Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-t5tsf Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-14T16:31:32.726Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

How CATIE brought us back to Kansas: a critical re-evaluation of the concept of atypical antipsychotics and their place in the treatment of schizophrenia

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

The subdivision of the class of antipsychotic drugs into two discrete groups – ‘conventional’ (or first generation) and ‘atypical’ (or second generation) – has been adopted as standard, with the latter generally accepted as ‘better’ and widely recommended as automatic first-line choices. However, this perception has been thrown into confusion with the results of large pragmatic trials that failed to identify advantages with the new, more expensive drugs, while identifying worrying tolerability issues. This article explores the origins of ‘atypicality’, its construction on the back of a confusing and weak clinical validator (diminished liability to promote parkinsonism) and how even in relation to the archetypical atypical, clozapine, the uncertain boundaries of drug-induced extrapyramidal dysfunction may be contributing to confusion about ‘efficacy’ and ‘tolerability’. It argues that abandoning atypicality would open up clinical practice to all drugs of a single class of ‘antipsychotics’ and allow for individualised risk/benefit appraisal as a basis for truly tailored treatment recommendations.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © The Royal College of Psychiatrists 2008 

Sometimes, the best way forward is back – to take stock, appraise anew, admit mistakes. This is never easy, especially if shadows haunt the way but, if necessary, is better undertaken sooner than later – and with honesty. It might be argued that there is a strong suggestion that psychiatry has been engaging in sufficient wish-fulfilment to make such a backward step not only useful but essential for quality patient care – and professional validation.

It is over a decade since new antipsychotic drugs were launched to almost universal fanfare. And it did seem there was much to celebrate. On the back of clozapine, the offer was tantalising – not just ‘more’ but ‘better’. It was easy for those reluctant to join the carnival to be so drowned out by the clamour as to feel sceptical of their own scepticism. But now doubt, if not scepticism, has been sown with the results of studies that challenge the perception of new antipsychotics as universally ‘better’ options for people with schizophrenia. So now may be the time to go back and look with honesty at how this situation evolved and where we are left in relation to future prescribing practice.

This article raises fundamental questions about our understanding of the clinical pharmacology of antipsychotic drugs – so fundamental that (as numerous publication rejections attest) they were until recently considered unpublishable, a victim of the censorship of orthodoxy. Using recent evidence, the ideas are presented here in line with the view that challenging orthodoxy is an essential aspect of scientific debate.

Atypicality: its rise and fall and rise again

The idea that some antipsychotics might be ‘novel’ in what they do and ‘atypical’ in how they do it antedates the present generation to bear the ‘atypical’ label (Box 1). Generically, it emerged in the 1970s (Reference Costall and NaylorCostall & Naylor, 1975; Reference Jenner and MarsdenJenner & Marsden, 1979) for compounds that might have broader therapeutic profiles, including against negative (i.e. autistic) features (Reference Elizur and DavidsonElizur & Davidson, 1975), while possessing diminished liability to promote extrapyramidal side-effects. Thioridazine and pimozide were considered in this category but there was resistance to their ‘difference’, and following the widespread withdrawal of clozapine in 1975, the terms were mainly reserved for substituted benzamides, especially the already marketed sulpiride.

Box 1 The seven ages of atypicality: anti-psychotics to which the term ‘atypical’ has been attached

Elderly atypicals

  1. Clozapine

  2. Thioridazine

  3. Pimozide

  4. Sulpiride

Middle-aged

  1. Loxapine

  2. Molindone

  3. Methotrimeprazine (levomepromazine)

Youthful

  1. Risperidone

  2. Olanzapine

  3. Sertindole

  4. Quetiapine

  5. Ziprasidone

Infant

  1. Aripiprazole

Elderly posing as youthful

  1. Zotepine

Middle-aged posing as youthful

  1. Amisulpride

Deceased

  1. Remoxipride

This advocacy was largely theoretically driven by in vitro data from single-dose studies but was challenged by the demonstration that sulpiride's pharmacology seemed more ‘typical’ with chronic (i.e. repeat) administration (Reference Jenner, Hall and MurugaiahJenner et al, 1982). Furthermore, clinical results remained contradictory. In head-to-head comparisons it appeared no worse, but effectively little better, than chlorpromazine (Reference Toru, Shimazonio and MiyasakaToru et al, 1972) or haloperidol (Reference Cassano, Castrogiovanni and ContiCassano et al, 1975). Sulpiride's unusual pharmacokinetics, involving low lipophilicity with poor absorption/brain penetrance, meant that issues of in vivo dose equivalence continued to cloud clinical validation and the issue faded.

Clozapine's rehabilitation was key to establishing optimism in psychopharmacology. The results of the US Multicenter Study (Reference Kane, Honigfeld and SingerKane et al, 1988) were striking, if less impressive than the enthusiasm they generated. The major finding of a significantly greater improvement rate (30%) compared with chlorpromazine and benzatropine (4%) was based on a modest criterion (20% reduction in total score on the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale, PANSS). None the less, the target population – with operationally defined ‘treatment-resistant schizophrenia’ – had pessimistic therapeutic prospects and benefits were welcome whatever their size.

Clozapine's broad in vitro binding spectrum, strikingly different from that of the highly D2-selective sulpiride, offered a further possibility – progress beyond the dopamine hypothesis, which had come to be seen as sterile (Reference CrowCrow, 1987). The way was set for new launches, beginning with risperidone in 1993, where ‘atypical’ was not merely a qualifying adjective within a unified class (‘antipsychotics’) but was elevated to a classificatory term in itself from which difference (group membership) could be inferred. We now had ‘two dichotomous groups’ of antipsychotic (Reference Kinon and LiebermanKinon & Lieberman, 1996).

Challenges to atypicality?

Clozapine could wear an atypical mantle with ease. On other new shoulders this always sat less comfortably. Some reviews found little advantage for new antipsychotics over earlier drugs (Reference Geddes, Freemantle and HarrisonGeddes et al, 2000); others, no proven advantage in treatment-resistant schizophrenia (Reference Chakos, Lieberman and HoffmanChakos et al, 2001); still others, differential efficacy within the group (Reference Davis, Chen and GlickDavis et al, 2003). Notwithstanding this lack of consensus, the perception of ‘added benefit’ translated easily to clinical practice, with new drugs rapidly gaining a dominant first-line position.

A revealing insight into how practice variables might have contributed to the positive perception of atypicality came from Reference Rosenheck, Perlick and BinghamRosenheck et al (2003). In their industry-sponsored study, 309 individuals with schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder were randomly allocated to flexible dose regimes of olanzapine (5–20 mg/day) or haloperidol (5–20 mg/day) and followed over 12 months. To maintain masked status, the haloperidol group were given benzatropine (1–4 mg/day) prophylactically, the olanzapine group an identical dose of placebo. The authors hypothesised that olanzapine would outperform haloperidol on three primary outcomes – symptoms (fewer), quality of life (better), costs (lower).

