremost, intervention to reduce risk factor exposure should be prioritized. Among the “variable” risk factors (i.e., those that can be changed; Furber et al., Reference Furber, Leach, Guy and Segal2017) identified in the current study, some are reflective of broader structural inequalities (e.g., familial socioeconomic deprivation) that would require government action to remedy (Marmot, Reference Marmot2020), but others represent more immediately tractable issues (e.g., bullying victimization) that could feasibly be targeted through the implementation of school-based interventions, for which there is a robust evidence base (Fraguas et al., Reference Fraguas, Díaz-Caneja, Ayora, Durán-Cutilla, Abregú-Crespo, Ezquiaga-Bravo and Arango2021). For “fixed marker” risk factors (i.e. those that cannot be changed; Furber et al., Reference Furber, Leach, Guy and Segal2017), notably gender, further research is required to better understand what factors place adolescent girls at significantly higher risk of experiencing emotional distress than boys, in order that opportunities to intervene to address or mitigate these factors can be identified (Patalay & Demkowicz, Reference Patalay and Demkowicz2023).

The finding that perceived stress partially mediates the relationship between cumulative risk factor exposure and later emotional distress provides a second practical implication: intervention to modify stress appraisal processes. At the broadest level, aggregative reviews and meta-analytic evidence indicate that supporting and promoting relaxation, social problem solving and/or emotional regulation can be effective in helping adolescents to manage stress and increase coping skills (Kraag et al., Reference Kraag, Zeegers, Kok, Hosman and Abu-Saad2006; Rew et al., Reference Rew, Johnson and Young2014). However, our study also highlighted specific factors as being particularly important in modifying the effects of CRE on perceived stress, which brings us to our third practical implication: intervention to promote adult-level family and peer social support. With regard to the former, meta-analytic evidence indicates that intervening to improve parenting skills and/or the parent-child relationship can lead to small but meaningful reductions in emotional distress among children and adolescents, though targeted interventions yield stronger effects than universal, preventive programmes (Yap et al., Reference Yap, Morgan, Cairns, Jorm, Hetrick and Merry2016). In relation to the latter, a recent systematic review indicated that despite the widespread use of peer-led interventions (e.g., those involving peer mentoring, peer buddying, peer counselling and peer education), the evidence base for mental health outcomes is relatively sparse (King & Fazel, Reference King and Fazel2021). Given the findings of the current study, the development of efficacious approaches to peer-led interventions should therefore be prioritized in order to provide further opportunities to interrupt the processes through which CRE leads to emotional distress.

The above evidence pertaining to intervention efficacy assesses each substantive element in isolation (i.e., interventions to address identified risk factors; manage stress and increase coping; promote parenting support; or, foster peer support). This is not meant to imply they should be delivered in isolation; such a piecemeal, fragmented approach (i.e., “a program for every problem”) is unlikely to be successful or sustainable in the longer term (Domitrovich et al., Reference Domitrovich, Bradshaw, Greenberg, Embry, Poduska and Ialongo2010). It is therefore important to consider integrative frameworks that involve work across multiple tiers/levels and components (e.g., universal social and emotional skills curriculum to support well-being; identification of need; provision of targeted support; working with parents/carers; promotion of nurturing school ethos and environment), commonly referred to as the “whole school” approach to mental health promotion (Demkowicz & Humphrey, Reference Demkowicz and Humphrey2019). However, few interventions are truly whole school in nature, which means that the adoption of a whole-school approach is likely to involve drawing together a number of distinct interventions in an overarching framework (Domitrovich et al., Reference Domitrovich, Bradshaw, Greenberg, Embry, Poduska and Ialongo2010). By way of illustration, consider the Australian “Be You” mental health initiative, which comprises five strands: (i) learning resilience; (ii) early support; (iii) family partnerships; (iv) responding together; and (v) mentally healthy communities. Resources and professional development opportunities are provided to support the delivery of each strand. One key resource is a directory of over 80 programs that map onto one or more of the above strands, providing information on area(s) of focus, evidence base, theoretical framework, structure, and other salient features. One can easily envisage how the findings of the current study could be applied as part of the intervention selection process to provide a tailored yet comprehensive whole school response (for example, screening and intervention to reduce risk factor exposure as part of the early support strand; intervening to improve parenting skills and/or the parent-child relationship as part of the family partnerships strand).

