Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gvvz8 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-27T12:54:46.912Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Does the neighbourhood food environment contribute to ethnic differences in diet quality? Results from the HELIUS study in Amsterdam, the Netherlands

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 May 2021

Maartje P Poelman*
Affiliation:
Chair Group Consumption and Healthy Lifestyles, Wageningen University & Research, PO Box 8130, Wageningen, The Netherlands
Mary Nicolaou
Affiliation:
Department of Public Health, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam Public Health Research Institute, Meibergdreef, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
S Coosje Dijkstra
Affiliation:
Department of Health Sciences, Faculty of Science, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, Amsterdam Public Health Research Institute, De Boelelaan, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
Joreintje D Mackenbach
Affiliation:
Department of Epidemiology and Biostatistics, Amsterdam UMC, Vrije Universiteit, Amsterdam Public Health Research Institute, Amsterdam, The Netherlands Upstream Team, Amsterdam UMC, The Netherlands
Meng Lu
Affiliation:
Department of Physical Geography, Utrecht University, The Netherlands
Derek Karssenberg
Affiliation:
Department of Physical Geography, Utrecht University, The Netherlands
Marieke B Snijder
Affiliation:
Department of Public Health, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam Public Health Research Institute, Meibergdreef, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
Ilonca Vaartjes
Affiliation:
Julius Centre for Health Sciences and Primary Care, University Medical Centre Utrecht, The Netherlands
Karien Stronks
Affiliation:
Department of Public Health, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam Public Health Research Institute, Meibergdreef, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
*
*Corresponding author: Email maartje.poelman@wur.nl
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Objective:

The aim of the current study was to establish whether the neighbourhood food environment, characterised by the healthiness of food outlets, the diversity of food outlets and fast-food outlet density within a 500 m or 1000 m street network buffer around the home address, contributed to ethnic differences in diet quality.

Design:

Cross-sectional cohort study.

Setting:

Amsterdam, the Netherlands.

Participants:

Data on adult participants of Dutch, South-Asian Surinamese, African Surinamese, Turkish and Moroccan descent (n total 4728) in the HELIUS study were analysed.

Results:

The neighbourhood food environment of ethnic minority groups living in Amsterdam is less supportive of a healthy diet and of less diversity than that of participants of Dutch origin. For example, participants of Turkish, Moroccan and South-Asian Surinamese descent reside in a neighbourhood with a significantly higher fast-food outlet density (≤1000 m) than participants of Dutch descent. However, we found no evidence that neighbourhood food environment characteristics directly contributed to ethnic differences in diet quality.

Conclusion:

Although ethnic minority groups lived in less healthy food environments than participants of ethnic Dutch origin, this did not contribute to ethnic differences in diet quality. Future research should investigate other direct or indirect consequences of residing in less supportive food environments and gain a better understanding of how different ethnic groups make use of their neighbourhood food environment.

Type
Research paper
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of The Nutrition Society

Dietary behaviours have been found to vary among ethnic groups all over the world(Reference Dekker, Nicolaou and van Dam1Reference Sommer, Sletner and Jenum3). For example, studies in the Netherlands showed ethnic differences in meeting dietary recommendations for individual food groups as well as composite diet quality scores(Reference Yau, Adams and White4). To illustrate, individuals with a Surinamese or Moroccan background adhere relative less frequent to daily recommendations for vegetable consumption and individuals of Turkish origin adhere less frequent to recommendations for fish consumption and consumed higher amounts of saturated fat than to those from Dutch origin. Moreover, non-western ethnic minority groups had generally lower intake of fibre and micronutrients like Ca and vitamins A and B1 (Reference de Boer, Brants and Beukers5).

Different mechanisms have been proposed to explain ethnic differences in diet quality, including cultural and migration-related factors(Reference Wang and Chen6,Reference Landrine and Klonoff7) . To illustrate, cultural eating habits including the consumption of traditional foods may impact on diet quality, while the process of migration and contact between cultures may in turn induce dietary change due to acculturation(Reference Satia8,Reference Nicolaou, Benjelloun and Stronks9) . In addition to these ethnic specific factors, contextual variables may shape food choices and thus influence dietary intake and health(Reference Bandura10,Reference Sallis, Owen, Fisher, Glanz, Rimer and Viswanath11) . The food environment has been recognised as a strong determinant of food choices and health(Reference Swinburn, Kraak and Allender12). More specifically, the community food environment is defined as the number, type, location and accessibility of food outlets in the direct living environment(Reference Glanz, Sallis and Saelens13).

There is some evidence that individuals living in ethnic minority neighbourhoods have less access to food outlets selling foods contributing to a healthy diet (e.g. fruit and vegetables) and greater access to food outlets selling foods hindering a healthy diet (e.g. fast food)(Reference Black, Moon and Baird14,Reference Timmermans, Dijkstra and Kamphuis15) . If the presence of food outlets in the neighbourhood steer food choices, this could contribute to ethnic differences in dietary quality. Yet, evidence is mixed. A review indicated that 65 % (13 out of the 20) of the included studies found that the food environment (expressed as geographic availability of food outlets) was associated with multiple dietary outcomes(Reference Caspi, Sorensen and Subramanian16), although it should be acknowledged that the food environment–diet relationship remains ambiguous to date. Nevertheless, if the community food environment does contribute to ethnic differences in diet quality, food environment interventions – for example, banning fast-food outlets in certain areas, as has recently been proposed by a number of municipalities around the world, including Amsterdam – could also contribute to a decrease in ethnic dietary differences.

The aim of the present study was to establish whether the neighbourhood food environment is a contributor to ethnic differences in diet quality. To do so, we conducted mediation analyses. First, we assessed ethnic differences in diet quality, including Mediterranean diet score (MDS) and fast-food consumption. We then assessed ethnic differences in the neighbourhood food environment (500 m or 1000 m street network buffer around the home address), including three food environment proxies (healthiness of food outlets, diversity of food outlets and fast-food outlet density). Finally, we determined the association between this neighbourhood food environment and dietary quality and analysed its contribution to ethnic differences in dietary quality.

