Introduction
Bullying in youth is a major societal and public health problem linked to significant psychological (Vaillancourt et al., Reference Vaillancourt, Hymel and McDougall2013), economic (Wolke et al., Reference Wolke, Copeland, Angold and Costello2013), and societal harms (Jantzer et al., Reference Jantzer, Schlander, Haffner, Parzer, Trick, Resch and Kaess2019). Understood as aggressive social behaviors that involve the targeting of peers, often repetitively, to create or maintain an imbalance of power (Volk et al., Reference Volk, Dane and Marini2014), bullying remains common (Hong & Espelage, Reference Hong and Espelage2012) and one of the most damaging youth behaviors (Gladden et al., Reference Gladden, Vivolo-Kantor, Hamburger and Lumpkin2014). Victimization in youth has been shown to lead to commonly occurring psychopathology, such as anxiety (Siegel et al., Reference Siegel, La Greca and Harrison2009), depression (Klomek et al., Reference Klomek, Marrocco, Kleinman, Schonfeld and Gould2008), and suicidality (Takizawa et al., Reference Takizawa, Maughan and Arseneault2014), making the need to understand how to reduce perpetration an important goal. However, how to treat bullying and other destructive peer behaviors in youth, in conjunction with other types of commonly occurring psychopathology, has not been well explicated. Peer relationship behaviors in youth (e.g., bully perpetration, victimization, generalized aggression) have shared relationships to many symptoms of psychopathology (Parker et al., Reference Parker, Rubin, Erath, Wojslawowicz and Buskirk2015; Prinstein & Giletta, Reference Prinstein and Giletta2016); therefore, evaluating how common childhood symptoms interact with aggressive peer behaviors may provide valuable insights in guiding treatment and in designing interventions.
Anxiety is the most common diagnosis in pediatric samples (Kessler et al., Reference Kessler, Berglund, Demler, Jin, Merikangas and Walters2005), is marked by deviations in many executive functions (Shanmugan et al., Reference Shanmugan, Wolf, Calkins, Moore, Ruparel, Hopson, Vandekar, Roalf, Elliott, Jackson, Gennatas, Leibenluft, Pine, Shinohara, Hakonarson, Gur, Gur and Satterthwaite2016), and presents symptomatically as excessive worry (Weisberg, Reference Weisberg2009). Irritability, defined by excessive anger and temper outbursts (Leibenluft, Reference Leibenluft2017), is a common transdiagnostic symptom in childhood and adolescence (Copeland et al., Reference Copeland, Brotman and Costello2015). Irritability has been consistently linked with many prevalent childhood emotional problems, including internalizing syndromes, like pediatric anxiety (Stoddard et al., Reference Stoddard, Stringaris, Brotman, Montville, Pine and Leibenluft2014), and externalizing problems, including conduct and oppositional defiant disorder (Evans et al., Reference Evans, Burke, Roberts, Fite, Lochman, Francisco and Reed2017; Humphreys et al., Reference Humphreys, Schouboe, Kircanski, Leibenluft, Stringaris and Gotlib2019), though internalizing and externalizing syndromes are often not strongly associated with one another (Juvonen et al., Reference Juvonen, Graham and Schuster2003). While anxiety is often not significantly related to perpetration (Perino et al., Reference Perino, Guassi Moreira and Telzer2019), irritability is often related to perpetration and other aggressive behaviors (Chen et al., Reference Chen, Gardner, Clarkson, Eaton, Wiggins, Leibenluft and Jarcho2021; Humphreys et al., Reference Humphreys, Schouboe, Kircanski, Leibenluft, Stringaris and Gotlib2019). Intriguingly, recent work examining the relationship between anxiety, irritability, and bullying in early adolescence found that anxiety, while not correlated with perpetration, significantly moderated the relationship between adolescent irritability and perpetration. The link between irritability and bully perpetration decreased as anxiety increased (Chen et al., Reference Chen, Gardner, Clarkson, Eaton, Wiggins, Leibenluft and Jarcho2021), suggesting that decreases in anxiety, without co-occurring reductions in irritability, could theoretically lead to increases in adolescent perpetration behaviors.