None of these was supported. Retention and termination due to adverse effects were not different and although olanzapine-treated participants reported modestly reduced akathisia (non-significant after 6 months) and showed significant improvements on some cognitive tests, neither of these translated into improved quality of life. Significantly more olanzapine-treated participants experienced weight gain, and total treatment costs for this group were 4–5 times higher.

The authors attributed this striking lack of difference to enhanced performance of haloperidol compared with that in the regulatory olanzapine studies and proposed that absence of a prophylactic anticholinergic in the design of these earlier studies seriously compromised haloperidol's performance. The inference is that extrapyramidal side-effects can significantly impede therapeutic potential.

The stoutest blow to atypicality comes from the Clinical Antipsychotic Trials of Intervention Effectiveness (CATIE) study (Box 2). This flexible-dose trial, sponsored by the National Institute of Mental Health, compared the effectiveness over 18 months of all new antipsychotics licensed in the USA at study inception (olanzapine, quetiapine, risperidone; ziprasidone was added after licensing in January 2002; aripiprazole, licensed in November 2002, came too late) against the long-established perphenazine (Reference Lieberman, Stroup and McEvoyLieberman et al, 2005). It was hypothesised that significant differences would be evident between drugs in the primary outcome of discontinuation for any cause, taken as a global, pragmatic proxy for efficacy/safety/tolerability. Overall, 74% of the sample discontinued before 18 months, the lowest rate (64%) being with olanzapine (Table 1). Other outcomes also suggested that olanzapine has some modest advantages over its new rivals and the established antipsychotics (Table 2).

Table 1 CATIE: discontinuation rates over 18 months

Drug Mean modal dose, mg/day Patients discontinuing drug, %
Olanzapine 20.1 64
Risperidone 3.9 74
Perphenazine 20.8 75
Ziprasidone 112.8 79
Quetiapine 543.4 82
Overall discontinuation 74

Table 2 CATIE: major findings

Outcome Drug comparison Significant difference?
Time to discontinuation
Any cause and patient decision Olanzapine > quetiapine Yes
Olanzapine > risperidone Yes
Olanzapine > ziprasidone No
Olanzapine > perphenazine No
Lack of efficacy Olanzapine > quetiapine Yes
Olanzapine > risperidone Yes
Olanzapine > perphenazine Yes
Olanzapine > ziprasidone No
Intolerable side-effects All comparisons No
Duration of successful treatment Olanzapine > quetiapine Yes
Olanzapine > risperidone Yes
Olanzapine > perphenazine Yes
Risperidone > quetiapine Yes
Efficacy (PANSS totals/CGI) Improvement all groups: initially greatest with olanzapine, but not sustained

Box 2 Summary of the CATIE study

Pragmatic comparative double-blind randomised trial (18 months) of new antipsychotics (licensed in USA):

  1. olanzapine (7.5–30 mg/day)

  2. quetiapine (200–800 mg/day)

  3. risperidone (1.5–6 mg/day)

  4. ziprasidone (40–160 mg/day)

  5. comparator: perphenazine (8–32 mg/day)

Primary outcome: discontinuation of treatment for any cause (proxy for ‘effectiveness’)

Participants (in five groups):

(PANSS, Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale; CGI, Clinical Global Impression scale)

(Reference Lieberman, Stroup and McEvoyLieberman et al, 2005)

It is, however, adverse effects data that force the considered judgement. Rates of discontinuation owing to intolerable side-effects were greatest with olanzapine (18%), least with risperidone (10%), significantly more olanzapine-treated participants discontinuing because of weight gain (average 2 lb/month) and metabolic effects (9% v. 1–4%), which were striking (online data supplement, Table DS1). Although more participants discontinued perphenazine (8% v .2–4%) because of extrapyramidal side-effects – parkinsonism, akathisia, tardive dyskinesia – there were no significant differences in their incidence (online data supplement, Table DS2).

The limitations of this study have been acknowledged, including its short duration and relatively low completion rates, sample characteristics, exclusion of participants with tardive dyskinesia from the perphenazine arm, choice and doses of study drugs, reliance on intention-to-treat analysis and differences in pre-study treatments (Reference Rosenheck, Swartz and McEvoyRosenheck et al, 2007), but despite these it will surely be seen as seminal to psychopharmacology. The authors were circumspect in their comments, emphasising the limited effectiveness that antipsychotics continue to exert but leaving future patterns of use to ‘how clinicians, patients, families and policy makers evaluate the trade-offs between efficacy and side-effects’ (Reference Lieberman, Stroup and McEvoyLieberman et al, 2005). Within this carefully crafted statement can be deciphered a challenge (see ‘Back to basics’ below).

Advantage might still accrue to new drugs if cost-effectiveness benefits were to be identified. Although several such analyses have been favourable to new drugs, methodological problems cast doubt on the validity of their conclusions (Reference Polsky, Doshi and BauerPolsky et al, 2006). In the CATIE study, perphenazine treatment was less costly than treatment with new drugs (Reference Rosenheck, Leslie and SindelarRosenheck et al, 2006). And CATIE is now defended by a CUtLASS! The Cost Utility of the Latest Antipsychotic Drugs in Schizophrenia Study (Reference Jones, Barnes and DaviesJones et al, 2006) was a pragmatic multicentre study across England funded by the National Health Service's Health Technology Assessment Programme (Box 3). It sought to answer a simple question: are initial acquisition costs of new antipsychotics offset by improvements in health-related quality of life or other savings in patients in whom a change of medication is deemed appropriate? The specific hypothesis was that new antipsychotics would be associated with a clinically significant improvement in quality of life over 1 year compared with older drugs. Not only was this not supported – patients failed to express any preference – but also any (slight) benefits tended to favour older drugs, costs with these being (non-significantly) lower. An informative observation was UK clinicians' infrequent use of haloperidol.

Box 3 Summary of CUtLASS findings for treatment band 1

Band 1 participants were:

  1. eligible if medication change required for clinical reasons

  2. randomised to either first- or second-generation antipsychotic: n = 227

Results at 12-month follow-up

  1. No quality-of-life advantages to second-generation antipsychotics

  2. In terms of quality of life and symptom scores, patients on first-generation anti-psychotics showed a trend towards greater improvement

  3. No clear patient preference

  4. Similar costs

(Reference Jones, Barnes and DaviesJones et al, 2006)

Jones et al stated unequivocally that they were ‘not presenting a null result; the hypothesis that second generation antipsychotics are superior was clearly rejected’. Although ‘surprised’, they concluded that ‘all the data suggest that careful prescribing of FGAs [first-generation antipsychotics], at least in the context of a trial, is not associated with poorer efficacy or a greater adverse effect burden’ (Reference Jones, Barnes and DaviesJones et al, 2006).

