Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-v9fdk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-13T06:42:27.650Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Roll, right, repeat: short-term repeatability in the self-righting behaviour of a cold-water sea cucumber

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  13 January 2020

Jeff C. Clements*
Affiliation:
Department of Biology, Norwegian University of Science and Technology, Høgskoleringen 5, 7491Trondheim, Norway
Ellen Schagerström
Affiliation:
Department of Biological and Environmental Sciences, University of Gothenburg, Sven Lovén Centre for Marine Infrastructure – Kristineberg, Kristineberg 566, 45178Fiskebäckskil, Sweden Swedish Mariculture Research Centre (SWEMARC), University of Gothenburg, P.O. Box 463, 405 30Gothenburg, Sweden
Sam Dupont
Affiliation:
Department of Biological and Environmental Sciences, University of Gothenburg, Sven Lovén Centre for Marine Infrastructure – Kristineberg, Kristineberg 566, 45178Fiskebäckskil, Sweden
Fredrik Jutfelt
Affiliation:
Department of Biology, Norwegian University of Science and Technology, Høgskoleringen 5, 7491Trondheim, Norway
Kirti Ramesh
Affiliation:
Department of Biological and Environmental Sciences, University of Gothenburg, Sven Lovén Centre for Marine Infrastructure – Kristineberg, Kristineberg 566, 45178Fiskebäckskil, Sweden
*
Author for correspondence: Jeff C. Clements, E-mail: jeffery.clements@dfo-mpo.gc.ca
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

For many benthic marine invertebrates, inversion (being turned upside-down) is a common event that can increase vulnerability to predation, desiccation and unwanted spatial transport, and requires behavioural ‘self-righting’ to correct. While self-righting behaviour has been studied for more than a century, the repeatability (R) – the portion of behavioural variance due to inter-individual differences – of this trait is not well understood. Heritability and the evolution of animal behaviour rely on behavioural repeatability. Here, we examined the self-righting technique of a cold-water holothurid, Parastichopus tremulus, and assessed the repeatability of this behaviour. Under laboratory conditions, P. tremulus consistently used muscle contractions to curl its body and roll itself back to an upright position, which provided for rapid ( ± SD = 96.7 ± 49.8 s) and highly repeatable (R = 0.75) self-righting in the short term that varied between individuals (range of individual average righting times = 34.8–217.0 s). Righting time tended to increase with animal size; however, substantial variation was evident at comparable sizes, as average righting time ranged from 34.8–155.5 s for animals ~20 cm in body length. Contrary to previous studies on other echinoderms, we found no evidence of improved righting times for P. tremulus over time. This study ultimately provides the first detailed documentation of self-righting behaviour for P. tremulus and suggests that this species displays a high degree of repeatability for this trait in the short term.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © Marine Biological Association of the United Kingdom 2020

Introduction

Benthic organisms are often challenged with inversion (i.e. being dislodged from the benthos such that the ventral side of the body is oriented away from the benthos toward the water column) resulting from wave action or from falling off vertical surfaces (Blake, Reference Blake1981). While inverted, these animals are vulnerable to predation, desiccation and unwanted transport due to water currents, all of which can influence survivorship (Penn & Brockmann, Reference Penn and Brockmann1995). For example, Penn & Brockmann (Reference Penn and Brockmann1995) reported that quicker self-righting reduced the probability of stranding for intertidal horseshoe crabs. Furthermore, righting response time is often associated with stress physiology, as stressed animals tend to right slower than less-stressed animals (Lawrence & Cowell, Reference Lawrence and Cowell1996). Consequently, employing ‘self-righting’ strategies (i.e. actively regaining proper orientation) that minimize the time spent inverted is adaptively beneficial for benthic fauna that experience inversion regularly.

Self-righting behaviour is common to many benthic marine invertebrates. In this context, echinoderms are perhaps the most well-studied (Hyman, Reference Hyman1955; Reese, Reference Reese and Boolootian1966). For example, self-righting in sea stars has been extensively studied since the 19th century (Jennings, Reference Jennings1907) and righting responses in sea urchins are well documented (Challener & McClintock, Reference Challener and McClintock2017). As such, righting behaviour in echinoderm taxa can provide a model system for exploring broad questions in behavioural ecology.

An area of contemporary interest for behavioural ecologists is behavioural repeatability. Behavioural repeatability is reflected by within-individual consistency and between-individual variation for a given behaviour (Briffa et al., Reference Briffa, Bridger and Biro2013) and is typically measured as the proportion of phenotypic variance encompassed by inter-individual differences (denoted as R; Roche et al., Reference Roche, Careau and Binning2016). Mathematically, repeatability is calculated as