Uncovering why anxiety could impact links between irritability and aggressive behaviors could influence treatment decisions for adolescents presenting with certain symptoms. One potential explanation previously put forth is that bully perpetration behaviors are reflective of an antisocial interpersonal strategy (Juvonen & Ho, Reference Juvonen and Ho2008), where youth look for opportunities to gain resources at the expense of other peers (Vaillancourt et al., Reference Vaillancourt, Hymel and McDougall2003). Perpetration is linked to being viewed as a leader by one’s peers (Vaillancourt et al., Reference Vaillancourt, Hymel and McDougall2003), increased social status (Hawley et al., Reference Hawley, Little and Card2007; Rose et al., Reference Rose, Swenson and Waller2004), intact socio-emotional intelligence (Garandeau & Lansu, Reference Garandeau and Lansu2019; Kaukiainen et al., Reference Kaukiainen, Björkqvist, Lagerspetz, Österman, Salmivalli, Rothberg and Ahlbom1999), and lower emotional distress (Juvonen et al., Reference Juvonen, Graham and Schuster2003). Developmentally, adolescence is a period of time where youth increase approach-oriented behaviors (McCormick & Telzer, Reference McCormick and Telzer2017) and preferentially respond to rewards (Galván, Reference Galván2013), particularly social ones (Perino et al., Reference Perino, Miernicki and Telzer2016; Somerville et al., Reference Somerville, Jones and Casey2010). If bully perpetration reflects an “approach” oriented behavioral strategy (Kokkinos et al., Reference Kokkinos, Voulgaridou and Markos2016) used to gain certain types of social resources or advantages (Perino et al., Reference Perino, Guassi Moreira and Telzer2019; Vaillancourt et al., Reference Vaillancourt, Hymel and McDougall2003; Volk et al., Reference Volk, Camilleri, Dane and Marini2012), then increasing levels of anxiety, implicated in behavioral inhibition (Vervoort et al., Reference Vervoort, Wolters, Hogendoorn, De Haan, Boer and Prins2010), may reduce perpetration when all other symptoms are held constant. In short, bully perpetration behaviors may be thought of as approach behaviors used to meet social needs (Hawley, Reference Hawley2002, Reference Hawley2003a; Hawley, Reference Hawley2003b; Hawley, Reference Hawley2015); therefore, symptoms which impact approach motivation systems may paradoxically decrease the relationship between co-occurring psychopathology and perpetration.
While the findings described above are suggestive, there are open questions about the relations among anxiety, irritability, and adolescent aggression. Given the emphasis on bullying as a social strategy in the literature, it is possible that any moderating effect of anxiety may be specific to social anxiety symptoms, rather than generalized anxiety (Chen et al., Reference Chen, Gardner, Clarkson, Eaton, Wiggins, Leibenluft and Jarcho2021). Replicating the original moderation and clarifying whether social anxiety is the specific driver is of great import. Additionally, bully perpetration is just one form of aggressive behaviors seen in peer relationships, so clarifying whether anxiety moderates the relationship between only irritability and bullying, or impacts other aggressive peer behaviors, such as generalized aggression, is needed. Significant factor analytic work (Espelage & Holt, Reference Espelage and Holt2001) has shown that bullying is distinct from other, more generalized forms of aggression, such as fighting and disagreeableness (Espelage et al., Reference Espelage, Low, Rao, Hong and Little2014, Reference Espelage, Van Ryzin and Holt2018; Hawley et al., Reference Hawley, Stump and Ratliff2010). However, the bullying measure (Jarcho et al., Reference Jarcho, Grossman, Guyer, Quarmley, Smith, Fox, Leibenluft, Pine and Nelson2019) previously used included items related to fighting, which may be more related to generalized aggression (Espelage & Holt, Reference Espelage and Holt2001; Hawley et al., Reference Hawley, Stump and Ratliff2010; Juvonen et al., Reference Juvonen, Graham and Schuster2003; Vaillancourt et al., Reference Vaillancourt, Hymel and McDougall2003). Understanding how anxiety impacts associations of irritability with different forms of aggression comprehensively will better elucidate expected outcomes when symptoms change.