Two dichotomous groups?

These data might make some a little flustered so it may be informative to explore the subtle ways by which atypicality achieved a second incarnation.

Atypicality was resurrected enthusiastically despite indications that the corpse was cold. Searching out the unique pharmacological action that would justify the idea that there were indeed ‘two dichotomous groups’ of antipsychotic has consumed vast endeavour, but while many properties of these drugs have been called, no single one has yet received universal acclamation (Reference ReynoldsReynolds, 2004). However, clinical criteria could still give it life. Clozapine provided several pointers (see pp. 23–25) but for drugs developed as ‘first-line’, not ‘treatment-resistance’, agents there was only one – reduced liability to promote extrapyramidal side-effects. In establishing the validity of atypicality, extrapyramidal side-effects means parkinsonism. This was, and remains, the one characteristic that atypical antipsychotics claim to share – within the therapeutic (i.e. antipsychotic) range, a uniquely diminished liability to promote parkinsonism.

Drug-induced parkinsonism – a robust validator?

Chlorpromazine's propensity to promote parkinsonism was recognised from the start and regarded as a clinical manifestation of the desirable pharmacological action. Even after parkinsonism had joined the ranks of adverse, as opposed to therapeutic, effects it was many years before it was viewed negatively. However, drug-induced parkinsonism can be subtle, pervasive, disabling and frequently overlooked or misattributed (Reference Weiden, Mann and HassWeiden et al, 1987). It tends to resolve over time (Reference Marsden, Mindham, Mackay, Bradley and HirschMarsden et al, 1986; Reference Ungvari, Chiu and LamUngvari et al, 1999) and may be modified by wider (especially antimuscarinic) pharmacological properties of the drugs whose D2 antagonism causes it (Reference Miller and HileyMiller & Hiley, 1974). It is a weak and confusing candidate with which to validate a ‘new’ subclass of drugs, especially utilising short-term efficacy alone (Box 4).

Box 4 Antipsychotic-induced parkinsonism in people with non-organic conditions

Various predisposing factors and practice variables determine whether parkinsonism is present and whether it is recognised and recorded:

  1. Increasing age

  2. Individual predisposition (dopamine ‘endowment’ and rate of dopaminergic loss)

  3. Female gender (possibly confounded with dose or other practice variables)

  4. Higher starting doses

  5. Rapid rate of increment

  6. Finishing dose

  7. Duration of exposure (may resolve with time)

  8. Inherent liability of compound (potency/receptor binding profile)

  9. Breadth of concept (any symptom v. syndrome only)

  10. Sensitivity/breadth of examination (including subjective phenomena)

The problem of relying on an adverse effect to validate a new classification is that trial differences between the compound thought to be different and a standard comparator may reflect either genuine pharmacological differences or practice differences in the way the two agents are used. This has haunted interpretation of relevant data from the start and raises some questions relating to the conduct of trials in antipsychotic psychopharmacology.

Qualitative issues in antipsychotic drug appraisal

Psychopharmacology has matured in the past 20 years but remains an infant in the family of science. Problems translating in vitro data to in vivo contexts are great enough but difficulties also lie in trial design and execution which, of necessity, must reflect realities – i.e. any study must first and foremost be ‘doable’. As a result, studies are based on a series of assumptions and compromises that are infrequently acknowledged. The following issues are pertinent to the present discussion.

Choice of comparator

Haloperidol was crowned the gold standard comparator antipsychotic to reflect ‘common practice’, a justification that surprised those wary of its potentially vicious extrapyramidal proclivities. By the late 1980s it was apparently the market leader antipsychotic, although how much stellar sales reflected liberal use in US emergency rooms was never clear. In fact, whatever the position in the USA, butyrophenones were almost certainly not the psychiatric market leader worldwide (Table 3). The choice is the more perverse, however, when receptor binding and clinical characteristics are considered, even superficially – a selective, high-potency compound of known high (perhaps uniquely high) liability to cause extrapyramidal side-effects (Reference OwensOwens, 1999) v. non-selective, in general lower-potency, compounds postulated to have low extrapyramidal side-effects liability. One wonders what odds a betting layman would have obtained on the outcome of such comparisons.

Table 3 International antipsychotic prescribing trends (to 1992)

Medication before trial entry
Oral % Depot %
Phenothiazines 53 Phenothiazines 21.1
Chlorpromazine and thioridazine 21.4 Thioxanthenes 8.9
Butyrophenones 25.8 Butyrophenones 5.9
Haloperidol 23.6

The importance of comparator choice in extrapyramidal side-effects liability was evident in Reference Kane, Honigfeld and SingerKane et al's (1988) multicenter clozapine study, in which simply switching from haloperidol to chlorpromazine, even in doses now considered exceptionally high (up to 1800 mg/day), reduced Simpson–Angus ratings (see ‘Rating methodology’ below) by about 25% (Fig. 1).

Fig. 1 Extrapyramidal side-effect ratings of patients treated with haloperidol + benzatropine then switched to chlorpromazine + benzatropine (Reference Kane, Honigfeld and SingerKaneet al, 1988).

Dose of comparator

The shift of US psychiatrists to high-dose/high-potency antipsychotic regimes began in the 1980s (Reference Baldessarini, Cohen and TeicherBaldessarini et al, 1988). This transatlantic love of high doses even extended to clozapine (Reference Pollack, Lieberman and FleischhackerPollack et al, 1995). Reference Baldessarini, Katz and CottonBaldessarini et al (1984) found that with high-potency drugs, equivalent doses were on average 3.5 times those for patients treated with low-potency drugs. All phase III studies for new antipsychotics used several times the minimum effective dose range for haloperidol, which recent data suggest is as low as 2–5 mg/day (Reference Oosthuizen, Emsley and TurnerOosthuizen et al, 2001), a figure identical to theoretical projections from functional imaging (Reference Kapur, Zipursky and RoyKapur et al, 1997). In fact, this knowledge is nothing new (Reference McEvoy, Hogarty and SteingardMcEvoy et al, 1991). Adopting doses of standard comparator drug based on ‘common practice’ with high-potency drugs in the USA in the late 1980s and early 1990s did not merely set very low the hurdle any new drug had to overcome to establish advantage but had the effect of spiriting the hurdle away altogether.