$$R = s_{A\;}^2 s^2 + s_A^2 $$

where $s_{A\;}^2 $ is inter-individual variance and s 2 is intra-individual variance over time (Bell et al., Reference Bell, Hankison and Laskowski2009). Repeatability is a crucial component of behavioural theories, including animal personality (Gosling, Reference Gosling2001; Roche et al., Reference Roche, Careau and Binning2016), behavioural syndromes (Sih et al., Reference Sih, Bell and Johnson2004a, Reference Sih, Bell, Johnson and Ziemba2004b), temperament (Reale et al., Reference Reale, Reader, Sol, McDougall and Dingemanse2007) and behavioural coping styles (Koolhaas et al., Reference Koolhaas, Korte, de Boer, van der Vegt, van Reenen, Hopster, de Jong, Ruis and Blokhuis1999). Bell et al. (Reference Bell, Hankison and Laskowski2009) suggested that behaviour can be viewed as significantly repeatable at R ≥ 0.37 and studies have since used this threshold as evidence for or against repeatability (e.g. Gutowsky et al., Reference Gutowsky, Brownscombe, Wilson, Szekeres and Cooke2016). Repeatable variation in behaviour between individuals is related to fitness traits such as reproductive success and survival (see Smith & Blumstein, Reference Smith and Blumstein2008 for review) and can be influenced by the environment (Réale & Festa-Bianchet, Reference Réale and Festa-Bianchet2003; Dingemanse et al., Reference Dingemanse, Both, Drent and Tinbergen2004). Furthermore, it is recognized that individual repeatability is a requirement for the heritability of behavioural traits (Falconer & Mackay, Reference Falconer and Mackay1996). As such, behavioural repeatability within individuals can moderate behavioural variation in animal populations and can influence selection for behavioural traits within a population (Réale & Festa-Bianchet, Reference Réale and Festa-Bianchet2003; Dingemanse et al., Reference Dingemanse, Both, Drent and Tinbergen2004).

A variety of behaviours spanning a diverse array of fauna are reported to be repeatable (Bell et al., Reference Bell, Hankison and Laskowski2009) and benthic marine organisms are no exception. For example, repeatability has been widely reported for startle responses in sea anemones (Sinn et al., Reference Sinn, Gosling and Moltschaniwskyj2008; Briffa & Greenaway, Reference Briffa and Greenaway2011; Rudin & Briffa, Reference Rudin and Briffa2012; Briffa et al., Reference Briffa, Bridger and Biro2013). Repeatability has also been documented in the escape response of the queen scallop, Aequipecten opercularis (Laming et al., Reference Laming, Jenkins and McCarthy2013). Self-righting behaviour in echinoderms is also thought to be repeatable (Polls & Gonor, Reference Polls and Gonor1975). Empirical studies of self-righting repeatability in echinoderms are, however, limited to only two recently published studies (to the best of our knowledge). Gutowsky et al. (Reference Gutowsky, Brownscombe, Wilson, Szekeres and Cooke2016) reported repeatable (R = 0.47) and improved self-righting behaviour in the Caribbean sea star, Oreaster reticulatus, while Buccheri et al. (Reference Buccheri, Foellmer, Christensen, Langis, Ritter, Wolf and Freeman2019) found moderately repeatable (R = 0.36) self-righting in tropical holothurids from the Great Barrier Reef.

Although well documented for other echinoderm taxa, studies on self-righting techniques in holothurids (i.e. sea cucumbers) are lacking, with the exception of a few species (Pearse, Reference Pearse1908; Olmsted, Reference Olmsted1917; Berrill, Reference Berrill1966). Furthermore, sea cucumbers are increasingly targeted for aquaculture purposes globally (Lovatelli et al., Reference Lovatelli, Conand, Purcell, Uthicke, Hamel and Mercier2004; Han et al., Reference Han, Kessing and Liu2016), particularly as bottom feeders in Integrated Multitrophic Aquaculture operations (Nelson et al., Reference Nelson, MacDonald and Robonson2012; Zamora et al., Reference Zamora, Yuan, Carton and Slater2018). Therefore, more basic knowledge is needed. Only a single study on self-righting repeatability in holothurids exists to our knowledge (Buccheri et al., Reference Buccheri, Foellmer, Christensen, Langis, Ritter, Wolf and Freeman2019), which reports that self-righting in three species from the Great Barrier Reef was borderline repeatable (R = 0.36). As such, it is largely unclear if holothurid righting behaviour is repeatable. Studies on self-righting behaviour and its repeatability in holothurid echinoderms are thus needed, particularly given the importance of this behaviour in predator avoidance and survival (Penn & Brockmann, Reference Penn and Brockmann1995).

With a global interest in sea cucumber aquaculture, various species are being considered for farming. A common species being explored for aquaculture in Scandinavia is the red sea cucumber, Parastichopus tremulus, a subtidal epibenthic deposit feeder which uses short tentacles for feeding (20 tentacles arranged in a ring and surrounded by papillae). Spine-like papillae are sparsely scattered along a firm body that can reach a length of ~50 cm. Three rows of short tube feet (ambulacral podia; with little elongation capability) line the ventral side of the body, providing for a mobile lifestyle and adherence to hard substrate. In the Gullmar Fjord on the Bohuslän Archipelago of the west coast of Sweden, P. tremulus typically occur at 35–80 m depth where numerous predators exist, including sun stars (Crossaster papposus and Solaster endeca), shrimp (Pandalus borealis), fish (Gadus morhus and Melanogrammus aeglefinus) and (likely) large crabs (Lithoides maja) (Francour, Reference Francour1997). This sea cucumber can occupy soft-bottom areas and is commonly found on silt-covered hard substrates and adhering vertical surfaces (Picton & Morrow, Reference Picton and Morrow2016), from which falling off is common (Blake, Reference Blake1981). Consequently, self-righting is important for predator avoidance, and repeatable variation in individual righting times may play a role in maintaining behavioural variation under different levels of predation pressure (Réale & Festa-Bianchet, Reference Réale and Festa-Bianchet2003).