In this manuscript, we sought to replicate the finding that anxiety moderates the link between irritability and bully perpetration, while clarifying whether the effect is specific to social anxiety or generalized anxiety. We explored relations between irritability, anxiety symptoms (social, general), and aggressive peer relationship behaviors (bully perpetration, generalized aggression), as well as interaction effects of anxiety on the relationship between irritability and aggression. We hypothesized that anxiety would moderate the relationship between irritability and perpetration, but that the effect was specific to social anxiety, rather than generalized anxiety. Additionally, we attempted to determine whether the moderation effect differed for different types of aggressive childhood behaviors (bully perpetration, generalized aggression). The results of these analyses aim to improve our understanding of how to better intervene, and what to expect when interventions are applied in these commonly co-occurring behavioral problems in youth.
Methods
Participants
The sample used in this study was drawn from an ongoing longitudinal study, the Preschool Depression Study, (Luby et al., Reference Luby, Belden, Pautsch, Si and Spitznagel2009) conducted at Washington University School of Medicine. Preschool-aged participants (between ages 3 and 6 years) were recruited from primary care facilities and preschools/daycares in the surrounding metropolitan area from pamphlets about “assessing emotional development.” Parents who responded to promotional material were screened by trained research assistants via telephone interview, to recruit child participants (i) with internalizing psychopathology (endorsement of ≥ 2 symptoms of depression), as well as participants (ii) without psychopathology and (iii) with externalizing psychopathology (endorsement of ≥ 2 symptoms of externalizing psychopathology [ADHD, ODD, or CD]). Participants were excluded if there was evidence of (i) chronic medical illnesses, (ii) neurological problems, (iii) pervasive developmental disorders, or (iv) language/cognitive delays that would impact the ability to answer questionnaires. (Luby et al., Reference Luby, Heffelfinger, Mrakotsky, Brown, Hessler, Wallis and Spitznagel2003) Consent and assent was collected from all participants who completed assessments and all protocols were approved by the Washington University Institutional Review Board.
Measures
Assessment of clinical symptoms of anxiety
Upon screening and consent, participants and parents were assessed via semi-structured interviews by Master’s level raters for psychopathology using the Kiddie-Schedule of Affective Disorders – Present and Lifetime Version (KSADS-PL) (Kaufman et al., Reference Kaufman, Birmaher, Brent, Ryan and Rao2000). The KSADS-PL is a semi-structured clinician-rated assessment derived from interviews with both parents and children to assess psychopathology in youth, which has been shown to be reliable and valid for childhood psychiatric diagnoses (Kaufman et al., Reference Kaufman, Birmaher, Brent, Rao, Flynn, Moreci, Williamson and Ryan1997). Master’s level raters were trained to reliability by an experienced clinician (J.L) and to ensure reliability, all interviews were audiotaped and calibration was provided on 20% of each raters’ cases. (Luby & Belden, Reference Luby and Belden2008) Participants completed a baseline assessment at age 3-6, and were subsequently invited back to continue completing assessments of cognitive and social skills and psychopathology every 1–2 years (Gaffrey et al., Reference Gaffrey, Barch, Singer, Shenoy and Luby2013).
To match the protocol of Chen et al., Reference Chen, Gardner, Clarkson, Eaton, Wiggins, Leibenluft and Jarcho2021, we focused our analyses on study wave 12, which focused on early adolescence (mean age 12). Social anxiety and generalized anxiety scores were tabulated by summing symptoms of each from the K-SADS module. The Social anxiety score includes six items (e.g., “is your child shy, fearful in social situations, or uncomfortable with people they don’t know well”) which were marked as either present or not present, while the generalized anxiety score includes nine items (e.g., “does your child worry, have somatic complaints, or have over concern about competence”) which were also marked as present or not present. Positive endorsements were added to create sum scores for social anxiety and generalized anxiety.
Irritability
Consistent with the assessment of anxiety, we utilized measures from study wave 12 to assess for irritability. Specifically, we used an irritability measure previously validated within the sample (Vogel et al., Reference Vogel, Jackson, Barch, Tillman and Luby2019) that took a factor analytic approach using items from the clinician-administered Preschool Age Psychiatric Assessment (PAPA) (Egger et al., Reference Egger, Angold, Small and Copeland1999) (and later the Childhood and Adolescent Psychiatric Assessment:CAPA) (Angold et al., Reference Angold, Prendergast, Cox, Harrington, Simonoff and Rutter1995) to differentiate irritability from other forms of emotion dysregulation. Irritability items were from the depression, mania, and conduct modules of the PAPA and CAPA, and included items pertaining to irritability intensity, frequency, spontaneity, irritability concern to caretakers, tearful and crying, angry or resentful intensity and temper tantrum intensity.