Methods

Study design

The HELIUS study is a multi-ethnic population-based cohort study of residents of Amsterdam, the Netherlands(Reference Stronks, Snijder and Peters17,Reference Snijder, Galenkamp and Prins18) . The primary aim is to unravel the causes of the unequal burden of disease across ethnic groups. HELIUS study participants are residents of Dutch, African Surinamese, South-Asian Surinamese, Turkish, Moroccan or Ghanaian origin. Adults aged 18–70 years were randomly sampled, stratified by ethnic origin, through the municipal registry of Amsterdam. Data were collected by means of a questionnaire – either self-reported or completed by researchers – and a physical examination. Detailed information about the HELIUS study and procedure can be found elsewhere(Reference Snijder, Galenkamp and Prins18). Data from a cross-sectional study of dietary patterns within a sub-population of the HELIUS study (the ‘HELIUS dietary patterns study’(Reference Dekker, Snijder and Beukers19)) were used for the current study.

Participants

In the HELIUS Dietary Patterns study, a sub-sample of the HELIUS cohort (n 24·789) was included that was especially set up as a sub-cohort on itself(Reference Stronks, Snijder and Peters17,Reference Snijder, Galenkamp and Prins18) . In this sampling, it was aimed to include equal representation of men and women, of all age groups. Among the migrant groups, it was aimed for equal representation of generations. However, HELIUS participants of Ghanaian origin did not participate in the dietary pattern study, as no Ghanaian-specific FFQ was available at the point in time of data collection.

In total, 5084 individuals (20·5 %) participated in the HELIUS dietary patterns study(Reference Dekker, Snijder and Beukers19). The individuals were of Dutch (n 1482), South-Asian Surinamese (n 1086), African Surinamese (n 1068), Turkish (n 658) and Moroccan (n 790) descent. In the current study, only participants (n 4728, 93·0 %) with a representative energy intake using commonly used cut-off values(Reference Willett20) (women=500–3500 kcal/d, men=800–4000 kcal/d), and no other missing values were included, resulting in a total of 4728 participants of n 1433 Dutch, n 1005 South-Asian Surinamese, n 985 African Surinamese, n 588 Turkish and n 717 Moroccan descent. Participants in the current study were slightly younger and somewhat higher educated than the entire HELIUS cohort.

Study procedure

Dietary intake was collected using ethnic-specific semi-quantitative FFQ with a reference period of 4 weeks. The FFQ included approximately 220 food items covering more than 90 % of the intake of the main nutrients of interest. In brief, the food items used in this ethnic-specific FFQ were selected on the basis of single 24-h recalls of prior cohort studies including Moroccan, Turkish and Surinamese adults as well as an existing validated Dutch FFQ. The selection of the food items was based on two criteria: (1) the percentage of contribution of that food item to absolute nutrient intake and its percentage contribution to the variance in absolute nutrient intake and (2) the comparability of food items with the FFQ for other ethnic groups. Both face validity and relative validity of the HELIUS FFQ have been evaluated(Reference Beukers, Dekker and de Boer21,Reference Visser, Elstgeest and Winkens22) . Details about the development and characteristics of the FFQ can be found elsewhere(Reference Beukers, Dekker and de Boer21).

Participants self-reported the eating frequency and portion size of the food items they might have consumed over the past 4 weeks. Intake of all food items was calculated in g/d, based on the frequency of consumption in the previous 4 weeks and portion size estimation using common and household units and, where relevant, photographs of specific food items (e.g. butter spread on bread). Food items were grouped according to similarity in nutrient profile, culinary use or ethnic origin.

We enriched the HELIUS cohort with food environment data using the data set of Locatus, in which retail food outlets were defined as all places that primarily offer food. Locatus (www.locatus.nl) maintains a database of retail information independently sourced via annual onsite surveys, from which twenty-seven food outlet types were extracted for the year 2013; the outlets include supermarkets, fast-food outlets, greengrocers, bakeries, fish shops and restaurants (a full list of outlets is presented in Appendix 1). The co-authors (D.K. and M.L.) from the Global and Geo Health Data Centre (www.gghdc.nl) calculated food outlet exposure for each food outlet type and each address in Amsterdam. To guarantee participant anonymity, the coordinator of the HELIUS study (M.B.S.) extracted the food environment determinants for each participant from this food outlet exposure dataset.

Measures

Socio-demographic characteristics

Sex, age and educational level

In the HELIUS study, data on sex, age and educational level were obtained from Amsterdam’s municipal registry (Gemeentelijke Basis Administratie)(Reference Stronks, Snijder and Peters17). Educational level was indicated by the highest level of education attained in either the Netherlands or the country of origin. This variable was classified into low education (never been to school, elementary schooling, lower vocational schooling or lower secondary schooling only), medium education (intermediate vocational schooling or intermediate/higher secondary education schooling) and high education (higher vocational schooling or university).

Ethnic origin

Ethnic origin was based on the country of birth. The Dutch origin sample comprised individuals who were born in the Netherlands and whose parents were also born in the Netherlands. People were defined as being of non-Dutch ethnic origin if they fulfilled one of two criteria, namely: (1) born outside the Netherlands and having at least one parent who was also born outside the Netherlands (first-generation migrants) or (2) born in the Netherlands and both parents were born outside the Netherlands (second-generation migrants). Participants of Surinamese descent were further classified according to their self-reported ethnic origin, namely African Surinamese (African descent) or South-Asian Surinamese (South Asian/Indian descent).

Mediterranean diet score and fast-food consumption

Diet quality

The FFQ was used to calculate two proxies for participants’ diet quality, that is, the MDS and fast-food consumption.

Mediterranean diet score

The Mediterranean diet is defined as a primarily plant-based diet characterised by a high ratio of monounsaturated to saturated fats(Reference Davis, Bryan and Hodgson23). The diet has been linked to a variety of health benefits(Reference Dinu, Pagliai and Casini24), including reduced mortality risk and lower incidence of CVD(Reference Sofi, Macchi and Abbate25). The MDS as applied in the current study was calculated using the method described by Panagiotakos et al. (Reference Panagiotakos, Pitsavos and Arvaniti26).

For the consumption of items presumed to be typical of Mediterranean dietary patterns (i.e. non-refined cereals, fruits, vegetables, legumes, olive oil, fish and potatoes), scores of 0 to 5 for never, rare, frequent, very frequent, weekly and daily consumption were assigned, while for the consumed foods presumed to be less typical of this pattern (i.e. red meat and red meat products, poultry and full-fat dairy products), scores were assigned on a reverse scale. After summing the individual component scores, the overall MDS ranged from 0 (lowest compliance) to 80 (highest compliance).