In this study, we describe (in detail) the self-righting technique employed by this species. We also tested the repeatability of self-righting for adults and assessed whether or not self-righting time was related to body size. We hypothesized that self-righting times for individual P. tremulus would be moderately repeatable (based on results of Buccheri et al., Reference Buccheri, Foellmer, Christensen, Langis, Ritter, Wolf and Freeman2019 for other holothurids) and positively related to body size.

Materials and methods

Animal collection and husbandry

Adult Parastichopus tremulus (N = 17) were collected from the Gullmar Fjord (40–80 m depth) as by-catch in lobster pots and were held for more than one year under laboratory conditions at a nearby public aquarium (Havets Hus, Lysekil, Sweden; 2.2 km from the centre) in flow-through aquaria with seawater (33 m depth) from the fjord. With the exception of two animals which remained at Havets Hus, sea cucumbers from the public aquarium were transferred to the Sven Lovén Centre for Marine Infrastructure – Kristineberg (58.249700°N 11.445074°E; near Fiskebäckskil, Sweden and hereafter referred to as ‘Kristineberg’) in March 2019 (N = 15). They were subsequently held in a flow-through laboratory tank under ambient deep seawater conditions in the fjord (6–9 °C) until May 2019 when the temperature was stabilized at 7°C. One day before the experiment, the seawater temperature was increased to 10°C to match the seasonal increase in natural temperatures at which the animals would be exposed. The animals at Havets Hus (N = 2) were maintained in seawater at 10°C from catch until the self-righting trials. During holding, the animals at both locations were maintained in flow-through seawater at ambient salinity (mean of hourly measurements 33.1 ± 0.8 SD) and fed ad libitum with a mixture of mud and algae (Sargassum thunbergii).

Behavioural experiments

We used all 17 animals including those at Kristineberg (N = 15) and Havets Hus (N = 2). Trials at Kristineberg were conducted on 19 June 2019 and at Havets Hus on 28 June 2019. At both locations, self-righting assays were conducted under constant abiotic conditions with individual animals contained in the same seawater tank; the animals were permitted to feed up until the experiment. Behavioural trials were conducted in the original holding tank to minimize effects of handling on righting behaviour; long-term holding of the animals minimized effects of short-term captivity on self-righting (Bose et al., Reference Bose, Zayonc, Avrantinis, Ficzycz, Fischer-Rush, Francis, Gray, Manning, Robb, Schmidt, Spice, Umedaly, Warden and Côté2019). Individuals were identified visually by tracking distinctive morphological characteristics. For each self-righting trial, individual P. tremulus were gently inverted by hand and the time taken for the animal to right itself was recorded using a stopwatch. An individual was considered righted when both sides of the dorsal surface touched the bottom of the tank. Preliminary observations suggested that the vast majority of individuals righted in less than 3 minutes (180 s). We therefore terminated an individual trial if the animal did not successfully self-right in 5 min and allocated the individual a self-righting time of 300 s; however, this only occurred in a single instance (N = 1 out of 85 observations). A total of five trials were conducted. At Kristineberg, once all animals had righted in a given trial, the next trial commenced immediately and individuals were tested in the same sequence in each trial. This provided ~20 min of recovery time between each trial for each individual, which was comparable to a recent study of echinoderm self-righting repeatability (Gutowsky et al., Reference Gutowsky, Brownscombe, Wilson, Szekeres and Cooke2016). At Havets Hus, we also allowed the two animals ~20 min in between trials for consistency. Once all five trials were completed, the body length of each individual was measured while the animals were in a relaxed state. To describe the behaviour, we first assessed the technique visually and recorded notes on the righting method; after the trials were completed, the righting of a subset of individuals was video recorded to further characterize the behaviour (see Supplementary Files S1 and S2 for videos).

Data analyses

All statistical analyses were conducted using R version 3.5.3 (R Core Team, 2019) with a statistical threshold of α ≤ 0.05. All raw data and annotated R Script can be found in Supplementary Files S3 and S4, respectively. Fixed effects of trial, location (Kristineberg or Havets Hus), their interaction (trial  ×  location), and body length (covariate) on self-righting time were tested by building a linear mixed model (LMM), with individual ID as a random variable, using the lmer function in the lmerTest package (Kuznetsova et al., Reference Kuznetsova, Brockhoff and Christensen2017) and subsequently using the anova function to determine fixed effects. The raw self-righting data initially violated the assumptions of homoscedasticity and residual normality based on visual assessments of Q-Q plots and histograms for normality, and residual fitted plots for homoscedasticity (see Supplementary File S4). The statistical test was therefore conducted on ln-transformed righting time values, which did not violate assumptions (see Supplementary File S4). The relationship between body length and the average self-righting time (individual−1) was further explored using a linear model (LM). Repeatability (R) in self-righting time (and the corresponding 95% confidence interval) was estimated with generalized linear mixed models (GLMM) and Bayesian interpretation according to Dingemanse & Dochtermann (Reference Dingemanse and Dochtermann2013). The GLMM was built using the MCMCglmm() function (MCMCglmm package; Hadfield, Reference Hadfield2010) and the posterior.mode() and HPDinterval() functions were used to estimate repeatability and the corresponding 95% confidence interval (CI), respectively (code adapted from Roche et al., Reference Roche, Careau and Binning2016). The model included the fixed effects of trial, location, their interaction (trial  ×  location), and body length, with individual ID as a random variable; the model therefore provided a measure of adjusted repeatability (accounting for variance associated with fixed effects).

