Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-lnqnp Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T19:53:36.304Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Response to Morina et al.'s criticisms of Horigome et al.'s recent report on the efficacy of virtual reality exposure therapy for social anxiety disorder

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  19 October 2021

Toshiro Horigome
Affiliation:
Department of Neuropsychiatry, Keio University School of Medicine, Tokyo, Japan
Shunya Kurokawa
Affiliation:
Department of Neuropsychiatry, Keio University School of Medicine, Tokyo, Japan
Kyosuke Sawada
Affiliation:
Department of Neuropsychiatry, Keio University School of Medicine, Tokyo, Japan
Shun Kudo
Affiliation:
Department of Neuropsychiatry, Keio University School of Medicine, Tokyo, Japan
Kiko Shiga
Affiliation:
Department of Neuropsychiatry, Keio University School of Medicine, Tokyo, Japan
Masaru Mimura
Affiliation:
Department of Neuropsychiatry, Keio University School of Medicine, Tokyo, Japan
Taishiro Kishimoto*
Affiliation:
Department of Neuropsychiatry, Keio University School of Medicine, Tokyo, Japan Keio University School of Medicine, Hills Joint Research Laboratory for Future Preventive Medicine and Wellness, Tokyo, Japan Donald and Barbara Zucker School of Medicine at Hofstra/Northwell, New York, USA
*
Author for correspondence: Taishiro Kishimoto, E-mail: tkishimoto@keio.jp
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Type
Correspondence
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

While research on virtual reality exposure therapy (VRET) for social anxiety disorder (SAD) has been conducted, large-scale studies remain scarce. We found that there were more studies than previously reported in meta-analyses, and we have included these newly identified studies in our analysis to understand the effectiveness of VRET interventions (Horigome et al., Reference Horigome, Kurokawa, Sawada, Kudo, Shiga, Mimura and Kishimoto2020).

In response to our paper, Morina, Kampmann, Emmelkamp, Barbui, and Hoppen (Reference Morina, Kampmann, Emmelkamp, Barbui and Hoppen2021) published a paper stating that ‘Horigome and colleagues misleadingly imply that there is a larger body of literature on the efficacy of VRET for SAD than there actually is.’ We would like to thank the authors who have been working in this area for bringing our paper to their attention and starting a discussion. Reading the criticisms by Morina et al. (Reference Morina, Kampmann, Emmelkamp, Barbui and Hoppen2021), we can assume that their goals are different from ours. While they tried to examine the effects of VRET as purely as possible using only studies comparing the effects of VRET and in vivo exposure, we wished to perform a broader examination of whether interventions using VR can have a therapeutic effect on SAD and public speaking anxiety, whether the effect is sustained, and whether VR is effective compared with other treatments. The points that Morina et al. have made are probably all a result of this difference in stance, and we hope that the following explanation will aid understanding.

First, they pointed out that Table 1, which lists 22 papers that met our eligibility criteria, counts Anderson et al. (Reference Anderson, Price, Edwards, Obasaju, Schmertz, Zimand and Calamaras2013) and Safir, Wallach, and Bar-Zvi (Reference Safir, Wallach and Bar-Zvi2012) twice each. It is true that each of these studies published follow-up studies on the same population, and we agree that it would have been more appropriate to say 22 publications reporting 20 studies, rather than 22 studies. However, the data from these studies were not included twice in our analysis. For example, in our comparison of the baseline and post-intervention symptoms, only data comparing symptoms at these two time points were included; data from the two follow-up studies were only included in the analysis of the long-term outcomes. Since the data were not duplicated in our analyses, we believe that our procedure is not problematic.

Second, Morina et al. argue that it is wrong to consider two of the included studies as VRET. The first was a study by Lister, Piercey, and Joordens (Reference Lister, Piercey and Joordens2010). Morina et al. claimed that in this study, VR, and not VRET, was used for the assessment. However, in this study, participants were exposed to a virtual audience created using shutter glasses and cathode-ray tube television, and anxiety was induced by reading; so, we believe that VRET was used for therapeutic purposes. The second study was by Yuen et al. (Reference Yuen, Goetter, Stasio, Ash, Mansour, McNally and Watkins2019). Morina et al. once again argued that it was incorrect to determine that the treatment was VRET because less than 50% of the treatments consisted of VRET, so the potential treatment effect could be attributed to factors other than VRET. However, exposure to SAD inevitably occurs outside of sessions as well, and treatment sessions were only held once a week for an hour in most studies. Consequently, it is practically difficult to control for these factors and to ensure that exposures other than those produced using VR do not occur outside of sessions. On the other hand, we were concerned about the fact that Yuen et al. (Reference Yuen, Goetter, Stasio, Ash, Mansour, McNally and Watkins2019) also conducted exposure exercises in the VR group session. However, our eligibility criteria did not exclude in vivo exposure in the VR intervention group, and all other criteria were met; consequently, we decided to include the study in order to obtain a broader picture of the effectiveness of VR interventions.