Although some reviewers concluded that comparator doses played a role in laying down an unlevel playing field (Reference Geddes, Freemantle and HarrisonGeddes et al, 2000), this has been strongly refuted (Reference Davis, Chen and GlickDavis et al, 2003). There are two problems with statistical refutations. First, dose and liability to cause extrapyramidal side-effects may not bear a close linear relationship to one another, particularly with high-potency compounds. Dose–response relationships with antipsychotics are complex (Reference Baldessarini, Cohen and TeicherBaldessarini et al, 1988) and study has resulted in few hard facts. However, it is clinical experience that following dose reductions, such side-effects may resolve suddenly, as if threshold-related (Reference Owens, Stein and WilkinsonOwens, 1998). Second, absolute dosage is important in the expression of extrapyramidal side-effects, but so too is the way ultimate doses are attained. When high-potency antipsychotics are introduced in high dosage to susceptible, drug-naïve individuals, extrapyramidal side-effects emerge rapidly and virtually universally (Reference ChilesChiles, 1978); when initial exposure is low and increments slow, they may not emerge at all, even with high-potency compounds (Reference Rosebush and MazurekRosebush & Mazurek, 1999; Reference Oosthuizen, Emsley and TurnerOosthuizen et al, 2001). As important as where you end dose-wise is where you start (remembering the rise from zero to anything, no matter how small, is probably the greatest increment of all) and how fast you get there (Reference OwensOwens, 1999) (Box 4).

Despite challenge, the issue of inappropriate comparator dosage continues to stalk interpretation of studies claiming extrapyramidal side-effects advantage for new antipsychotics.

Other issues

Trial duration

Duration of drug trials is necessarily limited but when parkinsonism is a focus this can be crucial. Drug-induced parkinsonism tends to resolve over weeks or months, so an advantageous extrapyramidal side-effects profile after 6–8 weeks of an efficacy study does not mean that such short-term benefit will be sustained. Comparing clozapine with chlorpromazine, Reference Lieberman, Phillips and GuLieberman et al (2003) found that significant advantage to clozapine in terms of parkinsonism at 6 months lost significance by 12 months.

Rating methodology

This is crucial. Unlike neurology, psychiatry remains wedded to a single parkinsonism scale, the Simpson–Angus Extrapyramidal Symptoms Rating Scale (Reference Simpson and AngusSimpson & Angus, 1970). Debate remains about its item construction, especially its strong bias towards rigidity, which compared with bradykinesia is a minor feature of drug-induced disorder (Reference OwensOwens, 1999). It further ignores subjective symptomatology. Recent work suggests that the Simpson–Angus is reliable but the traditional cut-off (0.3) is far too low, thus compromising specificity (Reference Janno and HoliJanno et al, 2005), a crucial point in interpretation.

LOCF

Analysis of data using ‘last observation carried forward’ (LOCF) is the standard statistical method to which data from all trials of new drugs was subjected but, it is suggested, this may have contributed advantage to new antipsychotics (Reference RosenheckRosenheck, 2005). Although this has not received unqualified support (Reference Leucht, Engel and BaumlLeucht et al, 2007), there remains an issue about what is the appropriate method by which to analyse data from studies with high drop-out rates that may differentially affect the groups being compared.

Sponsorship

The influence of industry sponsorship in outcomes of clinical trials has been much discussed, with clear evidence that this is a potent factor in attributing advantage to trial compounds, including new antipsychotics (Reference Heres, Davis and MainoHeres et al, 2006).

Efficacy v. effectiveness

When placing trial-based recommendations in meaningful clinical contexts, the distinction between ‘efficacy’ (performance under ‘ideal’ conditions) and ‘effectiveness’ (performance under ‘everyday’ conditions) must be maintained. In addition to the unique contexts in which efficacy is assessed, the less ideal situations of everyday practice must in future be given separate consideration before place in management of an illness can be considered comprehensively adjudged.

And now for something completely different…

It is surprising, considering the elusiveness of a unifying pharmacological property, the weakness of the primary validating parameter and the limitations inherent in clinical appraisal that enthusiasm for things ‘new’ was not balanced by circumspection as to whether they were genuinely a-typical. Some did urge caution in reading too much into ‘atypical’ (Reference Kupfer and SartoriusWorld Psychiatric Association, 2002), but attempts to neutralise its specific implications by alternatives such as ‘new’ or ‘second generation’ only reinforced the perception that what was ‘new’ must indeed be ‘different’, for it still demanded subclassification.

How could such a scantly dressed concept feel so cosy in the psychiatric vernacular? As its foundations weaken, one must point out that the idea has become entrenched in professional consciousness with a surety that, outside religious conviction, is nowadays usually instilled only by the subtle and pervasive workings of those for whom ‘atypical’ always played a strong hand – the marketing men! Maybe this was simply the term that industry adopted to delineate its post-clozapine products, creating in the minds of prescribers ‘clear blue water’ between what was ‘new’ and what was just ‘conventional’, an image powerfully boosted by association with the one thing that was un-typical, clozapine.

This proposition will be too radical for many but is worth debating. Indeed, in a revivified climate of questioning can we turn the spotlight on clozapine too?

Clozapine – enhanced efficacy or enhanced tolerability?

Even those who accept the wounding, if not death, of atypicality will accept that clozapine is different. The US Multicenter Study (Reference Kane, Honigfeld and SingerKane et al, 1988) suggested advantage in three domains:

  1. neurological: reduced liability to extra-pyramidal side-effects (i.e. enhanced tolerability)

  2. negative symptomatology: specific efficacy

  3. positive symptomatology (in treatment resistance): enhanced efficacy.

Effects on extrapyramidal function

The ultimate test of a drug's liability to cause extrapyramidal side-effects would be in people with idiopathic extrapyramidal disease, although in contrast to the frequency of claims for atypicality, there has been striking reluctance to utilise this model.

Two randomised controlled trials clearly demonstrated the antipsychotic benefits of clozapine for psychosis in people with Parkinson's disease, without exacerbation of motor disorders (Reference Friedman, Lannon and CorneliaFriedman et al, 1999; Reference Pollak, Destee and TisonPollak et al, 1999). These effects are sustained (Reference Factor, Friedman and LannonFactor et al, 2001) and may be associated with long-term improvements in mortality (Reference Factor, Feustel and FreidmanFactor et al, 2003). This is a unique profile, for although similar benefits have been claimed for low-dose quetiapine, these come mainly from open or retrospective studies, double-blind prospective data being less encouraging (Reference Rabey, Prokhorov and MoniovitzRabey et al, 2007). There is no evidence that claims for other new drugs stand on this test.

Clozapine's advantage in terms of extrapyramidal tolerability seems unique within its class. But are extrapyramidal advantages restricted just to the neurological sphere or can they influence other areas – i.e. are they domain-specific?

Effects on negative and positive symptomatology – efficacy or tolerability?

Clozapine is associated with reduction in negative symptom scores (Reference Kane, Honigfeld and SingerKane et al, 1988). Interpretation is, however, muddied by the conceptual confusion that has surrounded schizophrenic negativity for the past quarter of a century.