Two previous studies report that echinoderms have the propensity to improve self-righting performance over time (i.e. self-righting times decrease as number of trials increases, which can be indicative of habituation or learning; Migita, Reference Migita and Johnson2012; Gutowsky et al., Reference Gutowsky, Brownscombe, Wilson, Szekeres and Cooke2016). To further explore the propensity for improved self-righting performance, we tested the individual relationships between trial and righting time for statistical significance using linear regression. We then placed individuals into one of three categories: (1) ‘null’ responders, defined as individuals with a non-significant regression slope; (2) ‘worsened’ responders, defined as individuals with a significantly positive slope (i.e. self-righting time increased – or worsened – with increasing trials); and (3) ‘improved’ responders, defined as individuals with a significantly negative slope (i.e. self-righting time decreased – or improved – with increasing trials). The proportion of individuals in each category was calculated as a measure of self-righting improvement.

Results

Our behavioural observations showed that P. tremulus did not use their tube feet to self-right, but rather used longitudinal muscles along the body wall to re-orient themselves to a righted position using a ‘rolling’ motion (Figure 1, Supplementary Files S1 and S2). While not recorded empirically, many individuals tended to curl their body into a ‘C-shape’ prior to rolling (e.g. Figure 1). Rolling appeared to occur simultaneously across the entire body length of the animals and was not initiated by the anterior or posterior end of the body. In all observations of self-righting, each individual employed the same general strategy of righting, suggesting a single method of self-righting for this species.

Fig. 1. Photographic sequence of a self-righting event for an individual Parastichopus tremulus showing the animal: (a) initially inverted; (b) as it began to roll to its right-hand side using muscle contractions; (c) approximately half-way through the righting process; and (d) fully righted.

Individual self-righting times ranged between 25 and 240 s ( ± SD = 96.1 ± 52.7 s) with the exception of a single observation where the individual did not right itself in 5 min (Trial 1; individual righted the anterior end of its body but failed to properly re-orient the posterior end). Results also indicated a substantial degree of repeatability in self-righting for adult P. tremulus, with an adjusted repeatability measure (accounting for effects of trial, location, and body size) of R = 0.75 (95% CI = 0.52–0.87) (Figure 2a). Variation in self-righting time was not significantly related to trial, location, (LMM: F 4,60 = 0.211, P = 0.931), location (LMM: F 1,14 = 0.003, P = 0.961), nor their interaction (LMM: F 4,60 = 0.257, P = 0.904), but was marginally related to body length (LMM: F 1,14 = 4.14, P = 0.061). There was a positive relationship between individual body length and mean righting time (LM: F 1,15 = 6.936, P = 0.019; Figure 2b). Nonetheless, substantial variation existed for similarly sized individuals, as mean righting times for individuals ~20 cm body length ranged from 34.8–155.5 s (Figure 2b).

Fig. 2. (a) Individual (N = 17) self-righting times across the five trials for Parastichopus tremulus. Black points and error bars represent the mean ± 95% CI for self-righting time in each trial. R is the adjusted measure of repeatability (accounting for effects of trial, location and body length) and the values in brackets represent the upper and lower bounds of the 95% CI. (b) The relationship between mean righting time and body length. Each point represents an individual. The grey bar highlights the variation in mean righting time for individuals ~20 cm body length.

Only two of the 17 individuals exhibited a significant linear relationship between trial and righting time, while righting time was not related to trial for most (15/17, 88%). Of those two individuals, one displayed a positive relationship (ID8), while the other displayed a negative relationship (ID6) (Supplementary File S4).

Discussion

Our results suggest that adult P. tremulus employ a single and consistent ‘rolling’ method of self-righting. This method provides reasonably fast righting times that are highly consistent within individuals with an apparent lack of habituation or improvement in the short term. Furthermore, while some of the inter-individual variation in righting times can be explained by body size, there was substantial variation among similarly sized individuals. Collectively, these results suggest that individual adult P. tremulus consistently employ a successful self-righting strategy that varies across individuals and differs from other echinoderms.