Conclusion

In the current study, preregistered longitudinal moderated mediation analyses of a large adolescent cohort dataset were used to test a series of hypotheses relating to the association between CRE and later emotional distress; the mediating role of perceived stress in the relationship between CRE and later emotional distress; and, the moderating effects of peer and adult-level family support on the relationship between CRE and later perceived stress. Based on these analyses, we conclude that CRE predicts later adolescent emotional distress; that this relationship is partially mediated by perceived stress; and, that both peer and adult-level family support can moderate the impact of CRE on later perceived stress. Collectively, our findings provide support for both cumulative risk theory (Evans et al., Reference Evans, Li and Whipple2013) and the protective model of adolescent resilience (Fergus & Zimmerman, Reference Fergus and Zimmerman2005; Masten, Reference Masten2014), while also providing insights into when and how we might intervene to disrupt the processes and mechanisms underpinning the development of emotional distress in adolescence.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579424001275.

Acknowledgments

The authors are grateful for the work of the wider research teams at the Anna Freud Centre and to the University of Manchester for their role in collecting and managing data and to the local authorities and schools for coordinating data collection. Thanks are due to the participants for taking the time to complete measures and to the National Pupil Database, from which demographic data was obtained.

Funding statement

The data used in this study were collected as part of HeadStart learning programme and supported by funding from The National Lottery Community Fund. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and it does not reflect the views of The National Lottery Community Fund.

Competing interests

None.