This arose as an unanticipated consequence of Crow's type 1/type 2 hypothesis, which postulates that authentic negative symptoms are based on structural brain changes, and should therefore show ‘a component of irreversibility’ with antipsychotics (Reference CrowCrow, 1980). This was strongly contested and became the most fruitful source of hypothesis-generated research in psychiatry throughout the 1980s. However, testing forced a conceptual shift. Traditionally, schizophrenic negativity (or ‘defect’) was largely considered in higher, broad-based functional domains such as psychosocial and occupational competence, assessed longitudinally, often over years (Reference KantKant, 1943; Reference JilekJilek, 1968). Although some researchers had considered chronic deficits cross-sectionally (e.g. the activity–withdrawal dichotomy (Reference VenablesVenables, 1957; Reference DepueDepue, 1976)), it now became imperative to convert negativity into something symptom-driven, measurable cross-sectionally. Whereas the reliability of this process was established for the many rating scales devised to address the question, the validity of the exercise never was and it remains unclear to what extent the varied clinical states that present ‘negatively’ (Reference Carpenter, Heinrichs and AlphsCarpenter et al, 1985) can be separated by cross-sectional clinical assessment alone.

A major problem is the bradykinesia of drug-induced parkinsonism, which in both its objective and subjective characteristics can be difficult, if not impossible, to distinguish from primary negative schizophrenic states (Reference OwensOwens, 1999) (Table 4). Any drug claiming efficacy in negative states cannot produce change simply by reducing bradykinesia (or other ‘secondary’ phenomena). In this light, even clozapine's benefits are not clear-cut. Reference Breier, Buchanan and KirkpatrickBreier et al (1994) found that improvements in negative states were restricted to those who did not satisfy criteria for ‘deficit’ schizophrenia (Reference Carpenter, Heinrichs and WagmanCarpenter et al, 1988), a concept that, by incorporating time/durability criteria, to some extent reconnects with the traditional idea of schizophrenic negativity.

Table 4 Characterisitics distinguishing parkinsonism from authentic (‘primary’) negative schizophrenic states

Parkinsonism Negative schizophrenia
Facial expression ‘Masked’
Loss of facial contours
Loss of gradations of expression
Eye contact preserved
Staring ‘reptilian’ gaze from reduced blink rate
Parted lips
Fixed through emotional range
‘Empty’/‘vapid’
Avoidance of eye contact
Gradation maintained, variety lost
Mood Dysphoria and depression may or may not be present
Range and depth retained
Range and depth restricted
Speech Loss of pitch, power and intonation
Impaired articulation
Spontaneous generation of words generally preserved
Loss of intonation
Articulation preserved
Reduced word usage
Posture Characteristically mild hyperextension
May show nuchal/thoracic flexion ‘Actively imposed’ disorder
No characteristic change
May be avoidant (‘aversive’) and awkward
Engagement/rapport Preserved Impaired – the ‘brick wall’
n 1493
Age 40.0–40.5 years
Gender 70–76% male
Education 12.0–12.2 years
Never married 57–63%
PANSS totals at entry 74.3–76.4
CGI score at entry 3.9–4.0

Thus, are clozapine's benefits in negative states efficacy or tolerability? With its uniquely benign extrapyramidal side-effects profile, could improvements on switching not reflect the waning of prior extrapyramidal features, especially the subjective component of bradykinesia? Indeed, if negative states rateable on standardised rating scales for negative symptoms can emerge in normal volunteers administered antipsychotics liable to cause extrapyramidal side-effects (Reference Ramaekers, Louwerens and MuntjewerffRamaekers et al, 1999; Reference Artaloytia, Arango and LahtiArtaloytia et al, 2006) should this not be the initial assumption to be disproved rather than the more ambitious, if appealing, alternative so widely accepted? Applying statistical methods to the issue does not address the point that the problem is clinical, not analytical.

This interpretation could be extended to all recently released antipsychotics for which efficacy in negative states has been claimed but poorly substantiated (Reference Keefe, Harvey and LenzenwegerKeefe et al, 1999) – new drugs, regardless of inherent extrapyramidal side-effect profiles, prescribed within much tighter (and equivalently lower) licensed dosages than earlier compounds, setting up better tolerability to create a mirage of efficacy. It might also explain contradictions in the large and complex literature that set out to test Crow's hypothesis, summary of which now forces doubt that anything currently available can deliver efficacy to these stubborn states (Reference Erhart, Marder and CarpenterErhart et al, 2006).

Recently, a consensus group of the US National Institute of Mental Health reiterated that in developing treatments for negative states the distinction between primary and secondary is ‘not essential’ (Reference Kirkpatrick, Fenton and CarpenterKirkpatrick et al, 2006). This implies that the way in which clinical improvements are conceptualised might not be of crucial importance and, contrary to the suggestion here, that clinical confusion does not lie buried deep in a quarter of a century's lack of progress in the therapeutics of negative states.

Would it matter, though, if conceptual confusion were contributing to another fundamental misunderstanding? A further intriguing question is whether clozapine's extrapyramidal tolerability can also explain enhanced efficacy in the positive symptom domain.

There is a long but curiously disregarded literature pointing to escalating antipsychotic doses triggering the law of diminishing returns – sometimes referred to as neuroleptic or behavioural toxicity (Reference Wilkens and MalitzWilkens and Malitz, 1960; Reference Simpson, Varega and HaberSimpson et al, 1976). Recent studies support this, even with clozapine (Reference Pollack, Lieberman and FleischhackerPollack et al, 1995). Reviewing dose–response relationships and blood levels, Reference Baldessarini, Cohen, Teicher, Levy and NinanBaldessarini et al (1990) suggested that the curvilinear relationships most frequently reported may reflect the development of ‘untoward neurological side-effects’. Further, in the immediate pre-clozapine years it was proposed that strategies designed specifically to diminish extrapyramidal side-effects might be associated with clinical improvement in refractory schizophrenia (Reference Opler, Kay and Vital-HerneOpler et al, 1985).

As with benefits in the negative domain, is it not possible – probable even – that improvements in positive symptoms following the switch to clozapine result from removal of the extrapyramidally mediated dysphoria that, when present, drives such symptoms, and hence that they reflect tolerability rather than efficacy? Again, in view of what the literature holds, one wonders why this was not the initial, rather than the by now somewhat radical, supposition.

The view that benefits are benefits however mediated and distinctions between efficacy and tolerability are hardly important is seductive and clinicians in particular might chorus ‘So what?’ in response to the above. Perhaps one of the most important lessons our journey back might teach us, one of the darkest shadows that has haunted our path this past decade, is that we, as clinicians, have a profoundly shallow grasp of the clinical pharmacology of antipsychotics, ignorance that sits comfortably on a bed of ‘so what?’ If this view seems arrogant, ask oneself why we – an entire profession – must now face a dramatic turn-around in our perception of one of our most important therapeutic agents. Conceptualisation bears fundamentally on interpretation of trial data and hence on advice clinicians offer to patients as well as to preclinical colleagues on whose expertise future developments depend. If, at our current crossroads, there are only ‘soft’ lessons to be learned, ‘selective’ implications to salvage, we stand embarrassed indeed.