Echinoderm self-righting behaviours typically involve, at least to some degree, activity of the tube feet. Tube feet serve multiple functions, including (but not limited to) adhesion, locomotion, feeding (Smith, Reference Smith1937; Flammang, Reference Flammang, Jangoux and Lawrence1996), photoreception (Ullrigh-Lüter et al., Reference Ullrigh-Lüter, Dupont, Arboleda, Hausen and Arnone2011) and self-righting (Lawrence, Reference Lawrence1976a, Reference Lawrence1976b; Ghiold, Reference Ghiold1983; Migita et al., Reference Migita, Mizukami and Gunji2005). The use of tube feet in self-righting requires strong adhesive capability, which is typically associated with the presence of adhesive secretions (Flammang & Jangoux, Reference Flammang and Jangoux1992). Holothurids (including P. tremulus) share a similar tube foot morphology to adhesive-secreting taxa (Smith, Reference Smith1937) and some use tube feet for self-righting (Sclerodactyla briareus: Pearse, Reference Pearse1908); however, we found no evidence of tube feet being involved in the self-righting behaviour of P. tremulus. This is likely due to the fact that P. tremulus only have three rows of tube feet (Barnes, Reference Barnes, Tyler-Walters and Hiscock2008) which are comparably shorter and less extendible than other species that use tube feet for self-righting (E. Schagerstrom, personal observations). While adaptively beneficial for keeping the body close to the substrate and reducing the probability of dislodgement (which suits the ecology of this species), short tube feet would not fare well in self-righting as they would not readily allow P. tremulus to adhere to a horizontal surface when inverted. Similarly, while some holothurids use their tentacles for self-righting (Opheodesoma spectabilis: Berrill, Reference Berrill1966; Synaptula hydriformis: Olmsted, Reference Olmsted1917), we did not observe P. tremulus using tentacles for righting. Like its tube feet, the tentacles of P. tremulus are comparably shorter than other species and are probably not useful for self-righting. Instead, this species used longitudinal muscle contractions to roll itself back to an upright position. This is consistent with Olmsted (Reference Olmsted1917), who noted that, in addition to using tentacles, S. hydriformis would sometimes retract its tentacles, contract its body, and roll to reorient itself upright. Our behavioural observations suggest that morphological constraints in tube feet and tentacle size preclude their use for self-righting in P. tremulus.

Our results align with previous reports suggesting that echinoderm self-righting is repeatable (Gutowsky et al., Reference Gutowsky, Brownscombe, Wilson, Szekeres and Cooke2016; Buccheri et al., Reference Buccheri, Foellmer, Christensen, Langis, Ritter, Wolf and Freeman2019). Interestingly, however, the righting time repeatability for P. tremulus observed in our study was more than two times greater than that reported for holothurids on the Great Barrier Reef (Buccheri et al., Reference Buccheri, Foellmer, Christensen, Langis, Ritter, Wolf and Freeman2019). This may have resulted from the fact that we studied self-righting repeatability in a single species under stable environmental conditions, while Buccheri et al. (Reference Buccheri, Foellmer, Christensen, Langis, Ritter, Wolf and Freeman2019) studied three species under different temperature conditions. We also used a different species than Buccheri et al. (Reference Buccheri, Foellmer, Christensen, Langis, Ritter, Wolf and Freeman2019), which has different biology and perhaps different constraints with respect to self-righting behaviour. Conversely, it may be that attributes of different locations or climate regions result in higher repeatability in holothurid self-righting, or that different species are simply more repeatable than others. Repeatability of self-righting may have fitness consequences for holothurids (Dingemanse & Réale, Reference Dingemanse and Réale2005), as proper orientation can allow for more effective escape responses from potential predators (i.e. increased mobility). For P. tremulus, proper orientation may also provide a degree of camouflage from predators against a sedimentary backdrop, whereas the animals would be more conspicuous while inverted because of their white underside. Spine-like papillae on the dorsal side of the animals may also provide protection from predation. As such, there is likely an adaptive advantage to righting faster and righting times may be heritable, as behavioural responses to mortality factors such as predation are known to be heritable (Åbjörnsson et al., Reference Åbjörnsson, Hansson and Brönmark2004). Studies of heritability and selection on righting response time and its repeatability, however, remain absent from the literature.

Our results suggest that larger individuals tend to take more time to self-right in P. tremulus. The influence of body size on echinoderm self-righting is conflicting in the literature. Gutowsky et al. (Reference Gutowsky, Brownscombe, Wilson, Szekeres and Cooke2016) reported that body size had no effect on righting in O. reticulatus, while larger individuals of the sea urchin, Lytechinus variegatus had slower righting times than smaller conspecifics (Challener & McClintock, Reference Challener and McClintock2017). Other studies with echinoderms also suggest that righting times tend to increase with increasing animal size (e.g. Strongylocentrotus droebachiensis: Percy, Reference Percy1973; S. purpuratus: Sonnenholzner et al., Reference Sonnenholzner, Montaño-Moctezuma and Searcy-Bernal2010). Where present, the effect of body size on righting likely reflects differences in surface area:volume ratios across different animal sizes. Compared with larger individuals, the forces generated by smaller animals are greater relative to their body mass because smaller individuals have a smaller mass per unit of cross-sectional area than larger individuals, resulting in faster self-righting times (Weihs, Reference Weihs and Pedley1977; Schmidt-Nielsen & Duke, Reference Schmidt-Nielsen and Duke1984; Challener & McClintock, Reference Challener and McClintock2017).