Fast-food consumption

Fast food generally consists of processed meat and refined carbohydrates and is high in salt, saturated fat and calories(Reference Jaworowska, Blackham and Davies27). Fast-food consumption in g/d was calculated by summing the weight of the following products consumed: French fries, deep-fried snacks (including fish snacks, spring rolls, bara, baked banana (pisang goreng)), sausage rolls, kebabs, hamburgers, pizzas (American/Italian and Turkish) and skewered meat.

Food environment characteristics

Many food environment studies disregard the spatial mix and diversity of opportunities(Reference Black, Moon and Baird14,Reference Caspi, Sorensen and Subramanian28) by, for example, assessing only the density of fast-food outlets(Reference Clary, Matthews and Kestens29). Yet, a high density of healthier outlets can compensate for a high density of fast-food outlets, and vice versa, a low number of fast-food outlets surrounded by an even lower number of healthy alternatives may provide even less options for healthy food choices. People are often exposed simultaneously to outlets that sell both ‘healthy’ and ‘unhealthy’ foods(Reference Vernez Moudon, Drewnowski and Duncan30). Capturing a wider food environment spectrum may provide a more nuanced insight into the potential contribution of the neighbourhood food environment to ethnic differences in dietary intake. Therefore, two comprehensive measures were used for the purpose of the current study and were included along with the commonly used food environment measure ‘density of fast-food outlets’. These three exposure measures were calculated within a walking distance (i.e. 500 m and 1000 m) based on the street network TOP10L (https://zakelijk.kadaster.nl/-/top10nl). The highways were removed from the street network, and the road vectors were rasterised to a 10 m resolution.

Food environment healthiness index

In line with prior international studies(Reference Vogel, Lewis and Ntani31Reference Moayyed, Kelly and Feng33), we create a food environment healthiness index (FEHI) for the Netherlands, and we assigned to each food outlet a corresponding ‘healthiness score’(Reference Timmermans, Dijkstra and Kamphuis15), ranging from −5 (unhealthy) to +5 (healthy), derived through a Delphi procedure. These scores were included to avoid a dichotomous categorisation of retail outlets into either ‘healthy’ or ‘unhealthy’ and to give a more nuanced indication of the healthiness of the neighbourhood food environment, based on the overall number of retailers selling food. For each street network buffer around the home address (500 m and 1000 m), an overall FEHI score was calculated by summing the scores corresponding to all food outlets within the buffer. A higher (positive) FEHI score represents a healthier neighbourhood food environment.

Food environment diversity index

To gain better understanding of the diversity of food outlets in the neighbourhood, we created the food environment diversity index (FEDI), and we defined the relative diversity of shops offering core foods (i.e. fruit, vegetables, nut and, fish) as opposed to those selling predominantly non-core foods (i.e. snacks, fast food, sweets), irrespective of the number of shops. To assess the diversity of outlets offering core foods, we assessed whether one or more of the following shops were present (present yes = 1, present no = 0) within a 500 m or 1000 m street network buffer around the home address: fruit and vegetable shop, nut shop, organic shop, fish shop, oriental supermarket, supermarket and mini-supermarket. To assess the diversity of outlets selling predominately non-core foods, we assessed whether one or more of the following shops were present (present yes = 1, present no = 0) within the 500 m or 1000 m residential street network buffer: fast-food outlet, grillroom/kebab, takeaway, ice-cream shop, sweet shop, chocolate shop and cake shop. The diversity of food outlets offering core foods relative to the diversity of outlets selling predominantly non-core foods was calculated (500 m and 1000 m) with a score above 1·0 indicates a higher diversity of food outlets offering core foods as opposed to food outlets selling predominantly non-core foods. A score below 1·0 indicates a higher diversity of food outlets selling predominantly non-core foods as opposed to food outlets offering core foods.

Fast-food density

Fast-food density was defined as the total number of fast-food outlets, grillrooms, kebab outlets and takeaways within a 500 m or 1000 m street network buffer around the home address.

Covariates

Sex and age were included as covariates. To assess the contribution of FEHI and FEDI to MDS, energy intake (in kilocalories) was also included as a covariate. Educational level is highly correlated with ethnicity in our study population and could be a mediator in the association between ethnicity and diet quality. We therefore did not adjust for this variable, as our main aim was to establish whether the food environment contributes to ethnic differences in diet quality, irrespective of participants’ socio-economic position.

Statistical analyses

Descriptive statistics were used to describe participant characteristics for the total sample and stratified by ethnicity. Continuous variables were presented as either means or sd or median and inter-quartile range for skewed data. Categorical variables were presented as relative frequencies.

To examine the potential contribution of the neighbourhood food environment to ethnic differences in MDS and fast-food consumption, mediation analyses using the Hayes Process Macro (V3.3, model 4)(Reference Hayes34) with IBM SPSS V25.0 were conducted. Mediation models estimated the contribution of either FEHI or FEDI to ethnic differences in MDS and the contribution of fast-food density to ethnic differences in fast-food consumption, for both the 500 m and the 1000 m residential street network buffers. Because data on FEHI and fast food density were skewed, decile groups were created and included in the mediation analyses. We used a parallel mediation model to examine total, direct and indirect associations between ethnicity and diet quality (Fig. 1). The total effect (path c) represents the association between ethnicity and diet quality when no mediators are included in the model, while the direct effect (path c’) represents the association between ethnicity and diet quality when mediators are included. Indirect effects (paths a and b) represent the association between ethnicity and diet quality through food environment characteristics. If the indirect association was statistically significant, we concluded that mediation had occurred. The significance of indirect association was tested by bootstrapped 95 % CI, using a resample procedure of 5000 repeated samples. A 95 % CI of the indirect effect that does not cross 0 indicates a statistically significant indirect effect. Models were adjusted for sex, age and kilocalories, SES was not included as confounder because of its strong association with ethnicity. However, we did run sensitivity analysis (path c) with addition for SES as covariate.