Next, Morina et al. criticised our eligibility criterion of ‘study participants numbered 10 or more people.’ In clinical trials using new technologies such as VR, it is ethically and scientifically correct to conduct feasibility studies first. As Morina et al. argued, it is true that studies with fewer subjects may not reflect a true effect, and caution regarding this issue is needed. We set the number of participants at ten per study to exclude case studies that may have reported only cases in which an effect had been obtained. It would have been safer to include only studies with ten or more participants per condition, but we believe that an inclusion criterion of ten or more participants per study was sufficient to exclude case series. We also agree with Morina's assertion about the possibility of over-reporting effects in studies with smaller numbers of participants, but their reanalysis, which excluded studies with smaller numbers of participants, seemed to show even stronger effects (Morina et al., Reference Morina, Kampmann, Emmelkamp, Barbui and Hoppen2021).

Finally, Morina et al., stated that we are using Wallach, Safir, and Bar-Zvi's (Reference Wallach, Safir and Bar-Zvi2011) cognitive therapy (CT) as a TAU; however, as explained in Table 1, the CT comparison was different from that used in the in vivo exposure and waiting list groups and was therefore not included in our analysis. For the study by Wallach et al. (Reference Wallach, Safir and Bar-Zvi2011), we compared the VRET group with data from the waiting list. However, the waiting list of Wallach et al. (Reference Wallach, Safir and Bar-Zvi2011) group used waiting list data from Wallach, Safir, and Bar-Zvi's (Reference Wallach, Safir and Bar-Zvi2009) study, and the subjects overlapped. Therefore, we compared the VRET group of Wallach et al. (Reference Wallach, Safir and Bar-Zvi2011) with the waiting list of Wallach et al. (Reference Wallach, Safir and Bar-Zvi2009).

As mentioned above, VR is being used in various ways. We believe that it is significant to include many of these studies in our meta-analysis to examine not only the effect of VRET, the changes in effect over time, and differences in the effect size relative to a comparison arm, but also to examine the differences in effect according to study design (randomised controlled trial or not, with or without structured psychotherapy). Of course, as we mentioned in the limitation section, the studies examined VR in diverse ways, and many studies had problems with the quality of the reported research.

We would like to express our respect for the pioneering work of Morina et al. VR is effective against SAD, as they have acknowledged, and yet it is safe to use. We hope to see increasing numbers of high-quality, large-scale studies in the future.

Financial support

This research received no specific grant from any funding agency, commercial or not-for-profit sectors.

References

Anderson, P. L., Price, M., Edwards, S. M., Obasaju, M. A., Schmertz, S. K., Zimand, E., & Calamaras, M. R. (2013). Virtual reality exposure therapy for social anxiety disorder: A randomized controlled trial. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 81(5), 751.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Horigome, T., Kurokawa, S., Sawada, K., Kudo, S., Shiga, K., Mimura, M., & Kishimoto, T. (2020). Virtual reality exposure therapy for social anxiety disorder: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychological Medicine, 50(15), 24872497.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lister, H. A., Piercey, C. D., & Joordens, C. (2010). The effectiveness of 3-D video virtual reality for the treatment of fear of public speaking. Journal of Cyber Therapy and Rehabilitation, 3(4), 375381.Google Scholar
Morina, N., Kampmann, I., Emmelkamp, P., Barbui, C., & Hoppen, T. H. (2021) Meta-analysis of virtual reality exposure therapy for social anxiety disorder. Psychological Medicine, Published online by Cambridge University Press: 18 May 2021, 13. doi:10.1017/S0033291721001690.Google Scholar
Safir, M. P., Wallach, H. S., & Bar-Zvi, M. (2012). Virtual reality cognitive- behavior therapy for public speaking anxiety: One-year follow-up. Behavior Modification, 36(2), 235246.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wallach, H. S., Safir, M. P., & Bar-Zvi, M. (2009). Virtual reality cognitive behavior therapy for public speaking anxiety: A randomized clinical trial. Behavior Modification, 33(3), 314338.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wallach, H. S., Safir, M. P., & Bar-Zvi, M. (2011). Virtual reality exposure versus cognitive restructuring for treatment of public speaking anxiety: A pilot study. The Israel Journal of Psychiatry and Related Sciences, 48(2), 9197.Google ScholarPubMed
Yuen, E. K., Goetter, E. M., Stasio, M. J., Ash, P., Mansour, B., McNally, E., … Watkins, J. (2019). A pilot of acceptance and commitment therapy for public speaking anxiety delivered with group videoconferencing and virtual reality exposure. Journal of Contextual Behavioral Science, 12, 4754.CrossRefGoogle Scholar