Contrary to previous studies on sea stars (Migita, Reference Migita and Johnson2012; Gutowsky et al., Reference Gutowsky, Brownscombe, Wilson, Szekeres and Cooke2016), we found no evidence of improved righting times for P. tremulus under laboratory conditions. Sea stars employ variable methods of self-righting and have a high probability of tangling themselves (Polls & Gonor, Reference Polls and Gonor1975; Gutowsky et al., Reference Gutowsky, Brownscombe, Wilson, Szekeres and Cooke2016). In contrast, holothurids have a more basic and streamlined body plan, perhaps limiting their ability to alter righting responses, as evidenced by the single method of self-righting we observed. In addition, we conducted experiments in the same tank that the animals were held in for >7 months prior to the experiment to avoid confounding effects of captivity and handling (Bose et al., Reference Bose, Zayonc, Avrantinis, Ficzycz, Fischer-Rush, Francis, Gray, Manning, Robb, Schmidt, Spice, Umedaly, Warden and Côté2019), while Gutowsky et al. (Reference Gutowsky, Brownscombe, Wilson, Szekeres and Cooke2016) conducted experiments less than one day after animal collection. Indeed, acclimatization time is reported to affect behavioural repeatability (O'Neill et al., Reference O'Neill, Williamson, Tosetto and Brown2018). Although the time interval between trials in our experiment may not have been sufficiently long to allow improvement (growth of muscles and neurons necessary to confer improvement can take days), studies reporting improved echinoderm righting performance used similar time periods (Migita, Reference Migita and Johnson2012; Gutowsky et al., Reference Gutowsky, Brownscombe, Wilson, Szekeres and Cooke2016). It thus appears that P. tremulus is unable to improve self-righting times, at least in the short term.

Our study comes with some limitations that are important to acknowledge. We only had 17 individuals and for the relationship between size and righting time, there is a gap between ~21–26 cm body length (Figure 2b). Nonetheless, the three individuals of larger body sizes consistently had a righting time >100 s and the slowest individual was indeed the largest individual. It therefore seems like a size relationship probably exists; however, studies with a larger sample size and a broader size range would be beneficial. It is also important to note that the biological state of individuals and surrounding environmental conditions can affect repeatability but were maintained under constant conditions over the short duration of our experiment. In the same vein, we only tested animals from two locations (although they were from the same population) and only had two individuals from one of those locations (Havets Hus). Further experiments testing effects of biological state, environment, and location and population on self-righting repeatability in holothurids and other echinoderms are warranted.

Conclusions

This study documents self-righting behaviour in P. tremulus, suggesting a single method of self-righting. This righting method provided for a high degree of intra-individual repeatability in self-righting times, which varied across individuals (even those of a similar size) in the short term. Our results also suggest that righting times in this species may be a function of body size (smaller animals self-right faster than larger animals), which aligns with numerous other studies in echinoderms and may be a function of differing surface area:volume ratios at different sizes. Finally, contrary to studies in sea stars, we did not find any evidence of improved behaviour and performance in subsequent trials – a discrepancy which could be explained by taxonomy and/or morphology, or by differences in acclimation time to laboratory conditions.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0025315419001218

Data accessibility

All raw data and R code are available as supplementary material.

Acknowledgements

We thank the staff at Havets Hus for the experimental use of their animals, particularly Dr Helen Sköld, Martin Stjernstedt and Jacob Nysveen.

Author contributions

JCC and KR conceived the idea and carried out the experiment. JCC analysed data and wrote the manuscript. ES maintained animals during husbandry and provided lab space for the experiment. SD and FJ provided in-kind support and technical guidance. All authors revised and approved the manuscript.

Financial support

JCC was funded by a Marie Skłodowska-Curie Individual Fellowship through the European Union Horizon 2020 program (Project number 752813), along with a KVA Fund through the University of Gothenburg and an Assemble Plus Grant from the European Marine Biological Resource Centre (EMBRC). ES was supported by the project ‘Seabased Integrated Multitrophic Aquaculture (IMTA) with focus on re-circulation of nutrients’; journal # 2016-4815 funded by European Maritime and Fisheries Fund and Swedish Board of Agriculture. FJ was supported by the Research Council of Norway (FJ: 262942). KR was supported by a Carl Tryggers Fellowship.

Conflict of interest

We declare no conflict of interest.

Footnotes

*

Current address: Aquaculture and Coastal Ecosystems, Fisheries and Oceans Canada, Gulf Fisheries Centre, 343 Université Avenue, P.O. Box 5030, Moncton, NB E1C 9B6, Canada