Fig. 1 Path model diagram of parallel mediation analysis. The path model showing the association between ethnicity (X) and diet quality (Y) as mediated by neighbourhood food environment characteristics (M). Path c represents the association between ethnicity and diet quality with no mediators in the model. Path c’ represents the association between ethnicity and diet quality when the mediator is included in the model. Indirect effects (paths a and b) represent the association between ethnicity and diet quality through the neighbourhood food environment

Results

Participant characteristics

Participant characteristics for the total sample and stratified by ethnicity are presented in Table 1. The total sample comprised 2821 women and 1907 men, with a mean age of 46·5 (sd 12·6) years. The majority of the participants of Dutch descent had a high educational level (61·8 %), whereas the greater part of the ethnic minority groups had a low educational level.

Table 1 Participant characteristics

* Only including participants with food outlets present within residential buffers. For the 500 m buffers, n 4337 (91·7 %) of the entire sample, n 1297 (90·5 %) of the participants of Dutch descent, n 937 (93·2 %) of the participants of South-Asian Surinamese descent, n 904 (91·8 %) of the participants of African Surinamese descent, n 544 (92·5 %) of the participants of Turkish descent and n 655 (91·4 %) of the participants of Moroccan descent had at least one food outlet in the neighbourhood environment and were included in the calculation of 500 m buffers. For the 1000 m buffers, n 4617 (97·7 %) of the entire sample, n 1404 (98·0 %) of the participants of Dutch descent, n 983 (97·8 %) of the participants of South-Asian Surinamese descent, n 963 (97·8 %) of the participants of African Surinamese descent, n 573 (97·4 %) of the participants of Turkish descent and n 694 (96·8 %) of the participants of Moroccan descent had at least one food outlet in the neighbourhood environment and were included in the calculation of 1000 m buffers.

Only including participants with fast-food outlets within the residential buffers. For the 500 and 1000 m buffer, n 4697 (99·3 %) of the entire sample, n 1421 (99·2 %) of the participants of Dutch descent, n 1000 (99·5 %) of the participants of South-Asian Surinamese descent, n 980 (99·5 %) of the participants of African Surinamese descent, n 585 (99·5 %) of the participants of Turkish descent and n 711 (99·2 %) of the participants of Moroccan descent had at least one fast-food outlet within the residential buffers.

A higher (positive) score indicates a healthier food environment.

§ A higher (positive) score indicates a higher diversity of food outlets offering core foods as opposed to food outlets selling predominantly non-core foods.

Ethnic differences in dietary quality

Mediterranean diet score

Table 1 shows that the participants of Dutch descent had the highest MDS score (Mean = 33·2, sd = 4·9) compared with the other ethnic groups. Tables 2 and 3 show that this was significantly higher than other ethnic groups. For example, participants of South-Asian Surinamese descent scored 2·84 (se 0·22, 95 % CI −3·27, −2·41) points lower on the MDS than participants of Dutch descent (Table 2). Sensitivity analyses in which we adjusted for educational level indicated that ethnic differences in MDS score remained fairly similar (data not presented).

Table 2 Parallel mediator model summary of the association between ethnicity and Mediterranean diet score via neighbourhood food environment healthiness index (FEHI, 500 m or 1000 m buffers)

FEHI: food environment healthiness index.

Outcomes of path c for the 500 m and 1000 buffers are slightly different because these models only include participants with food outlets present within residential buffers (see footnote Table 1).

Data were obtained from the Hayes Process Macro(Reference Hayes34).

β: linear regression estimates; total β effect (path c) represents the association between ethnicity and Mediterranean diet score with no mediators in the model. Direct β effect (path c’) represents the association between ethnicity and Mediterranean diet score when FEHI (mediator – 500 m or 1000 m) is included in the model.

Indirect effects (paths a and b) represent the association between ethnicity and Mediterranean diet score through FEHI.

Models are adjusted for age, sex and energy intake (in kilocalories).

* P ≤ 0·05.

Dutch Ethnic descent is the reference category/constant (data not presented in the model).

Additionally adjusted for ethnicity.

§ A higher (positive) score indicates a healthier food environment.

Table 3 Parallel mediator model summary of the association between ethnicity and Mediterranean diet score via neighbourhood food environment diversity index (FEDI, 500 m or 1000 m buffers)

FEDI: food environment diversity index.

Outcomes of the c path for the 500 m and 1000 m buffers are slightly different because these models only include participants with food outlets present within residential buffers (see footnote Table 1).

Data were obtained from the Hayes Process Macro(Reference Hayes34).

β: Linear regression estimates; total β effect (path c) represents the association between ethnicity and Mediterranean diet score with no mediators in the model. Direct β effect (path c’) represents the association between ethnicity and Mediterranean diet score when FEDI (mediator – 500 m or 1000 m) is included in the model.

Indirect effects (paths a and b) represent the association between ethnicity and Mediterranean diet score through FEDI.

Models are adjusted for age, sex and energy intake (in kilocalories).

* P ≤ 0·05.

Dutch ethnic descent is the reference category/constant (data not presented in the model).

Additionally adjusted for ethnicity.

§ A higher (positive) score indicates a higher diversity of food outlets offering core foods as opposed to food outlets selling predominantly non-core foods.

Fast-food consumption

Table 1 shows that participants of Turkish descent had the highest fast-food consumption, with a median of 38·1 g/d. Table 4 shows that these participants had a significantly higher fast-food consumption than participants of Dutch descent (β = 0·29, se 0·07, 95 % CI 0·16, 0·42). Participants of African Surinamese descent (β = −0·15, se 0·06, 95 % CI −0·26, −0·05) and Moroccan descent (β = −0·39, se 0·06, 95 % CI −0·51, −0·27) consumed less fast food than participants of Dutch descent. No differences were observed between participants of Dutch and those of South-Asian Surinamese descent. Sensitivity analyses in which we adjusted for educational level indicated that ethnic differences in fast food consumption remained similar (data not presented).

Table 4 Parallel mediator model summary of the association between ethnicity and fast food consumption via neighbourhood fast-food environment (500 m or 1000 m buffers)

* P= <0·05.

Dutch ethnic descent is the reference category/constant (data not presented in the model).

Additionally adjusted for ethnicity.

Outcomes of path c for the 500 m and 1000 buffers are slightly different because these models only include participants with food outlets present within residential buffers (see footnote Table 1).

Data were obtained from the Hayes Process Macro(Reference Hayes34).