Bullying behaviors
Bully role behaviors were assessed using a fourteen item composite measure derived from the Parent Report of the Health and Behavior Questionnaire (Essex et al., Reference Essex, Boyce, Goldstein, Armstrong, Kraemer, Kupfer and Group2002). Specifically, we conceptualized bullying behaviors as done in the Illinois Bully Scale (Espelage & Holt, Reference Espelage and Holt2001) and separately assessed for bully perpetration, generalized aggression, and victimization. The perpetration factor (α = .73) consisted of five items (e.g., taunts and teases peers; is cruel, bullies, is mean to others) and each item was rated on a 0–2 scale; the generalized aggression factor (α = .68) consisted of 5 items (e.g., temper tantrums; kicks, bites, or hits other children, gets in many fights) and each item was rated on a 0–2 scale; and the victimization factor (α = .83) consisted of four items (e.g., is actively picked on; is teased and ridiculed) and each item was rated on a 1–4 scale.
Analytic approach
To characterize the sample, we explored relationships between our demographic, clinical (social anxiety, generalized anxiety, irritability), and bully role (bully perpetration, generalized aggression, victimization) continuous variables. We focused exclusively on the behavioral and clinical assessments at the timepoint that most closely matched the sample from Chen et al., Reference Chen, Gardner, Clarkson, Eaton, Wiggins, Leibenluft and Jarcho2021, which was (Timepoint 12 [T12]). A total of 169 participants at T12 were assessed for psychopathology and social behaviors in the current analysis. To reduce biases introduced by including missing data in analyses (Woods et al., Reference Woods, Davis-Kean, Halvorson, King, Logan, Xu, Bainter, Brown, Clay, Cruz, Elsherif, Gerasimova, Joyal-Desmarais, Moreau, Nissen, Schmidt, Uzdavines, Van Dusen and Vasilev2021), we imputed missing data using linear regression (5 iterations) using all the variables included in our analyses (age, sex, race, bullying behaviors, anxiety scores). Descriptive statistics for original and imputed variables are shown in Table 1.
First, we ran zero-order correlations between our variables, to assess primary relationships between psychopathology and bully role behaviors. As an additional step, we ran independent t-tests to determine if sex was significantly related to the aforementioned continuous variables. Next, we used regression-based moderation analyses to explore whether anxiety symptoms were significant moderators of the relationship between irritability and bully perpetration and the relationship between irritability and victimization, while controlling for age, sex, and race. We also aimed to expand upon the interactions explored in Chen et al., Reference Chen, Gardner, Clarkson, Eaton, Wiggins, Leibenluft and Jarcho2021 by exploring whether there was a differential interaction when looking at the relationship between irritability and generalized aggression. To graphically explore interactions, we ran Johnson–Neyman tests (Johnson & Neyman, Reference Johnson and Neyman1936) to determine data ranges where anxiety significantly moderates the relationships between irritability and aggressive behaviors. Finally, given that bully role behaviors (perpetration, generalized aggression, victimization) are correlated and do not appear in isolation, we ran additional interaction models to control for the shared variance between bullying behaviors and more clearly extrapolate relationship between irritability and specific types of aggressive behaviors. We again ran Johnson–Neyman tests (Johnson & Neyman, Reference Johnson and Neyman1936) to determine data ranges where anxiety significantly moderates the relationships between irritability and aggressive behaviors.
All descriptive statistics, correlations, t-tests, and imputations were run using SPSS (Version 28.0; IBM SPSS, Armonk, NY). All interactions were completed using the PROCESS macro for SPSS (Hayes, Reference Hayes2016). Johnson–Neyman statistics and graphics were run using the Interactions R Toolkit (Long, Reference Long2019).