References

Åbjörnsson, K, Hansson, L-A and Brönmark, C (2004) Responses of prey from habitats with different predator regimes: local adaptation and heritability. Ecology 85, 18591866.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barnes, MKS (2008) A sea cucumber (Parastichopus tremulus). In Tyler-Walters, H and Hiscock, K (eds), Marine Life Information Network: Biology and Sensitivity Key Information Reviews [online]. Plymouth: Marine Biological Association of the United Kingdom. Available at https://www.marlin.ac.uk/species/detail/135 (Accessed 14 August 2019).Google Scholar
Bell, AM, Hankison, SJ and Laskowski, KL (2009) The repeatability of behaviour: a metaanalysis. Animal Behavior 77, 771783.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berrill, M (1966) The ethology of the synaptid holothurian, Opheodesoma spectabilis. Canadian Journal of Zoology 44, 457482.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blake, DB (1981) The new Jurassic sea star genus Eokainaster and comments on life habits and the origins of the modern Asteroidea. Journal of Paleontology 55, 3346.Google Scholar
Bose, APH, Zayonc, D, Avrantinis, N, Ficzycz, N, Fischer-Rush, J, Francis, FT, Gray, S, Manning, F, Robb, H, Schmidt, C, Spice, C, Umedaly, A, Warden, J and Côté, IM (2019) Effects of handling and short-term captivity: a multi-behaviour approach using red sea urchins, Mesocentrotus franciscanus. PeerJ 7, e6556.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Briffa, M and Greenaway, J (2011) High in situ repeatability of behaviour indicates animal personality in the beadlet anemone Actinia equina (Cnidaria). PLoS ONE 6, e21963.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Briffa, M, Bridger, D and Biro, PA (2013) How does temperature affect behaviour? Multilevel analysis of plasticity, personality and predictability in hermit crabs. Animal Behavior 86, 4754.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buccheri, E, Foellmer, MW, Christensen, BA, Langis, P, Ritter, S, Wolf, E and Freeman, AS (2019) Variation in righting times of Holothuria atra, Stichopus chloronotus, and Holothuria edulis in response to increased seawater temperatures on Heron Reef in the Southern GBR. Journal of Marine Biology 2019, 6179705.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Challener, RC and McClintock, JB (2017) In situ measurements of righting behavior in the common sea urchin Lytechinus variegatus: the importance of body size, substrate type, and covering material. Aquatic Biology 26, 3340.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dingemanse, NJ and Dochtermann, NA (2013) Quantifying individual variation in behaviour: mixed-effect modelling approaches. Journal of Animal Ecology 82, 3954.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dingemanse, NJ and Réale, D (2005) Natural selection and animal personality. Behaviour 142, 11591184.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dingemanse, NJ, Both, C, Drent, PJ and Tinbergen, JM (2004) Fitness consequences of avian personalities in a fluctuating environment. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences 271, 847852.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Falconer, DS and Mackay, TFC (1996) Introduction to Quantitative Genetics. Harlow: Longman.Google Scholar
Flammang, P (1996) Adhesion in echinoderms. In Jangoux, M and Lawrence, JM (eds), Echinoderm Studies, Volume 5. Rotterdam: A.A. Balkema, pp. 160.Google Scholar
Flammang, P and Jangoux, M (1992) Functional morphology of the locomotory podia of Holothuria forskali (Echinodermata, Holothuroida). Zoomorphology 111, 167178.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Francour, P (1997) Predation on holothurians: a literature review. Invertebrate Biology 116, 5260.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ghiold, J (1983) The role of external appendages in the distribution and life habits of the sand dollar Echinarachnius parma (Echinodermata: Echinoidea). Journal of Zoology 200, 405419.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gosling, SD (2001) From mice to men: what can we learn about personality from animal research? Psychological Bulletin 127, 4586.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gutowsky, LFG, Brownscombe, J, Wilson, ADM, Szekeres, P and Cooke, SJ (2016) Improved performance, within-individual consistency and between-individual differences in the righting behaviour of the Caribbean sea star, Oreaster reticulatus. Behaviour 153, 17631776.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hadfield, J (2010) MCMC Methods for multi-response generalized linear mixed models: the MCMCglmm package. Journal of Statistical Software 33, 122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Han, Q, Kessing, J and Liu, D (2016) A review of sea cucumber aquaculture, ranching, and stock enhancement in China. Reviews in Fisheries Science and Aquaculture 24, 326341.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hyman, LH (1955) The Invertebrates. Vol. IV: Echinodermata. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Jennings, HS (1907) Behavior of the starfish Asterias forreri de Loriol. University of California Publication of Zoology 4, 53185.Google Scholar
Koolhaas, JM, Korte, SM, de Boer, SF, van der Vegt, BJ, van Reenen, CG, Hopster, H, de Jong, IC, Ruis, MAW and Blokhuis, HJ (1999) Coping styles in animals: current status in behavior and stress-physiology. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 23, 925935.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuznetsova, A, Brockhoff, PB and Christensen, RHB (2017) Lmertest package: tests in linear mixed effects models. Journal of Statistical Software 82, 126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Laming, SR, Jenkins, SR and McCarthy, ID (2013) Repeatability of escape performance in the queen scallop, Aequipecten opercularis. Journal of Experimental Biology 216, 32643272.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lawrence, JM (1976 a) Covering response in sea urchins. Nature 262, 490491.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lawrence, JM (1976 b) On the role of the tube feet and spines in the righting response of sea urchins (Echinodermata: Echinoidea). American Zoologist 16, 228.Google Scholar
Lawrence, JM and Cowell, BC (1996) The righting response as an indication of stress in Stichaster striatus (Echinodermata, Asteroidea). Marine and Freshwater Behaviour and Physiology 27, 239248.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lovatelli, A, Conand, C, Purcell, S, Uthicke, S, Hamel, J-F and Mercier, A (2004) Advances in Sea Cucumber Aquaculture and Management. Rome: Food and Agriculture Organization of the United Nations.Google Scholar
Migita, M (2012) Complexity in the righting behavior of the starfish Asterina pectinifera. In Johnson, C (ed.), Echinoderms in a Changing world: Proceedings of the 13th International Echinoderm Conference, January 5–9, 2009, University of Tasmania, Hobart, Tasmania, Australia. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press.Google Scholar