β: Linear regression estimates; total β effect (path c) represents the association between ethnicity and fast-food consumption with no mediators in the model. Direct β effect (path c’) represents the association between ethnicity and fast-food consumption when fast-food density (mediator – 500 m or 1000 m) is included in the model.

Indirect effects (paths a, b) represent the association between ethnicity and fast-food consumption through fast-food density.

Models are adjusted for age and sex.

Ethnic differences in neighbourhood food environment characteristics

Food environment healthiness index

Table 1 shows the descriptive statistics of the neighbourhood FEHI. For this food environment proxy, a large variance was observed. The median FEHI was lower among ethnic minority groups than among participants of Dutch origin. For example, participants of Turkish descent had a median FEHI of −240 within a 1000 m residential buffer, whereas this figure was −76·4 for participants of Dutch descent. The results presented in Table 2 show that participants of Turkish descent (β = −0·89, se = 0·15, 95 % CI −1·18, −0·60) and Moroccan descent (β = −0·60, se = 0·14, 95 % CI −1·18, −0·60) had a lower FEHI score than participants of Dutch descent within 500 m residential buffers. With respect to the FEHI scores within the 1000 m buffers, all ethnic minority groups had a lower FEHI score than the participants of Dutch descent. This indicates that the neighbourhood food environment of ethnic minority groups, taking multiple food outlets into account, is less healthy than that of the participants of Dutch descent.

Food environment density index

Table 1 presents the descriptive statistics of neighbourhood FEDI. Within the 500 m buffers, FEDI scores were below 1 (indicating a higher diversity of food outlets selling predominantly non-core foods as opposed to food outlets offering core foods) for all ethnic groups. Within the 1000 m buffers, however, all scores were slightly above 1. The results presented in Table 3 show that participants of Moroccan descent (β = 0·04, se = 0·17, 95 % CI 0·009, 0·077) had a higher FEDI compared with those of Dutch descent within 500 m residential buffers, indicating that there was a slightly higher diversity of shops selling core foods as opposed to shops selling highly processed foods. This was in contrast to participants of South-Asian Surinamese descent (β = −0·03, se = 0·15, 95 % CI −0·06, −0·004). Within the 1000 m residential buffers, statistical differences in the FEDI disappeared between participants of Moroccan and those of Dutch descent. However, participants of South-Asian Surinamese descent (β = −0·05, se = 0·01, 95 % CI −0·08, −0·02), African Surinamese descent (β = −0·05, se = 0·01, 95 % CI −0·08, −0·20) and Turkish descent (β = −0·07, se = 0·02, 95 % CI −0·10, −0·03) had a lower FEDI than those of Dutch descent. This indicates a slightly higher diversity of shops predominantly selling non-core foods compared with shops selling core foods in the 1000 m residential buffer of these ethnic minority groups.

Fast-food outlet density

Table 1 presents the descriptive statistics of the neighbourhood fast-food outlet density. The inter-quartile range shows that there was a wide spread in the number of fast-food outlets within the 500 m and 1000 m buffers. However, the median number of fast-food outlets within the 1000 m buffers was higher among all the ethnic minority groups compared with the participants of Dutch descent. For example, participants of Turkish descent had a median of thirty-two fast-food outlets within a 1000 m residential buffer, whereas the figure was thirteen for participants of Dutch descent. Table 4 shows that participants of Turkish descent (β = 0·73, se = 0·15, 95 % CI 0·44, 1·01 and β = 1·00, se = 0·14, 95 % CI 0·72, 1·27) and Moroccan descent (β = 0·49, se = 0·14, 95 % CI 0·22, 0·76 and β = 0·64, se = 0·13, 95 % CI 0·38, 0·90) had a significantly higher fast-food outlet density within both 500 m and 1000 m residential buffers, respectively, in comparison with participants of Dutch descent. In addition, the 1000 m neighbourhood environment of participants of South-Asian Surinamese descent (β = 0·35, se = 0·12, 95 % CI 0·12, 0·58) had a larger number of fast-food outlets than that of participants of Dutch descent. No differences in neighbourhood fast-food density were observed between participants of Dutch and those of African Surinamese descent.

Neighbourhood food environment–diet quality association

The results (path b) presented in Table 2 (FEHI), Table 3 (FEDI) Table 4 (fast-food density) show the absence of a statistically significant association between the neighbourhood food environment characteristics (FEHI, FEDI/fast-food density) and diet quality (MDS/fast-food consumption) within both the 500 m and the 1000 m buffer.

Contribution of neighbourhood food environment characteristics to ethnic differences in diet quality

The direct effects (c’) remained similar or nearly similar to the total effects (c) when the food environment variables were entered into the model, determining the association between ethnicity and diet quality. Accordingly, the estimates of the indirect effects (paths a, b) of the contribution of the neighbourhood food environment characteristics to ethnic differences in dietary quality were low and non-significant (Tables 24).

Discussion

The results of the current study indicate a difference in diet quality and neighbourhood food environment characteristics between different ethnic groups living in Amsterdam. For example, ethnic minority groups had lower MDS than participants of Dutch origin, and they lived in less healthy and less diverse neighbourhood food environments. In contrast, participants of Dutch origin consumed more fast food than most ethnic minority groups, yet they resided in neighbourhoods with a lower number of fast-food outlets. The neighbourhood food environment characteristics did not contribute to the ethnic differences in dietary quality observed in the current study.

There are a number of possible explanations for our findings. Although the ethnic minority groups included in our study tend to reside in less healthy food environments, all ethnic groups – including participants of Dutch origin – had a relatively 'unhealthy' neighbourhood food environment with a high level of access to fast food, namely a median of thirteen and thirty-two fast-food outlets for participants of Dutch and Turkish descent, respectively, within 1000 m. Thus, the level of access to fast food is high for all residents, which may mean that neighbourhood food environments do not play a direct role in explaining ethnic differences in diet quality. This might be especially the case for dense cities like Amsterdam, where food outlets are highly accessible(35).