Results
Relationships between demographic, clinical, and bully role behaviors
Bully role behaviors were significantly correlated to clinical variables, consistent with past research. Perpetration was positively related with irritability (r = .403, p < .001) but negatively correlated with social anxiety (r = −.185, p = .016) and unrelated to generalized anxiety (r = −.021, p = .791). Generalized aggression was positively correlated with irritability (r = .535, p < .001) but not significantly correlated with social (r = .013, p = .869) or generalized (r = .145, p = .059) anxiety. Irritability was significantly correlated with generalized anxiety (r = .366, p < .001) but not social anxiety (r = .046, p = 552, see Table 2 for full results and supplemental Figure 1A–C to see distribution of bully role behaviors). When running independent samples t-tests, generalized aggression (t(167) = 2.223, p = .028) and irritability (t(167) = 2.190, p = .030) were the only variables with significant relations to sex; for both generalized aggression (male mean = 0.23, SD = 0.26; female mean = 0.15, SD = 0.20), and irritability (male mean = 39.88, SD = 8.26, female mean = 37.35, SD = 6.56) males scored higher than females.
1. Irritability, 2. Number of Social Phobia Symptoms, 3. Number of Generalized Anxiety Symptoms, 4. Bullying Perpetration, 5. Generalized Aggression, 6. Bullying Victimization, 7. Age. *p<.05, **p < .01, ***p < .001.
Moderation effects of anxiety on bully role behaviors
Our first two regression-based moderation analyses explored whether social anxiety or generalized anxiety significantly moderated the relationship between irritability and bully perpetration. As we hypothesized, there was a significant interaction term between irritability and social anxiety (t(162) = −2.11, b = −.0096, p = .036, ΔR 2 = .0198, F(1, 162) = 4.465), such that as social anxiety increased, the association between irritability and bully perpetration decreased. Using the Johnson–Neyman method, we observed that there was a significant positive association between irritability and bully perpetration at low levels of social anxiety, but no association between irritability and bully perpetration at higher levels of social anxiety. Specifically, irritability was positively, significantly associated with levels of perpetration when there were less than 0.92 social anxiety symptoms (CI [.000, .0157], p = .050, see Fig. 1).
When we ran our regression-based moderation analysis using generalized anxiety symptoms instead of social anxiety symptoms, we observed that there was no significant interaction term (t(162) = .22, b = .0006, p = .822, ΔR 2 = .0006, F(1, 162) = 0.051). Additional moderation analyses exploring whether anxiety moderated the relationship between irritability and victimization were nonsignificant for both social anxiety (t(162) = .049, b = .0005, p = .961, ΔR 2 = 0, F(1, 162) = 0.0024) and generalized anxiety (t(162) = .581, b = .0035, p = .561, ΔR 2 = 0018, F(1, 162) = 0.338).
Next, we explored whether generalized aggression was similar to bully perpetration in that it was moderated by anxiety. However, we found that neither social anxiety (t(162) = .27, b = .0010, p = .789, ΔR 2 = .0003, F(1, 162) = 0.072) nor generalized anxiety (t(162) = .28, b = .0006, p = .777, ΔR 2 = .0003, F(1, 162) = 0.080) significantly moderated the relationship between irritability and generalized aggression.
Given that only social anxiety, and not generalized anxiety, significantly moderated the relationship between irritability and bully behaviors, we focused solely on social anxiety in our follow-up set of moderation analyses. When examining the link between irritability and bully perpetration, while additionally controlling for generalized aggression and victimization, the moderation effect of social anxiety was still significant (t(160) = −2.94, b = −.01, p = .0038, ΔR 2 = .0229, F(1, 160) = 8.635); specifically, irritability was now significantly negatively associated with perpetration when there was endorsement of more than 1.12 social anxiety symptoms (see Fig. 2a). Social anxiety still did not significantly moderate the link between irritability and generalized aggression (t(160) = 1.85, b = .0056, p = .065, ΔR 2 = .009, F(1, 160) = 3.429) when additionally controlling for bully perpetration and victimization. For illustrative purposes, we provide the Johnson–Neyman plot showing that the link between irritability and generalized aggression is significantly positive at all levels of social anxiety (see Fig. 2b).