References

Ahmad, G., McManus, S., Bécares, L., Hatch, S. L., & Das-Munshi, J. (2022). Explaining ethnic variations in adolescent mental health: A secondary analysis of the millennium cohort study. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 57(4), 817828.10.1007/s00127-021-02167-wCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anderson, S. F., Salk, R. H., & Hyde, J. S. (2015). Stress in romantic relationships and adolescent depressive symptoms: Influence of parental support. Journal of Family Psychology, 29(3), 339348.10.1037/fam0000089CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Appleyard, K., Egeland, B., van Dulmen, M. H. M., & Sroufe, L. A. (2005). When more is not better: The role of cumulative risk in child behavior outcomes. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 46(3), 235245. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00351.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bains, S. A., & Gutman, L. A. (2021). Mental health in ethnic minority populations in the UK: Developmental trajectories from early childhood to mid adolescence. Journal of Youth and Adoelscence, 50(11), 21512165.10.1007/s10964-021-01481-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Black, L., Mansfield, R., & Panayiotou, M. (2020). Age appropriateness of the self-report strengths and difficulties questionnaire. Assessment, 28(6), 15561569.10.1177/1073191120903382CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Butler, N. A.-O., Quigg, Z., Bates, R., Jones, L., Ashworth, E., Gowland, S., & Jones, M. (2022). The contributing role of family, school, and peer supportive relationships in protecting the mental wellbeing of children and adolescents. School Mental Health Program, 14(3), 776778.10.1007/s12310-022-09502-9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Campbell, O. L. K., Bann, D., & Patalay, P. (2021). The gender gap in adolescent mental health: A cross-national investigation of 566,829 adolescents across 73 countries. SSM - Population Health, 13, 100742. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ssmph.2021.100742 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Christina, S., Magson, N. R., Kakar, V., & Rapee, R. M. (2021). The bidirectional relationships between peer victimization and internalizing problems in school-aged children: An updated systematic review and meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 85, 101979. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2021.101979 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cicchetti, D., & Rogosch, F. A. (1996). Equifinality and multifinality in developmental psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 8(4), 597600. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579400007318 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohen, S. (1986). Contrasting the hassles scale and the perceived stress scale: Who’s really measuring appraised stress? American Psychologist, 41(6), 716718. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.41.6.716 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohen, S., Kamarck, T., & Mermelstein, R. (1983). A global measure of perceived stress. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 24, 385396. https://doi.org/10.2307/2136404 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, S. F. W. T. A., & Wills, T. A. (1985). Stress, social support, and the buffering hypothesis. Psychological bulletin, 98(2), 310357.10.1037/0033-2909.98.2.310CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
D’agostino, A., Schirripa Spagnolo, F., & Salvati, N. (2022). Studying the relationship between anxiety and school achievement: Evidence from PISA data. Statistical Methods & Applications, 31(1), 120. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10260-021-00563-9 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deighton, J., Humphrey, N., Patalay, P., Belsky, J., Wolpert, M., & Vostanis, P. (2018). Longitudinal pathways between mental health difficulties and academic performance during middle childhood and early adolescence. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 36(1), 110126. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjdp.12218 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deighton, J., Lereya, S. T., Casey, P., Patalay, P., Humphrey, N., & Wolpert, M. (2019). Prevalence of mental health problems in schools: Poverty and other risk factors among 28 000 adolescents in England. British Journal of Psychiatry, 215(3), 565567. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.2019.19 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Demkowicz, O. A. (2019). A mixed methods investigation of multiple risk exposure and early adolescent girls’ emotional symptoms: Mechanisms, adaptive processes, and lived experience [Doctoral dissertation, University of Manchester]. Research Explorer The University of Manchester. https://research.manchester.ac.uk/en/studentTheses/a-mixed-methods-investigation-of-multiple-risk-exposure-and-early Google Scholar
Demkowicz, O., & Humphrey, N. (2019). Whole school approaches to promoting mental health: what does the evidence say? https://www.ucl.ac.uk/evidence-based-practice-unit/sites/evidence-based-practice-unit/files/evidencebriefing5_v1d7_completed_06.01.20.pdf Google Scholar
Demkowicz, O., Panayiotou, M., Ashworth, E., Humphrey, N., & Deighton, J. (2020). The factor structure of the 4-item perceived stress scale in English adolescents. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 36(5), 913917. https://doi.org/10.1027/1015-5759/a000562 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Demkowicz, O., Panayiotou, M., & Humphrey, N. (2021). Cumulative risk exposure and emotional symptoms among early adolescent girls. BMC Women’s Health, 21(1), 388. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12905-021-01527-7 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deniz, E., Humphrey, N., Demkowicz, O., Lereya, S., & Deighton, J. (2023, February 10). Cumulative risk and adolescent emotional distress: A longitudinal moderated mediation analysis focusing on stress and social support. OSF Registries. https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/R8ZY5 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deniz, E., & Toseeb, U. (2023). A longitudinal study of sibling bullying and mental health in autistic adolescents: The role of self-esteem. Autism Research, 16(8), 117. https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.2987 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ditzen, B., & Heinrichs, M. (2014). Psychobiology of social support: The social dimension of stress buffering. Restorative Neurology and Neuroscience, 32(1), 149162. https://doi.org/10.3233/RNN-139008 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Domitrovich, C. E., Bradshaw, C. P., Greenberg, M. T., Embry, D., Poduska, J. M., & Ialongo, N. S. (2010). Integrated models of school-based prevention: Logic and theory. Psychology School, 47, 7188.10.1002/pits.20452CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Evans, G., Kim, P., Ting, A., Tesher, H., & Shannis, D. (2007). Cumulative risk, maternal responsiveness, and allostatic load among young adolescents. Developmental Psychology, 43(2), 341351. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.43.2.341 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Evans, G., Li, D., & Whipple, S. (2013). Cumulative risk and child development. Psychological Bulletin, 139(6), 13421396. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0031808 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Evidence-Based Practice Unit (2023). HeadStart national evaluation: Final report. N. L. C. Fund.Google Scholar
Fergus, S., & Zimmerman, M. A. (2005). ADOLESCENT RESILIENCE: A framework for understanding healthy development in the face of risk. Annual Review of Public Health, 26(1), 399419. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.publhealth.26.021304.144357 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fraguas, D., Díaz-Caneja, C. M., Ayora, M., Durán-Cutilla, M., Abregú-Crespo, R., Ezquiaga-Bravo, I., & Arango, C. (2021). Assessment of school anti-bullying interventions: A meta-analysis of randomized clinical trials. JAMA Pediatric, 175(1), 4455.10.1001/jamapediatrics.2020.3541CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Furber, G., Leach, M., Guy, S., & Segal, L. (2017). Developing a broad categorisation scheme to describe risk factors for mental illness, for use in prevention policy and planning. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 51(3), 230240. https://doi.org/10.1177/0004867416642844 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodman, A., Patel, V., & Leon, D. A. (2008). Child mental health differences amongst ethnic groups in Britain: A systematic review. BMC Public Health, 8(1), 258269.10.1186/1471-2458-8-258CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodman, R. (1997). The strengths and difficulties questionnaire: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38, 581586.10.1111/j.1469-7610.1997.tb01545.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gottlieb, B. H., & Bergen, A. E. (2010). Social support concepts and measures. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 69(5), 511520. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychores.2009.10.001 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guidi, J., Lucente, M., Sonino, N., Fava, G A. (2020). Allostatic load and its impact on health: A systematic review. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 90(1), 1127. https://doi.org/10.1159/000510696 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hazel, N. A., Oppenheimer, C. W., Technow, J. R., Young, J. F., & Hankin, B. L. (2015). Parent relationship quality buffers against the effect of peer stressors on depressive symptoms from middle childhood to adolescence. Developmental psychology, 50(8), 21152123.10.1037/a0037192CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hazell, M., Thornton, E., Haghparast-Bidgoli, H., & Patalay, P. (2022). Socio-economic inequalities in adolescent mental health in the UK: Multiple socio-economic indicators and reporter effects. SSM-Mental Health, 2, 100176. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ssmmh.2022.100176 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
He, J. P., Burstein, M., Schmitz, A., & Merikangas, K. R. (2013). The strengths and difficulties questionnaire (SDQ): The factor structure and scale validation in US adolescents. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 41(4), 583595. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-012-9696-6 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hu, L., & Bentler, P. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 6(1), 155. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705519909540118 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hughes, K., Bellis, M. A., Hardcastle, K. A., Sethi, D., Butchart, A., Mikton, C., & Dunne, M. P. (2017). The effect of multiple adverse childhood experiences on health: A systematic review and meta-analysis. The Lancet Public Health, 2(8), e356e366. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2468-2667(17)30118-4 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Humphrey, N. (2018). Are the kids alright? Examining the intersection between educational and mental health. Psychology of Education Review, 42, 412.Google Scholar
Kavanaugh, M. S., Stamatopoulos, V., Cohen, D., & Zhang, L. (2016). Unacknowledged caregivers: A scoping review of research on caregiving youth in the United States. Adolescent Research Review, 1(1), 2949. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40894-015-0015-7 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kim, K. J., Conger, R. D., Elder, G. H. Jr, Lorenz, F. O. (2003). Reciprocal influences between stressful life events and adolescent internalizing and externalizing problems. Child Development, 74(1), 127143. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00525 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