Back to basics?

None of the recent data challenges the claim of new antipsychotics to a prominent place in the therapeutic armamentarium. However, neither do they support the universal downgrading of compounds that came before. Although clozapine is indeed ‘novel’ (perhaps emblematic of future aspirations), in evidence terms we still have a single class of antipsychotics, each member of which comes with its own balance of advantages and disadvantages. And each can find a place in individual patient care. RIP atypical!

Such simple reappraisal could be self-affirming for doctors increasingly constrained within a suffocating blanket of protocols, guidelines, treatment algorithms, etc., each telling them how treatment ought to be ordered. While the application of evidence-based principles achieves the admirable goal of driving down idiosyncratic care, the claim by which it is most frequently promoted – driving up quality care – appears less sound the more it is scrutinised. Overadherence to guidelines fosters algorithmic, tick-box practice that limits choice, deskills professionals and risks enshrining out-of-date information. Worst of all, it restricts clinicians' responses to those individual, uniquely personal aspects of each patient and each illness. The active imposition of homogeneity on trial populations reflects discordantly against the heterogeneous presentations of everyday practice and some bridging of the two is necessary.

How might current findings be translated into practice? One example comes from drug regulation. Granting a marketing authorisation is not based on detached consideration of efficacy v. safety separately but on their integration into a risk/benefit assessment, a concept in which clinical judgement (i.e. the interposition of context) comprises the added extra. Recent data encourage a repositioning of patient ‘context’ at the heart of treatment planning via individualised risk/benefit appraisal. One example is outlined in Fig. 2, although there are many others. CATIE and the rest have provided a broader set of considerations to enter into this clinical decision-making exercise.

Fig. 2 Some issues in individual risk/benefit appraisal with antipsychotic prescribing (illustrated in relation to first and acute presentations).

Furthermore, viewing antipsychotics as a single class widens enormously the available options, for it is a class that can be looked at from many angles (potency, D2 selectivity, additional cholinergic, histaminic and adrenergic actions, metabolic profile, etc.), the only one less useful than typical/atypical being the chemical groupings by which textbooks still present them.

Individualised risk/benefit appraisal does not negate the value of guidelines but does allow challenge to their increasingly dogmatic assertions and proscriptive options, restricting them, as their name implies, to general frameworks comprising large headings above small detail. Rather than simply highlighting professional weaknesses, recent data can open the way to more sophisticated psychopharmacological practice characterised by individualised care, an essential element of genuine quality that not only offers better prospects for patients but will reinvigorate a key element of psychiatric professionalism – expertise in prescribing – that with the ascent of ‘guidelines’ has tended to wither.

As Dorothy discovered, there is more of worth to be found in your own backyard than in some mythical Land of Oz over the rainbow.

Declaration of interest

D.O. is a former member of the Committee on Safety of Medicines and a current member of the Expert Advisory Panel on Psychiatry and Old Age Psychiatry of the UK Commission on Human Medicines. He has no associations with industry.

MCQs and EMI

  1. 1 The concept of atypicality is founded on:

    1. a limbic as opposed to nigrostriatal selectivity of action

    2. b a reduced liability to promote hyperprolactinaemia

    3. c ‘loose’ receptor-binding properties

    4. d efficacy on negative symptoms

    5. e diminished liability to promote parkinsonism.

  2. 2 According to the CATIE study:

    1. a olanzapine was clearly the most effective antipsychotic

    2. b risperidone was associated with the lowest rate of discontinuation for intolerable side-effects

    3. c perphenazine use was associated with an increased incidence of extrapyramidal side-effects

    4. d cost-effectiveness benefits were evident for ziprasidone

    5. e over 18 months, quetiapine was an ineffective anti-psychotic.

  3. 3 In relation to antipsychotic drugs:

    1. a linear dose–response relationships are the norm

    2. b Crow's type 1/type 2 hypothesis has been disproven

    3. c high lipophilicity and brain penetrance is characteristic of all class members

    4. d liability to extrapyramidal side-effects is independent of rate of dose increment

    5. e high-potency compounds are utilised in equivalently higher doses than low-potency compounds.

  4. 4 With regard to clozapine:

    1. a it was considered ‘atypical’ only after publication of the US Multicenter Study in 1988

    2. b its extrapyramidal tolerability profile is, on current evidence, uniqu1e

    3. c benefits in dopaminomimetic psychoses are offset by increased mortality

    4. d its benefits in treatment-resistant schizophrenia are clinically substantial

    5. e its benefits in negative states extend to those with durable disorder.

EMI

The following EMI, which requires reading outside of this article, offers readers a more taxing self-assessment.

Theme: ‘Risk–benefit appraisal’ in therapeutics

Options

  1. a slow-release trifluoperazine 20 mg at night

  2. b olanzapine (as orodispersible tablets) 10 mg at night

  3. c chlorpromazine 100 mg three times a day + 200 mg at night

  4. d haloperidol 10 mg three times a day

  5. e risperidone 2 mg at night

  6. f quetiapine 50 mg twice a day + 150 mg at night

  7. g perphenazine 4 mg in the morning + 8 mg at night + regular benzodiazepine

  8. h amisulpride 200 mg twice a day + chlorpromazine as required up to 400 mg per day

  9. i sulpiride 200 mg in the morning + 400 mg at night

  10. j zuclopentixol decanoate, 200 mg immediately, aiming for a two-weekly regime

  11. k aripiprazole 15 mg at night

  12. l clozapine, aiming for 100 mg twice a day + 200 mg at night.

Choose TWO of the treatment schedules above as representing the most reasonable initial options for each of the following clinical scenarios:

  1. i A 46-year-old man (BMI 34) who smokes 30 cigarettes a day and admits to drinking 40 units of alcohol a week, presenting with first-episode, late-onset psychosis into which he has no insight.

  2. ii A 19-year-old man, suspicious, restless and sleeping poorly, who committed an unprovoked attack on a stranger before admission and admits to imperative hallucinations ordering him to kill his persecutors.

  3. iii An obese 56-year-old woman with epilepsy well maintained on low dose of depot for many years, presenting with social decline characterised by apathy, withdrawal and deteriorating personal hygiene. On examination, her clothes are food-stained and cigarette burned. Articulation is impaired, she has a postural tremor and marked akathisia.

MCQ answers

1 2 3 4
a F a F a F a F
b F b T b F b T
c F c F c F c F
d F d F d F d F
e T e F e T e F

EMI correct matchings

I ii iii
e, g c, h f, I

Footnotes

* A more comprehensive list of references is available from the author.