Discussion
The current study found that as social anxiety symptoms increased, the link between irritability and bully perpetration became more negative. Without accounting for other bully role behaviors, we observed that as adolescents’ endorsement of social anxiety symptoms increased, the link between irritability and bully perpetration decreased and became nonsignificant. When accounting for other bully role behaviors (generalized aggression, victimization), we observed that as there was greater endorsement of social anxiety symptoms, the association between irritability and bully perpetration actually became significantly negative. The moderating of anxiety on the link between irritability and bully perpetration was seen with social anxiety – but not generalized anxiety – and these effects on the relationship between irritability to bully role behaviors was specific to bully perpetration, and not generalized aggression nor victimization. Bully perpetration was negatively related to social anxiety but positively related to irritability. Irritability was positively related to both generalized aggression and victimization; however, neither generalized aggression nor victimization was related to anxiety nor did anxiety moderate the link between irritability and these behaviors. Our results suggest that symptoms of psychopathology have complex associations with bully role behaviors, and that changes in one domain of psychopathology could impact the manifestation of a broad set of social behaviors.
We replicated some, but not all of the findings previously reported by Chen et al. (Reference Chen, Gardner, Clarkson, Eaton, Wiggins, Leibenluft and Jarcho2021). We also found a significant relationship between irritability and victimization and found that anxiety did not moderate the link between irritability and victimization. We did not find that generalized anxiety significantly moderated the relationship between irritability and perpetration (Chen et al., Reference Chen, Gardner, Clarkson, Eaton, Wiggins, Leibenluft and Jarcho2021); however, we did find that social anxiety symptoms moderated this relationship. Chen et al. (Reference Chen, Gardner, Clarkson, Eaton, Wiggins, Leibenluft and Jarcho2021) did not dissociate whether particular types of anxiety were driving moderation effects, so it is unclear if social anxiety was also responsible for the moderation they observed. The observation that social anxiety significantly negatively moderates the link between irritability, a transdiagnostic symptom (Klein et al., Reference Klein, Dougherty, Kessel, Silver and Carlson2021), and perpetration to the point where perpetration becomes negatively associated with irritability is quite interesting. It suggests that even though irritability predicts perpetration, the presence of social anxiety may blunt this expression.
Such a finding is consistent with the hypothesis that bully perpetration reflects a social strategy, and that symptoms which reduce social approach behaviors (e.g., social anxiety) may reduce expected links between psychopathology (such as irritability) and perpetration (Thomas et al., Reference Thomas, Connor and Scott2018). Bully perpetration has been shown to confer certain types of social advantages, such as increased numbers of romantic partners (Provenzano et al., Reference Provenzano, Dane, Farrell, Marini and Volk2018; Volk et al., Reference Volk, Dane, Marini and Vaillancourt2015), winning competitive endeavors (Dane et al., Reference Dane, Lapierre, Andrews and Volk2022), gaining social status (Spadafora et al., Reference Spadafora, Al-Jbouri, McDowell, Andrews and Volk2022), and deterring rivals (Cairns et al., Reference Cairns, Cairns, Neckerman, Gest and Gariepy1988). While irritability is related to aggression writ large (Humphreys et al., Reference Humphreys, Schouboe, Kircanski, Leibenluft, Stringaris and Gotlib2019), bullying is unique in that it is targeted, goal-directed and inherently about social position (Volk et al., Reference Volk, Dane and Marini2014). We posit that social anxiety acts as an inhibitory force, and when irritability is kept constant, increasing social anxiety will lead to reduced perpetration. On the other hand, other aggressive behaviors likely stem from frustration-intolerance or impulse control difficulties and not social goals (Little et al., Reference Little, Henrich, Jones and Hawley2003). As expected, social anxiety did not moderate the association between irritability and other aggressive behaviors, which were positively associated with irritability at all levels of social anxiety. Therefore, if irritability is held constant, increasing social anxiety may blunt the link between irritability and perpetration but remain positively associated with generalized aggression.