Migita, M, Mizukami, E and Gunji, Y-P (2005) Flexibility in starfish behavior by multi-layered mechanism of self-organization. Biosystems 82, 107115.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelson, EJ, MacDonald, BJ and Robonson, SMC (2012) A review of the northern sea cucumber Cucumaria frondosa (Gunnerus, 1767) as a potential aquaculture species. Reviews in Fisheries Science 20, 212219.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Olmsted, JMD (1917) The comparative physiology of Synaptula hydriformis (Lesueur). Journal of Experimental Zoology 24, 333379.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O'Neill, SJ, Williamson, JE, Tosetto, L and Brown, C (2018) Effects of acclimatisation on behavioural repeatability in two behaviour assays of the guppy Poecilia reticulata. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 72, 166.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pearse, AS (1908) Observations on the behavior of the holothurian, Thyone briareus (Leseur). Biological Bulletin 15, 259288.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Penn, D and Brockmann, J (1995) Age-biased stranding and righting in male horseshoe crabs, Limulus polyphemus. Animal Behavior 49, 15311539.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Percy, JA (1973) Thermal adaptation in the boreo-arctic echinoid Strongylocentrotus droebachiensis (O. F. Müller, 1776). II. Seasonal acclimatization and urchin activity. Physiological Zoology 46, 129138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Picton, BE and Morrow, CC (2016) Encyclopedia of Marine Life of Britain and Ireland – Parastichopus tremulus (Gunnerus, 1767). Available at http://www.habitas.org.uk/marinelife/species.asp?item=ZB4410 (Accessed 12 August 2019).Google Scholar
Polls, I and Gonor, J (1975) Behavioral aspects of righting in two asteroids from the Pacific coast of North America. Biological Bulletin 148, 6884.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
R Core Team (2019) R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Available at http://www.R-project.org/.Google Scholar
Réale, D and Festa-Bianchet, M (2003) Predator-induced natural selection on temperament in bighorn ewes. Animal Behaviour 65, 463470.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reale, D, Reader, SM, Sol, D, McDougall, PT and Dingemanse, NJ (2007) Integrating animal temperament within ecology and evolution. Biological Reviews 82, 291318.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reese, ES (1966) The complex behavior of echinoderms. In Boolootian, RA (ed.), Physiology of Echinodermata. New York, NY: Interscience Publishers, pp. 157218.Google Scholar
Roche, DG, Careau, V and Binning, SA (2016) Demystifying animal ‘personality’ (or not): why individual variation matters to experimental biologists. Journal of Experimental Biology 219, 38323843.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rudin, FS and Briffa, M (2012) Is boldness a resource-holding potential trait? Fighting prowess and changes in startle response in the sea anemone, Actinia equina. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 279, 19041910.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmidt-Nielsen, K and Duke, JB (1984) Scaling: Why is Animal Size so Important? Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sih, A, Bell, AM and Johnson, JC (2004 a) Behavioral syndromes: an ecological and evolutionary overview. Trends in Ecology and Evolution 19, 372378.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sih, A, Bell, AM, Johnson, JC and Ziemba, RE (2004 b) Behavioral syndromes: an integrative overview. Quarterly Review of Biology 79, 372378.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sinn, DL, Gosling, SD and Moltschaniwskyj, NA (2008) Development of shy/bold behaviour in squid: context-specific phenotypes associated with developmental plasticity. Animal Behavior 75, 433442.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, JE (1937) The structure and function of the tube feet in certain echinoderms. Journal of the Marine Biological Association of the United Kingdom 22, 345357.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, BR and Blumstein, DT (2008) Fitness consequences of personality: a meta-analysis. Behavioral Ecology 19, 448455.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sonnenholzner, JI, Montaño-Moctezuma, G and Searcy-Bernal, R (2010) Effect of three tagging methods on the growth and survival of the purple sea urchins Strongylocentrotus purpuratus. Pan-American Journal of Aquatic Sciences 5, 414420.Google Scholar
Ullrigh-Lüter, EM, Dupont, S, Arboleda, E, Hausen, H and Arnone, MI (2011) Unique system of photoreceptors in sea urchin tube feet. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 108, 83678372.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weihs, D (1977) Effects of size on sustained swimming speeds of aquatic organisms. In Pedley, TJ (ed.), Scale Effects in Animal Locomotion. New York, NY: Academic Press, pp. 333339.Google Scholar
Zamora, LN, Yuan, X, Carton, AG and Slater, MJ (2018) Role of deposit-feeding sea cucumbers in integrated multitrophic aquaculture: progress, problems, potential and future challenges. Reviews in Aquaculture 10, 5774.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Fig. 1. Photographic sequence of a self-righting event for an individual Parastichopus tremulus showing the animal: (a) initially inverted; (b) as it began to roll to its right-hand side using muscle contractions; (c) approximately half-way through the righting process; and (d) fully righted.

Figure 1

Fig. 2. (a) Individual (N = 17) self-righting times across the five trials for Parastichopus tremulus. Black points and error bars represent the mean ± 95% CI for self-righting time in each trial. R is the adjusted measure of repeatability (accounting for effects of trial, location and body length) and the values in brackets represent the upper and lower bounds of the 95% CI. (b) The relationship between mean righting time and body length. Each point represents an individual. The grey bar highlights the variation in mean righting time for individuals ~20 cm body length.

Clements et al. supplementary material

Clements et al. supplementary material 1

Download Clements et al. supplementary material(Video)
Video 5.8 MB
Supplementary material: File

Clements et al. supplementary material

Clements et al. supplementary material 2

Download Clements et al. supplementary material(File)
File 12.9 KB
Supplementary material: File

Clements et al. supplementary material

Clements et al. supplementary material 3

Download Clements et al. supplementary material(File)
File 27 KB

Clements et al. supplementary material

Clements et al. supplementary material 4

Download Clements et al. supplementary material(Video)
Video 5.9 MB