References

Artaloytia, F., Arango, C., Lahti, A. et al (2006) Negative signs and symptoms secondary to antipsychotics: a double-blind, randomised trial of a single dose of placebo, haloperidol, and risperidone in healthy volunteers. American Journal of Psychiatry, 163, 488493.Google Scholar
Baldessarini, R. J., Katz, B. & Cotton, P. (1984) Dissimilar dosing with high-potency and low-potency neuroleptics. American Journal of Psychiatry, 141, 748752.Google Scholar
Baldessarini, R. J., Cohen, B. M. & Teicher, M. H. (1988) Significance of neuroleptic dose and plasma level in the pharmacological treatment of psychosis. Archives of General Psychiatry, 45, 7991.Google Scholar
Baldessarini, R. J., Cohen, B. M. & Teicher, M. H. (1990) Pharmacological treatment. In Schizophrenia: Treatment of Acute Psychotic Episodes (eds Levy, S. & Ninan, P.) pp. 61118. American Psychiatric Press.Google Scholar
Breier, A., Buchanan, R. W., Kirkpatrick, B. et al (1994) Effects of clozapine on positive and negative symptoms in outpatients with schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry, 151, 2026.Google Scholar
Carpenter, W. T., Heinrichs, D. W. & Alphs, L. D. (1985) Treatment of negative symptoms. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 11, 440452.Google Scholar
Carpenter, W. T., Heinrichs, D. W. & Wagman, A. M. (1988) Deficit and non-deficit forms of schizophrenia: the concept. American Journal of Psychiatry, 145, 578583.Google Scholar
Cassano, G. C., Castrogiovanni, P. & Conti, L. (1975) Sulpiride versus haloperidol in schizophrenia: a double-blind comparative study. Current Therapeutic Research, 17, 189201.Google Scholar
Chakos, M., Lieberman, J. A., Hoffman, E. et al (2001) Effectiveness of second-generation antipsychotics in patients with treatment-resistant schizophrenia: a review and meta-analysis of randomised trials. American Journal of Psychiatry, 158, 518526.Google Scholar
Chiles, J. A. (1978) Extrapyramidal reactions in adolescents treated with high potency antipsychotics. American Journal of Psychiatry, 135, 239240.Google Scholar
Costall, B. & Naylor, R. J. (1975) Detection of the neuroleptic properties of clozapine, sulpiride and thioridazine. Psychopharmacologia, 43, 6974.Google Scholar
Crow, T. J. (1980) Molecular pathology of schizophrenia: more than one disease process? BMJ, 280, 6668.Google Scholar
Crow, T. J. (1987) The dopamine hypothesis survives, but there must be a way ahead. British Journal of Psychiatry, 151, 460465.Google Scholar
Davis, J. M., Chen, N. & Glick, I. D. (2003) A meta-analysis of the efficacy of second-generation antipsychotics. Archives of General Psychiatry, 60, 553564.Google Scholar
Depue, R. A. (1976) An activity-withdrawal distinction in schizophrenia: behavioural, clinical, brain damage and neuropsychological correlates. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 85, 174185.Google Scholar
Elizur, A. & Davidson, S. (1975) The evaluation of the anti-autistic activity of sulpiride. Current Therapeutic Research, 18, 578584.Google Scholar
Erhart, S. M., Marder, S. R. & Carpenter, W. T. (2006) Treatment of schizophrenia negative symptoms: future prospects. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 32, 234237.Google Scholar
Factor, S. A., Friedman, J. H., Lannon, M. C. et al (2001) Clozapine for the treatment of drug-induced psychosis in Parkinson's disease: results of the 12 week open label extension in the PSYCLOPS trial. Movement Disorders, 16, 135139.Google Scholar
Factor, S. A., Feustel, P. J., Freidman, J. H. et al (2003) Longitudinal outcome of Parkinson's disease patients with psychosis. Neurology, 60, 17561761.Google Scholar
Friedman, J., Lannon, M., Cornelia, C. et al (1999) Low-dose clozapine for the treatment of drug-induced psychosis in Parkinson's disease. New England Journal of Medicine, 340, 757763.Google Scholar
Geddes, J., Freemantle, N., Harrison, P. et al (2000) Atypical antipsychotics in the treatment of schizophrenia: systematic overview and meta-regression analysis. BMJ, 321, 13711376.Google Scholar
Heres, S., Davis, J., Maino, K. et al (2006) Why olanzapine beats risperidone, risperidone beats quetiapine, and quetiapine beats olanzapine: an exploratory analysis of head-to-head comparison studies of second-generation antipsychotics. American Journal of Psychiatry, 163, 185194.Google Scholar
Janno, S., Holi, M. M. et al (2005) Validity of Simpson-Angus Scale (SAS) in a naturalistic schizophrenia population. BMC Neurology, 5. doi: 10.1186/1471-2377-5-5.Google Scholar
Jenner, P. & Marsden, C. D. (1979) The substituted benzamides – a novel class of dopamine antagonists. Life Sciences, 25, 479486.Google Scholar
Jenner, P., Hall, M. D., Murugaiah, K. et al (1982) Repeated administration of sulpiride for three weeks produces behavioural and biochemical evidence for cerebral dopamine receptor supersensitivity. Biochemical Pharmacology, 31, 325328.Google Scholar
Jilek, W. G. (1968) The residual dimension: a study of residual syndromes in veterans with chronic psychiatric illness. Psychiatria Clinica, 1, 175191.Google Scholar
Jones, P. B., Barnes, T., Davies, L. et al (2006) Randomised controlled trial of the effect on quality of life of second- vs first-generation antipsychotic drugs in schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 63, 10791087.Google Scholar
Kane, J., Honigfeld, G., Singer, J. et al (1988) Clozapine for the treatment-resistant schizophrenia: a double-blind comparison with chlorpromazine. Archives of General Psychiatry, 45, 789796.Google Scholar
Kant, O. (1943) Clinical analysis of schizophrenic deterioration. Psychiatric Quarterly, 17, 426445.Google Scholar
Kapur, S., Zipursky, R., Roy, P. et al (1997) The relationship between D2 receptor occupancy and plasma levels on low dose oral haloperidol: a PET study. Psychopharmacology, 131, 148152.Google Scholar
Keefe, R. S., Harvey, P. D., Lenzenweger, M. F. et al (1999) Efficacy and extrapyramidal side-effects of the new antipsychotics olanzapine, quetiapine, risperidone and sertindole compared to conventional antipsychotics and placebo: a meta-analysis of randomised controlled trials. Schizophrenia Research, 35, 5168.Google Scholar
Kinon, B. J. & Lieberman, J. A. (1996) Mechanisms of action of atypical antipsychotic drugs: a critical analysis. Psychopharmacology, 124, 234.Google Scholar
Kirkpatrick, B., Fenton, W. S., Carpenter, W. T. et al (2006) The NIMH-MATRICS consensus statement on negative symptoms. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 32, 214224.Google Scholar