Further explicating how changes in symptom levels may impact the ecology of peer-networks broadly, and bully perpetration specifically, may be an important factor to consider when evaluating treatment efficacy (Gaffney et al., Reference Gaffney, Ttofi and Farrington2021). Addressing psychopathology in individuals may lead to positive, measurable effects for some behaviors, while counter-intuitively creating negative effects in others, suggesting a need for researchers to comprehensively assess psychopathology and social behaviors rather than focus on individual syndromes or behavioral phenotypes. While treating social anxiety in adolescents will have myriad positive outcomes (Fisher et al., Reference Fisher, Masia-Warner and Klein2004), it is imperative to also address underlying irritability, lest improvements in one domain (anxiety) potentially lead to decrements in another (less prosocial behaviors). Additionally, given the link of irritability to other adverse social behaviors (e.g., victimization), understanding whether interventions have impacts across a wide variety of domains is imperative. Furthermore, bully perpetration behaviors are heterogenous (Farrell et al., Reference Farrell, Della Cioppa, Volk and Book2014) and evolving (Waasdorp et al., Reference Waasdorp, Pas, Zablotsky and Bradshaw2017) so further research is necessary to determine whether the moderating effect of social anxiety on linkages between irritability and bully perpetration are universal or differentially impact specific bullying behaviors. For example, bullying that doesn't require an audience (with a romantic partner) or cyberbullying, which may provide anonymity, may be less impacted by social anxiety compared to relational perpetration. The answers to these questions may help determine how to best target individual-level interventions based on behavioral phenotype.
This study needs to be considered in light of its limitations. The recruitment and assessment protocols used in this longitudinal study (Luby et al., Reference Luby, Belden, Pautsch, Si and Spitznagel2009) resulted in their being ample participants with symptoms of psychopathology. However, our measures of anxiety (clinician-rated symptoms) (Kaufman et al., Reference Kaufman, Birmaher, Brent, Ryan and Rao2000) and bully role behaviors (composite measure) (Essex et al., Reference Essex, Boyce, Goldstein, Armstrong, Kraemer, Kupfer and Group2002) would have benefitted by utilizing alternative information sources, such as peer reports and self-reports (Makol et al., Reference Makol, Youngstrom, Racz, Qasmieh, Glenn and De Los Reyes2020). It is currently unclear if the relations reported here would equally apply to all forms of bullying. For example, cyberbullying, which often requires less direct contact and may provide perpetrators with anonymity and physical distance from victims may demonstrate weaker links with social anxiety. This hypothesis was not testable in our dataset, but is worthy of further inquiry. Additionally, given the relatively small distribution of scores in our measures of anxiety, using measures with greater distribution across trait levels may help improve statistical assessments. To increase the precision of our moderation analyses examining the links between irritability and bully perpetration, we controlled for demographic variables and co-occuring bully role behaviors. We observed that social anxiety interaction effect accounted for 2% of the model variance, which would be considered a small effect. While our own findings partially replicate prior work, the need to replicate these effects with higher powered samples is paramount. While this work adds vital information by examining generalized aggression in addition to perpetration, it is important for future work to comprehensively assess how associations with psychopathology relate to other types of bully role behaviors, such as prosocial behavior (i.e., bystander intervention) (Jenkins et al., Reference Jenkins, Snyder Kaminski and Miller2021). We hypothesize that increasing social anxiety may moderate the relationship between irritability and other approach-oriented behaviors, be they antisocial or prosocial, and suggest this as a needed line of inquiry.
This study demonstrates that social anxiety significantly impacts the relationship between irritability and bully perpetration. Bully perpetration is a persistent (Espelage & Swearer, Reference Espelage and Swearer2003) and damaging (Brimblecombe et al., Reference Brimblecombe, Evans-Lacko, Knapp, King, Takizawa, Maughan and Arseneault2018) problem, and current interventions have positive, albeit small, effects (Fraguas et al., Reference Fraguas, Díaz-Caneja, Ayora, Durán-Cutilla, Abregú-Crespo, Ezquiaga-Bravo, Martín-Babarro and Arango2021). Treating common symptoms can impact the relationship between perpetration and other related symptoms, which may ultimately provide informative insight into how symptom change can have cascading effects on reducing various forms of antisocial behavior.
Supplementary material
The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579424000439.
Acknowledgments
The authors thank the children and caregivers recruited for the Preschool Depression Study for their time and commitment.
Funding statement
All phases of this study were supported by a National Institutes of Health (NIH) grant, R01 MH064769-06A1. Dr Perino’s work was supported by NIH grant: K99HD105002/R00HD105002 (PI: Perino). Dr Vogel was supported by NIH grant: T32 MH100019 (PI’s: Barch and Luby).
Competing interests
None.