Previous studies mentioned the ‘lack of availability of traditional food’ as influencing the dietary behaviour of ethnic minority groups, which could mean that foods are sourced via channels other than the food outlets in the neighbourhood environment(Reference Osei-Kwasi, Nicolaou and Powell36). However, we lack insight into where the food consumed was obtained. Another explanation could be the age of our participants that was on average 46·5 years. It could be speculated that this sample has more traditional dietary habits and may be less influenced by the modern fast-food environment than younger people (e.g. adolescents)(Reference Dynesen, Haraldsdóttir and Holm37). Moreover, determinants other than the neighbourhood food environment – such as family preferences(Reference Nicolaou, Doak and van Dam38), cooking skills(Reference Patel, Ferrer and Tyrer39) or lack of time to prepare food(Reference Lawrence, Devlin and Macaskill40) – explain ethnic differences in diet quality, but were not included in our study.

Another explanation for our findings is that we only assessed neighbourhood food environment (within a < 1000 m range). Although the benefit of this approach is that the exposure of each participant is similar, we could not account for the fact that people are also exposed to and visit food outlets elsewhere(Reference Thornton, Crawford and Lamb41). Moreover, ethnic groups may use their neighbourhood food environment differently, for example, access to transportation for food shopping may differ between ethnic groups, although we do know from prior research that there are no ‘food-deserts’ in the city of Amsterdam(Reference Helbich, Schadenberg and Hagenauer42). Furthermore, as mentioned previously, socio-cultural considerations may also influence the choice of food outlets(Reference Nicolaou, Doak and van Dam38); for example, Muslims may prefer to consume halal fast-foods, which might influence their behaviour with regard to food outlet selection in their neighbourhood. As this may have confounded food environment exposure, future research should seek to better understand the role of socio-cultural considerations in ethnic differences in neighbourhood food environment use and exposure.

Our findings are in line with a study in the UK that did not find evidence of a mediating effect of fast-food availability in ethnic differences regarding fast-food intake(Reference Shareck, Benmarhnia and Berger43). However, in that study, a modification effect of the fast-food environment was observed, whereby ethnic differences in fast-food intake were the greatest in neighbourhoods that lacked fast-food outlets and narrowed as availability increased(Reference Shareck, Benmarhnia and Berger43). Another important consideration is the influence of broader contextual factors in shaping people’s engagement with their neighbourhood food environment. In our study, we extended the conceptualisation of neighbourhood food environment exposure with two comprehensive food environment proxies – namely the FEHI and the FEDI – but we did not incorporate the wider, socio-demographic context. For example, a New Zealand study found that the association between area-level deprivation and diet quality was stronger than that between fast-food availability and diet quality, stressing the importance of reconsidering pathways linking neighbourhood food environments and diet(Reference Hobbs, Green and Roberts44). Furthermore, other food environment aspects, such as food costs, have been found to be important in explaining ethnic differences in diet quality(Reference Mackenbach, Dijkstra and Beulens45).

The results of our study should be viewed in the light of some limitations. First, we used cross-sectional data, while dietary patterns are likely to be shaped by the food environment (and other factors) over a longer period of time, during which the neighbourhood food environment changes. Thus, a single measure of the neighbourhood food environment may not adequately reflect exposure or access to the food environment, which may have had an impact on diet quality. Moreover, the comprehensive food environment measures included (FEHI and FEDI) were based on the insights of experts and researchers and were thus a rather subjective way of indexing the healthiness and diversity of the neighbourhood food environment. Also, the FEDI does not provide insight in the actual core- and non-core foods for sale (e.g. the consumer food environment) and only captures food outlet typology. This should be taken into consideration when interpreting the outcomes. Methodological limitations should also be addressed. As we created decile groups of the FEHI and the fast-food density measures, only an indication of food environment differences and their contribution to ethnic differences in dietary quality could be defined. Finally, other food environment characteristics (e.g. food prices or special offers) or measures (e.g. size of or distance to outlets) were not taken into account, but are important aspects of the food environment.

The strengths of the study include the large sample of five different ethnic groups, the use of ethnic-specific FFQ to assess dietary intake and the use of individual-level exposure of a wide variety of food outlets of different sizes of street network buffers. Although the FFQ render the risk of social desirability bias (due to self-reporting), the FFQ instrument has been shown to be a valid and easy to use tool that provides a reasonably accurate ranking of low to high food intake(Reference Block, Gillespie and Rosenbaum46,Reference Kim Daniel and Holowaty Eric47) . We deliberately did not account for socio-economic position in the main analyses. However, sensitivity analyses in which we adjusted for educational level indicated that ethnic differences in all diet quality proxies remained similar. Future studies could further unravel the interaction between ethnicity and socio-economic position to explain group differences in the influence of the neighbourhood food environment on diet. Another point of consideration is that diet quality is a rather relative concept, and ethnic differences in diet quality depend on the outcome measure included. For example, whereas the MDS was lower among ethnic minority groups than among those of Dutch origin, the opposite was found for the Dutch Healthy Diet Index, a score based on the Dutch dietary recommendations(Reference Yau, Adams and White4). However, we expect that the overall conclusion of our study – namely that the neighbourhood food environment measures did not contribute to ethnic differences in diet quality – would be the same had we used a different diet quality index.

Future research might aim to gain an understanding of intermediate factors of the environment–diet relationship. For example, food purchases are conceptually more directly linked to neighbourhood food environment exposure than dietary intake. However, in our study, we were not able to include those consumed foods that were purchased only within the neighbourhood environment. Moreover, the underlying social-cognitive mechanisms of the potential impact of environmental characteristics on food intake – such as social norms(Reference Van Rongen, Poelman and Thornton48) or cultural perceptions(Reference Thompson, Ponsford and Lewis49) about the food environment and its contribution to food choices – are less explored.

In conclusion, we found no evidence that the neighbourhood food environment contributes to ethnic differences in diet quality because an association between the neighbourhood food environment and dietary quality was absent. Yet, the neighbourhood food environment of ethnic minority groups in Amsterdam was considerably less healthy and diverse than that of participants of Dutch origin. More research is needed to confirm our findings and to better understand how different population groups interact with their neighbourhood food environment as a determinant of dietary quality. For example, future research could investigate other consequences (e.g. implicit cognitions (unconscious influences)) of residing in less supportive neighbourhood food environments, or gain a better understanding of how different ethnic groups make use of their neighbourhood food environment.