King, T., & Fazel, M. (2021). Examining the mental health outcomes of school-based peer-led interventions on young people: A scoping review of range and a systematic review of effectiveness. PLoS One, 16(4), e0249553.10.1371/journal.pone.0249553CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klasen, F., Otto, C., Kriston, L., Patalay, P., Schlack, R., Ravens-Sieberer, U., & BELLA study group (2014). Risk and protective factors for the development of depressive symptoms in children and adolescents: results of the longitudinal BELLA study. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 24(6), 695703.10.1007/s00787-014-0637-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kraag, G., Zeegers, M. P., Kok, G., Hosman, C., & Abu-Saad, H. H. (2006). School programs targeting stress management in children and adolescents: A meta-analysis. Journal of School Psychology, 44(6), 449472. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsp.2006.07.001 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuhlman, K. R., Straka, K., Mousavi, Z., Tran, M. L., & Rodgers, E. (2021). Predictors of adolescent resilience during the COVID-19 pandemic: Cognitive reappraisal and humor. Journal of Adolescent Health, 69(5), 729736.10.1016/j.jadohealth.2021.07.006CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lacey, R. E., Xue, B., & McMunn, A. (2022). The mental and physical health of young carers: A systematic review. The Lancet Public Health, 7(9), e787e796. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2468-2667(22)00161-X CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lazarus, R. S., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal, and coping. Springer.Google Scholar
Leffert, N., Benson, P. L., Scales, P. C., Sharma, A. R., Drake, D. R., & Blyth, D. A. (1998). Developmental assets: Measurement and prediction of risk behaviors among adolescents. Applied Developmental Science, 2(4), 209230. https://doi.org/10.1207/s1532480xads0204_4 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lereya, S. T., Humphrey, N., Patalay, P., Wolpert, M., Böhnke, J. R., Macdougall, A., & Deighton, J. (2016). The student resilience survey: Psychometric validation and associations with mental health. Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Mental Health, 10, 115. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13034-016-0132-5 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacCallum, R. C., Roznowski, M., & Necowitz, L. B. (1992). Model modifications in covariance structure analysis: The problem of capitalization on chance. Psychological Bulletin, 111(3), 490504.10.1037/0033-2909.111.3.490CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mansfield, R., Santos, J., Deighton, J., Hayes, D., Velikonja, T., Boehnke, J. R., & Patalay, P. (2022). The impact of the COVID-19 pandemic on adolescent mental health: A natural experiment. Royal Society Open Science, 9(4), 211114. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsos.211114 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marmot, M. (2020). Health equity in England: The marmot review 10 years on. BMJ, 368, m693. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.m693 Google Scholar
Marquez, J., Humphrey, N., Black, L., Cutts, M., & Khanna, D. (2023). Gender and sexual identity-based inequalities in adolescent wellbeing: Findings from the# beeWell study. BMC Public Health, 23(1), 2211. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-023-16992-y CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Masten, A. S. (2014). Ordinary magic: Resilience in development. Guilford Press.Google Scholar
McLaughlin, K., & Sheridan, M. (2019). Beyond cumulative risk: A dimensional approach to childhood adversity. Physiology & Behavior, 176(3), 139148. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2017.03.040 Google Scholar
Moore, S. E., Norman, R. E., Suetani, S., Thomas, H. J., Sly, P. D., & Scott, J. G. (2017). Consequences of bullying victimization in childhood and adolescence: A systematic review and meta-analysis. World Journal of Psychiatry, 7(1), 60. https://doi.org/10.5498/wjp.v7.i1.60 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Newlove-Delgado, T., Marcheselli, F., Williams, T., Mandalia, D., Davis, J., McManus, S., Savic, M., Treloar, W., & Ford, T. (2022). Mental health of children and young people in England 2022 - wave 3 follow up to the 2017 survey.Google Scholar
NHS Digital (2018). Mental health of children and young people in England, 2017. https://digital.nhs.uk/data-and-information/publications/statistical/mental-health-of-children-and-young-people-in-england/2017/2017 (accessed September 11, 2024).Google Scholar
Orben, A., Tomova, L., & Blakemore, S.-J. (2020). The effects of social deprivation on adolescent development and mental health. The Lancet Child & Adolescent Health, 4(8), 634640. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2352-4642(20)30186-3 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Panayiotou, M., & Humphrey, N. (2017). Mental health difficulties and academic attainment: Evidence for gender-specific developmental cascades in middle childhood. Development and Psychopathology, 1-16(2), 523538. https://doi.org/10.1017/S095457941700102X Google Scholar
Patalay, P., Belsky, J., Fonagy, P., Vostanis, P., Humphrey, N., Deighton, J., & Wolpert, M. (2015). The extent and specificity of relative age effects on mental health and functioning in early adolescence. Journal of Adolescent Health, 57(5), 475481. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2015.07.012 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Patalay, P., & Demkowicz, O. (2023). Debate: Don’t mind the gap – why do we not care about the gender gap in common mental health difficulties? [https://doi.org/10.1111/camh.12647]. Child and Adolescent Mental Health, 28(2), 341343. https://doi.org/10.1111/camh.12647 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Patalay, P., & Fitzsimons, E. (2018). Development and predictors of mental ill-health and wellbeing from childhood to adolescence. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 53, 13111323. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00127-018-1604-0 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peng, P., & Kievit, R. A. (2020). The development of academic achievement and cognitive abilities: A bidirectional perspective. Child Development Perspectives, 14(1), 1520. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12352 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Phillips, A. C., Carroll, D., & Der, G. (2015). Negative life events and symptoms of depression and anxiety: Stress causation and/or stress generation. Anxiety, Stress, & Coping, 28(4), 357371. https://doi.org/10.1080/10615806.2015.1005078 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
R Core Team 2023). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing.Google Scholar