Leucht, S., Engel, R. R., Bauml, J. et al (2007) Is the superior efficacy of new generation antipsychotics an artifact of LOCF? Schizophrenia Bulletin, 33, 183191.Google Scholar
Lieberman, J. A., Phillips, M., Gu, H. et al (2003) Atypical and conventional antipsychotic drugs in treatment-naïve first-episode schizophrenia: a 52-week randomised trial of clozapine vs chlorpromazine. Neuropsychopharmacology, 28, 9951003.Google Scholar
Lieberman, J. A., Stroup, T. S., McEvoy, J. P. et al (2005) Effectiveness of antipsychotic drugs in patients with chronic schizophrenia. New England Journal of Medicine, 353, 12091223.Google Scholar
Marsden, C. D., Mindham, R. H. & Mackay, A. V. (1986) Extrapyramidal movement disorder produced by antipsychotic drugs. In The Psychopharmacology and Drug Treatment of Schizophrenia (eds Bradley, P. & Hirsch, S.) pp. 340402. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
McEvoy, J. P., Hogarty, G. E. & Steingard, S. (1991) Optimal dose of neuroleptic in acute schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 48, 739745.Google Scholar
Miller, R. J. & Hiley, C. R. (1974) Anti-muscarinic properties of neuroleptics and drug-induced parkinsonism. Nature, 248, 596597.Google Scholar
Oosthuizen, P., Emsley, R. A., Turner, J. et al (2001) Determining the optimal dose of haloperidol in first-episode psychosis. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 15, 251255.Google Scholar
Opler, L. A., Kay, S. R. & Vital-Herne, J. (1985) Combined mesoridazine and amantadine treatment in refractory schizophrenics. Current Therapeutic Research, 37, 318325.Google Scholar
Owens, D. C. (1998) The drug treatment of schizophrenia. In Seminars in General Adult Psychiatry: Volume 1 (eds Stein, G. & Wilkinson, G.) pp 381449. Gaskell.Google Scholar
Owens, D. G. C. (1999) A Guide to the Extrapyramidal Side-Effects of Antipsychotic Drugs. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Pollack, S., Lieberman, J. A., Fleischhacker, W. W. et al (1995) A comparison of European and American dosing regimens of schizophrenic patients on clozapine: efficacy and side-effects. Psychopharmacology Bulletin, 31, 315320.Google Scholar
Pollak, P., Destee, A., Tison, F. et al (1999) Clozapine in drug-induced psychosis in Parkinson's disease. Lancet, 353, 20412042.Google Scholar
Polsky, D., Doshi, J. A., Bauer, M. S. et al (2006) Clinical trial-based cost-effectiveness analysis of antipsychotic use. American Journal of Psychiatry, 163, 20472056.Google Scholar
Rabey, J. M., Prokhorov, T., Moniovitz, A. et al (2007) Effect of quetiapine in psychotic Parkinson's disease patients: a double-blind labelled study of 3 months duration. Movement Disorders, 22, 313318.Google Scholar
Ramaekers, J. G., Louwerens, J. W., Muntjewerff, N. D. et al (1999) Psychomotor, cognitive, extrapyramidal, and affective functions of healthy volunteers during treatment with an atypical (amisulpride) and a classic (haloperidol) antipsychotic. Journal of Clinical Psychopharmacology, 19, 209221.Google Scholar
Reynolds, G. P. (2004) Receptor mechanisms in the treatment of schizophrenia. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 18, 340345.Google Scholar
Rosebush, P. I. & Mazurek, M. F. (1999) Neurologic side effects in neuroleptic-naïve patients treated with haloperidol or risperidone. Neurology, 52, 782785.Google Scholar
Rosenheck, R. (2005) Effectiveness versus efficacy of second-generation antipsychotics: haloperidol without antipsychotics as a comparator. Psychiatric Services, 56, 8592.Google Scholar
Rosenheck, R., Perlick, D., Bingham, S. et al (2003) Effectiveness and cost of olanzapine and haloperidol in the treatment of schizophrenia: a randomised controlled trial. JAMA, 290, 26932702.Google Scholar
Rosenheck, R., Leslie, D., Sindelar, J. et al (2006) Cost-effectiveness of second-generation antipsychotics and perphenazine in a randomised trial of treatment for chronic schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry, 163, 20802089.Google Scholar
Rosenheck, R., Swartz, M., McEvoy, J. et al (2007) Second-generation antipsychotics: reviewing the cost-effectiveness component of the CATIE trial. Expert Review of Pharmacoeconomics and Outcomes Research, 7, 103111.Google Scholar
Simpson, G. M. & Angus, J. W. S. (1970) A rating scale for extrapyramidal side-effects. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica Supplementum, 212, 1119.Google Scholar
Simpson, G. M., Varega, E. & Haber, E. J. (1976) Psychotic exacerbations produced by neuroleptics. Diseases of the Nervous System, 37, 367369.Google Scholar
Toru, M., Shimazonio, Y., Miyasaka, M. et al (1972) A double-blind comparison of sulpiride with chlorpromazine in chronic schizophrenia. Journal of Clinical Pharmacology, 12, 221229.Google Scholar
Ungvari, G. S., Chiu, H., Lam, L. et al (1999) Gradual withdrawal of long-term anticholinergic antiparkinson medication in Chinese patients with chronic schizophrenia. Journal of Clinical Psychopharmacology, 19, 141148.Google Scholar
Venables, P. H. (1957) A short scale for rating ‘activity-withdrawal’ in schizophrenics. Journal of Mental Science, 103, 197199.Google Scholar
Weiden, P. J., Mann, J. J., Hass, G. et al (1987) Clinical non-recognition of neuroleptic-induced movement disorders: a cautionary study. American Journal of Psychiatry, 144, 11481153.Google Scholar
Wilkens, B. & Malitz, S. (1960) Some problems of dose variations in the use of tranquillising drugs. American Journal of Psychiatry, 117, 2329.Google Scholar
World Psychiatric Association (2002) The usefulness and use of second-generation antipsychotic medications (eds Kupfer, D. & Sartorius, N.) Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 15 (suppl. 1), S1S51.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1 CATIE: discontinuation rates over 18 months

Figure 1

Table 2 CATIE: major findings

Figure 2

Table 3 International antipsychotic prescribing trends (to 1992)

Figure 3

Fig. 1 Extrapyramidal side-effect ratings of patients treated with haloperidol + benzatropine then switched to chlorpromazine + benzatropine (Kaneet al, 1988).

Figure 4

Table 4 Characterisitics distinguishing parkinsonism from authentic (‘primary’) negative schizophrenic states

Figure 5

Fig. 2 Some issues in individual risk/benefit appraisal with antipsychotic prescribing (illustrated in relation to first and acute presentations).

Supplementary material: PDF

Owens supplementary material

Supplementary Table S1-S2

Download Owens supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 61.1 KB
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.