Acknowledgements

Acknowledgements: The authors would like to thank Locatus BV (www.locatus.com) for using the database. Also they would like to acknowledge the statistical advise by Dr Henrike Galenkamp. Financial support: MPP is funded by the Innovational Research Incentives Scheme Veni (grant number 451-16-029, project ‘Geographies of food consumption’), financed by the Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research (NWO). JDM is funded by the Dutch Heart Foundation and ZonMW (CVON2016–04) and an NWO VENI grant on ‘Making the healthy choice easier – role of the local food environment’ (grant number 451–17-032). The HELIUS study is conducted by the Amsterdam UMC, University of Amsterdam and the Public Health Service of Amsterdam. Both organisations provided core support for HELIUS. The HELIUS study is also funded by the Dutch Heart Foundation, the Netherlands Organization for Health Research and Development (ZonMw), the European Union (FP7) and the European Fund for the Integration of non-EU immigrants (EIF). The sub-study of dietary intake was funded by The Netherlands Organisation for Health Research and Development, grant number 115100005. These funders had no role in the collection, analysis and interpretation of data; in the writing of the report and in the decision to submit the article for publication. Conflict of interest: There are no conflicts of interest. Authorship: M.P., M.N. and K.S. formulated the research questions. M.P. carried out the analyses of the data and wrote the article. All authors read and provided input and critical feedback on the research questions, results and reflections and approved the manuscript before submitting. Ethics of human subject participation: The current study was conducted according to the guidelines laid down in the Declaration of Helsinki and all procedures involving research study participants were approved by the AMC Medical Ethics Committee approved the HELIUS study (METC 10/100, NL32251.018.10), and written informed consent was obtained from all subjects.

Supplementary material

For supplementary material accompanying this paper visit https://doi.org/10.1017/S1368980021001919