Rew, L., Johnson, K. F., & Young, C. (2014). A systematic review of interventions to reduce stress in adolescence. Issues in Mental Health Nursing, 35(11), 851863.10.3109/01612840.2014.924044CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rutter, M., Tizard, J., Yule, W., Graham, P., & Whitmore, K. (1976). Isle of wight studies, 1964-1974. Psychological Medicine, 6(2), 313332. https://doi.org/10.1017/S003329170001388X CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Solmi, M., Radua, J., Olivola, M., Croce, E., Soardo, L., Salazar de Pablo, G., Il Shin, J., Kirkbride, J. B., Jones, P., Kim, J. H., Kim, J. Y., Carvalho, A. F., Seeman, M. V., Correll, C. U., & Fusar-Poli, P. (2021). Age at onset of mental disorders worldwide: Large-scale meta-analysis of 192 epidemiological studies. Molecular Psychiatry, 27(1), 281295. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41380-021-01161-7 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Squires, G., Humphrey, N., Barlow, A., & Wigelsworth, M. (2012). The identification of special educational needs and the month of birth: Differential effects of category of need and level of assessment. European Journal of Special Needs Education, 27(4), 469481. https://doi.org/10.1080/08856257.2012.711961 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stapley, E., Demkowicz, O., Eisenstadt, M., Wolpert, M., & Deighton, J. (2019). Coping with the stresses of daily life in England: A qualitative study of self-care strategies and social and professional support in early adolescence. The Journal of Early Adolescence, 40(5), 605632. https://doi.org/10.1177/0272431619858420 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
The Lancet Child & Adolescent Health. (2023). Time to take academic pressure seriously. The Lancet Child & Adolescent Health, 7(10), 671. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2352-4642(23)00231-6 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Umberson, D., Crosnoe, R. F., & Reczek, C. (2010). Social relationships and health behavior across life course. Annu Rev Sociol, 36, 139157.10.1146/annurev-soc-070308-120011CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ungar, M., Ghazinour, M., & Richter, J. (2013). Annual research review: What is resilience within the social ecology of human development? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 54(4), 348366. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12025 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van Roy, B., Veenstra, M., & Clench-Aas, J. (2008). Construct validity of the five-factor strengths and difficulties questionnaire (SDQ) in pre-, early, and late adolescence. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49(12), 13041312. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.01942.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Verboom, C. E., Sijtsema, J. J., Verhulst, F. C., Penninx, B. W., & Ormel, J. (2014). Longitudinal associations between depressive problems, academic performance, and social functioning in adolescent boys and girls. Developmental psychology, 50(1), 247257.10.1037/a0032547CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Verhulst, F., & Tiemeier, H. (2020). Socioeconomic inequalities and mental health problems in children and adolescents. In Taylor, E., Verhulst, F., Wong, J. C. M., & Yoshida, K. (Ed.), Mental health and illness of children and adolescents (pp. 257274). Springer Singapore. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-981-10-2348-4_57 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Visser, K., Bolt, G., Finkenauer, C., Jonker, M., Weinberg, D., & Stevens, G. W. (2021). Neighbourhood deprivation effects on young people’s mental health and well-being: A systematic review of the literature. Social Science & Medicine, 270, 113542. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2020.113542 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wang, J., & Wang, X. (2019). Structural equation modeling: Applications using Mplus. John Wiley & Sons.10.1002/9781119422730CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wille, N., Bettge, S., & Ravens-Sieberer, U. (2008). Risk and protective factors for children’s and adolescents’ mental health: Results of the BELLA study. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 17(S1), 133147. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-008-1015-y CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
World Health Organization (2012). Risks to mental health: An overview of vulnerabilities and risk factors (Background paper by WHO secretariat for the development of a comprehensive mental health action). WHO.Google Scholar
Yap, M. B. H., Morgan, A. J., Cairns, K., Jorm, A. F., Hetrick, S. E., & Merry, S. (2016). Parents in prevention: A meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials of parenting interventions to prevent internalizing problems in children from birth to age 18. Clinical Psychology Review, 50, 138158.10.1016/j.cpr.2016.10.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zwierzynska, K., Wolke, D., & Lereya, T. S. (2013). Peer victimization in childhood and internalizing problems in adolescence: A prospective longitudinal study. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 41(2), 309323. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-012-9678-8 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Figure 1. Proposed moderated mediation model.

Figure 1

Table 1. Candidate risk factors: indicative evidence and theorization

Figure 2

Table 2. Point biserial correlations (zero-order correlations) between the individual risk factors and emotional distress at T3

Figure 3

Table 3. Regression of emotional distress at T3 on to the cumulative risk exposure index, controlling for all individual risk factors and covariates (N = 19,159a)

Figure 4

Table 4. The mediating role of perceived stress at T2 between cumulative risk exposure at T1 and emotional distress at T3 (N = 19,159)

Figure 5

Figure 2. Reported moderated mediation model.

Figure 6

Table 5. Hypothesized moderated mediation (N = 18,979, missing not handled = 180)

Supplementary material: File

Deniz et al. supplementary material

Deniz et al. supplementary material
Download Deniz et al. supplementary material(File)
File 19.9 KB