References

Dekker, LH, Nicolaou, M, van Dam, RM et al. (2015) Socio-economic status and ethnicity are independently associated with dietary patterns: the HELIUS-Dietary Patterns study. Food Nutr Res 59, 26317.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brenner, DR, Boucher, BA, Kreiger, N et al. (2011) Dietary patterns in an ethnoculturally diverse population: of young Canadian adults. Can J Diet Pract Res 72, e161e168.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sommer, C, Sletner, L, Jenum, AK et al. (2013) Ethnic differences in maternal dietary patterns are largely explained by socio-economic score and integration score: a population-based study. Food Nutr Res 57, 21164.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yau, A, Adams, J, White, M et al. (2019) Differences in diet quality and socioeconomic patterning of diet quality across ethnic groups: cross-sectional data from the HELIUS Dietary Patterns study. Eur J Clin Nutr 74(3), 387396.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Boer, EJ, Brants, HAM, Beukers, M et al. (2015) Voeding van Marokkaanse, Turkse, Surinaamse en autochtone Nederlanders in Amsterdam (Diet of Moroccan, Turkish, Surinamese and native Dutch in Amsterdam). National Institute for Public Health and Environment. https://www.rivm.nl/bibliotheek/rapporten/2015-0099.pdf (accessed June 2020).Google Scholar
Wang, Y & Chen, X (2011) How much of racial/ethnic disparities in dietary intakes, exercise, and weight status can be explained by nutrition- and health-related psychosocial factors and socioeconomic status among US adults? J Am Diet Assoc 111, 19041911.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Landrine, H & Klonoff, EA (2004) Culture change and ethnic-minority health behavior: an operant theory of acculturation. J Behav Med 27, 527555.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Satia, JA (2010) Dietary acculturation and the nutrition transition: an overview. Appl Physiol Nutr Metab 35, 219223.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nicolaou, M, Benjelloun, S, Stronks, K et al. (2012) Influences on body weight of female Moroccan migrants in the Netherlands: a qualitative study. Heal Place 18, 883891.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bandura, A (1989) Human agency in social cognitive theory. Am Psychol 44, 11751184.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sallis, JF, Owen, N & Fisher, EB (2015) Ecological models of health behavior. In Health Behavior and Health Education Theory Research and Practice, 5th ed., pp. 4364 [Glanz, K, Rimer, BK and Viswanath, K, editors], San Fransisco: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
Swinburn, BA, Kraak, VI, Allender, SP et al. (2011) The global obesity pandemic: shaped by global drivers and local environments. Lancet 378, 804814.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Glanz, K, Sallis, JF, Saelens, BE et al. (2005) Healthy nutrition environments: concepts and measures. Am J Heal Promot 19, 330333.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Black, C, Moon, G & Baird, J (2014) Dietary inequalities: what is the evidence for the effect of the neighbourhood food environment? Health Place 27, 229242.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Timmermans, J, Dijkstra, C, Kamphuis, C et al. (2018) ‘Obesogenic’ school food environments? An urban case study in the Netherlands. Int J Environ Res Public Health 15, 619.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caspi, CE, Sorensen, G, Subramanian, SV et al. (2012) The local food environment and diet: a systematic review. Health Place 18, 11721187.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stronks, K, Snijder, MB, Peters, RJ et al. (2013) Unravelling the impact of ethnicity on health in Europe: the HELIUS study. BMC Public Health 13, 402.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Snijder, MB, Galenkamp, H, Prins, M et al. (2017) Cohort profile: the healthy life in an urban setting (HELIUS) study in Amsterdam, the Netherlands. BMJ Open 7, e017873.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dekker, LH, Snijder, MB, Beukers, MH et al. (2011) A prospective cohort study of dietary patterns of non-western migrants in the Netherlands in relation to risk factors for cardiovascular diseases: HELIUS-Dietary Patterns. BMC Public Health 11, 441.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Willett, W (2013) Nutritional Epidemiology, 3rd ed. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Beukers, MH, Dekker, LH, de Boer, EJ et al. (2015) Development of the HELIUS food frequency questionnaires: ethnic-specific questionnaires to assess the diet of a multiethnic population in The Netherlands. Eur J Clin Nutr 69, 579584.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Visser, M, Elstgeest, LEM, Winkens, LHH et al. (2020) Relative validity of the HELIUS food frequency questionnaire for measuring dietary intake in older adult participants of the longitudinal aging study Amsterdam. Nutrients 12, 1998.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davis, C, Bryan, J, Hodgson, J et al. (2015) Definition of the Mediterranean diet; a literature review. Nutrients 7, 91399153.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dinu, M, Pagliai, G, Casini, A et al. (2018) Mediterranean diet and multiple health outcomes: an umbrella review of meta-analyses of observational studies and randomised trials. Eur J Clin Nutr 72, 3043.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sofi, F, Macchi, C, Abbate, R et al. (2014) Mediterranean diet and health status: an updated meta-analysis and a proposal for a literature-based adherence score. Public Health Nutr 17, 27692782.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Panagiotakos, DB, Pitsavos, C, Arvaniti, F et al. (2007) Adherence to the Mediterranean food pattern predicts the prevalence of hypertension, hypercholesterolemia, diabetes and obesity, among healthy adults; the accuracy of the MedDietScore. Prev Med 335340.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jaworowska, A, Blackham, T, Davies, IG et al. (2013) Nutritional challenges and health implications of takeaway and fast food. Nutr Rev 71, 310318.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caspi, CE, Sorensen, G, Subramanian, SV et al. (2012) The local food environment and diet: a systematic review. Health Place 18, 11721187.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clary, C, Matthews, SA & Kestens, Y (2017) Between exposure, access and use: reconsidering foodscape influences on dietary behaviours. Health Place 44, 17.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vernez Moudon, A, Drewnowski, A, Duncan, GE et al. (2013) Characterizing the food environment: pitfalls and future directions. Public Health Nutr 16, 12381243.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vogel, C, Lewis, D, Ntani, G et al. (2017) The relationship between dietary quality and the local food environment differs according to level of educational attainment: a cross-sectional study. PLoS One 12, e0183700.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thornton, LE & Kavanagh, AM (2012) Association between fast food purchasing and the local food environment. Nutr Diabetes 2, 53.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moayyed, H, Kelly, B, Feng, X et al. (2017) Is living near healthier food stores associated with better food intake in regional Australia? Int J Environ Res Public Health 14, 884.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hayes, AF (2018) Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Analysis. New York, London: The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Statistics Netherlands (2019) Supermarkt en restaurant goed bereikbaar (Supermarkets and restaurants easily accessible). https://www.cbs.nl/nl-nl/achtergrond/2018/48/supermarkt-en-restaurant-goed-bereikbaar (accessed September 2019).Google Scholar
Osei-Kwasi, HA, Nicolaou, M, Powell, K et al. (2016) Systematic mapping review of the factors influencing dietary behaviour in ethnic minority groups living in Europe: a DEDIPAC study. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 13, 85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dynesen, AW, Haraldsdóttir, J, Holm, L et al. (2003) Sociodemographic differences in dietary habits described by food frequency questions – results from Denmark. Eur J Clin Nutr 57, 15861597.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nicolaou, M, Doak, CM, van Dam, RM et al. (2009) Cultural and social influences on food consumption in Dutch residents of Turkish and Moroccan origin: a qualitative study. J Nutr Educ Behav 41, 232241.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Patel, N, Ferrer, HB, Tyrer, F et al. (2017) Barriers and facilitators to healthy lifestyle changes in minority ethnic populations in the UK: a narrative review. J Racial Ethn Heal Disparities 4, 11071119.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lawrence, JM, Devlin, E, Macaskill, S et al. (2007) Factors that affect the food choices made by girls and young women, from minority ethnic groups, living in the UK. J Hum Nutr Diet 20, 311319.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thornton, LE, Crawford, DA, Lamb, KE et al. (2017) Where do people purchase food? A novel approach to investigating food purchasing locations. Int J Health Geogr 16, 9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Helbich, M, Schadenberg, B, Hagenauer, J et al. (2017) Food deserts? Healthy food access in Amsterdam. Appl Geogr 83, 112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shareck, M, Benmarhnia, T, Berger, N et al. (2019) Does the neighborhood food environment contribute to ethnic inequalities in fast-food intake? Findings from the ORiEL study. Prev Med Reports 16, 100998.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hobbs, M, Green, M, Roberts, K et al. (2019) Reconsidering the relationship between fast-food outlets, area-level deprivation, diet quality and body mass index: an exploratory structural equation modelling approach. J Epidemiol Community Health 73, 861866.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mackenbach, JD, Dijkstra, SC, Beulens, JWJ et al. (2019) Socioeconomic and ethnic differences in the relation between dietary costs and dietary quality: the HELIUS study. Nutr J 18, 21.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Block, G, Gillespie, C, Rosenbaum, EH et al. (2000) A rapid food screener to assess fat and fruit and vegetable intake. Am J Prev Med 18, 284288.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim Daniel, J & Holowaty Eric, J (2003) Brief, validated survey instruments for the measurement of fruit and vegetable intakes in adults: a review. Prev Med 36, 440447.Google ScholarPubMed
Van Rongen, S, Poelman, MP, Thornton, L et al. (2020) Neighbourhood fast food exposure and consumption: the mediating role of neighbourhood social norms. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 17, 19. doi: 10.1186/s12966-020-00969-w.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thompson, C, Ponsford, R, Lewis, D et al. (2018) Fast-food, everyday life and health: a qualitative study of ‘chicken shops’ in East London. Appetite 128, 713. https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S019566631830285X (accessed June 2020).CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Fig. 1 Path model diagram of parallel mediation analysis. The path model showing the association between ethnicity (X) and diet quality (Y) as mediated by neighbourhood food environment characteristics (M). Path c represents the association between ethnicity and diet quality with no mediators in the model. Path c’ represents the association between ethnicity and diet quality when the mediator is included in the model. Indirect effects (paths a and b) represent the association between ethnicity and diet quality through the neighbourhood food environment

Figure 1

Table 1 Participant characteristics

Figure 2

Table 2 Parallel mediator model summary of the association between ethnicity and Mediterranean diet score via neighbourhood food environment healthiness index (FEHI, 500 m or 1000 m buffers)

Figure 3

Table 3 Parallel mediator model summary of the association between ethnicity and Mediterranean diet score via neighbourhood food environment diversity index (FEDI, 500 m or 1000 m buffers)

Figure 4

Table 4 Parallel mediator model summary of the association between ethnicity and fast food consumption via neighbourhood fast-food environment (500 m or 1000 m buffers)

Supplementary material: File

Poelman et al. supplementary material

Poelman et al. supplementary material

Download Poelman et al. supplementary material(File)
File 14.4 KB